Next Article in Journal
Experimental Consideration on Suppression Effect of Elastic Vibration in Electromagnetic Levitation System for Flexible Thin Steel Plate with Curvature
Previous Article in Journal
Measurement and Analysis of Crowdsourced Vehicle Vibration Levels during Last Mile Delivery Segments for Parcel Shipments
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Vibrotactile Perception Thresholds following Short-Term Exposure to Hand–Arm Vibration: Application for Identifying Potential Workers at Risk of Neurosensory Disorders

National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health, 6-21-1, Nagao, Tama-Ku, Kawasaki 214-8585, Japan
Vibration 2022, 5(4), 803-816; https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration5040047
Submission received: 23 October 2022 / Revised: 12 November 2022 / Accepted: 14 November 2022 / Published: 17 November 2022

Abstract

:
Background: The aim of this study was to propose and validate a novel indicator that characterizes the potential effects of exposure to hand–arm vibration (HAV) and evaluates the increasing risk of neurosensory components of hand–arm vibration syndrome (HAVS). The author focused on a quantity calculated from ascending and descending thresholds and residual shifts in vibrotactile perception thresholds (VPTs) observed at the fingertips in the recovery process after exposure to HAV. Methods: Thirty subjects—10 old exposed (G1), 10 old non-exposed (G2), and 10 young non-exposed subjects (G3)—were required to perform a series of grip tasks with exposure to two intensities of HAV, which was followed by 90 s of vibration perception measurements at the tip of each subject’s right index finger. Vibrotactile perception was measured every 5 min for 30 min. Results: Mean differences between ascending and descending thresholds (VPTWs) for G2 and G3 remained nearly unchanged over time after exposure to HAV. In contrast, the mean VPTWs for G1 gradually increased over time after exposure to HAV. The mean VPTWs for G1 were always larger than those for G2 and G3. TTS recovery was observed at 125 Hz under both of the HAV exposure conditions in each group. TTSs of nearly zero were observed for the low-HAV condition in G3. TTS recovery after exposure to HAV was not observed at 31.5 Hz in any of the subject groups. Regardless of elapsed time, the mean TTSs for G2 and G3 were smaller than those for G1. Negative TTS values showing a lower TTS than the baseline were sometimes observed for the low-HAV condition in G3. Conclusions: VPTWs can be a screening parameter that detects potential patients with only neurosensory components observed as an early sign of HAVS.

1. Introduction

Recently, the lifespan of people has been getting longer, and more and more people have been becoming highly health-literate [1]. If people with high health literacy take actions to improve their health, they can live longer than ever in good health conditions and work beyond the conventional time limit of job retirement. This trend of change in people’s working lives leads to an increase in individuals’ total working time. Similarly, workers using handheld power tools are likely to work longer beyond the job retirement age of 60 years old. Tool operators aged around 60–70 years, working in manufacturing, construction, heavy industries, etc., have usually used power tools for a few decades or more. Hence, the total operating time (TOT) of older workers using power tools has become longer than ever, and adverse health effects due to hand–arm vibration (HAV) exposure are increasingly accumulating in such workers’ hand–arm systems. Many older power-tool operators in various industries are thus at a high risk of hand–arm vibration syndrome (HAVS).
HAVS consists of adverse health effects in the peripheral vascular, peripheral neurological, and musculoskeletal systems in the hand–arm system [2,3]. Many HAVS patients show a complex combination of these symptoms [4,5]. Vibration-induced white finger (VWF)—one of the vascular components of HAVS, also known as Raynaud’s phenomenon—has been considered a typical symptom in HAVS patients [6]. The appearance of VWF represents an increased occurrence of vasospasm in the digital capillaries. The risk assessment in the annex of ISO 5349-1 [7] is based on the relation to the level of vibrational intensity, total exposure time, and the prevalence of VWF. Moreover, VWF has been one of the important criteria for recognition of industrial accident compensation for HAVS. As well as in other countries [8], those who have occupationally developed VWF in Japan have been recognized without exception as having HAVS that is subject to industrial accident compensation. Previously, cases of workers who had not developed VWF but had shown other symptoms related to neurosensory disorders typically observed in HAVS patients were hard to recognize as being subject to industrial accident compensation.
Recently, industrial accident compensation cases without any diagnosis of Raynaud’s phenomenon have been increasingly recognized as HAVS, many of which include cases of workers exposed to HAV at a relatively low vibrational intensity for a long time of 30–40 years. Because of this, the author focused on potential HAVS patients exhibiting only neurological components. These potential patients did not show typical peripheral vascular symptoms of VWF but mainly complained of neurosensory symptoms in the fingers, such as hypoesthesia, numbness, tingling, and pain. Until the middle of 20th century, the neurosensory symptoms were regarded as precursors of the development of HAVS-related vascular disorders. A recent report that performed a systematic review and meta-analysis on hand–arm vibration and the risk of vascular and neurological diseases showed that neurosensory injury occurs with a threefold shorter latency than Raynaud’s phenomenon at equal exposures [9]. This finding suggests that thermotactile and vibrotactile perceptions that are often used as monitoring indicators of neurosensory components can be effective in detecting potential HAV patients.
The author intends to establish a novel indicator related to vibrotactile perception that is a kind of physical quantity reflecting individual neurosensory characteristics at the fingertips and is not affected by environmental conditions or individual health status when vibrotactile perception is measured. A vibrotactile perception threshold (VPT) measured at the fingertips is one of the screening parameters often used to diagnose the development of HAVS [10]. However, VPTs, often obtained by averaging pairs of ascending and descending thresholds, are sensitively affected by environmental conditions and individual health status when the VPT measurement is performed [11]. Therefore, only VPTs much larger than ordinary ones have been used in conventional screening tests for HAVS as an indicator that detects patients with neurosensory damage to their fingers.
The aim of this study was to propose and validate a novel indicator that characterizes the potential effects of exposure to HAV and evaluates the increasing risk of neurological components of HAVS. The author focused on a quantity calculated from ascending and descending thresholds and residual shifts of VPTs observed at the fingertips in the recovery process after exposure to HAV. The findings obtained from this study can be used to establish a novel indicator of the development of neurosensory components appearing prior to the main symptom of HAVS, which can be useful for identifying potential HAVS patients and preventing them from developing HAVS.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

A total of 30 healthy male subjects participated in this study. All of the subjects were nonsmokers and reported no cardiovascular or neurological disorders in their hands. The subjects were classified into three groups: Group 1 (G1) consisted of 10 male subjects aged over 60 years with occupational exposure to HAV, Group 2 (G2) consisted of 10 male subjects aged over 60 years without exposure to HAV, and Group 3 (G3) consisted of 10 male subjects aged 20–30 years without exposure to HAV. None of the subjects in G2 or G3 had any prior history of regular use of handheld power tools in occupational or leisure activities.
The mean age of the subjects in G1 was 65.8 years (ranging from 63 to 71 years), their mean height was 170.0 cm (ranging from 166.0 to 177.8 cm), and their mean weight was 76.0 kg (ranging from 61.0 to 87.2 kg). The subjects in G2 were aged 66–75 years (mean age: 70.8 years), with a mean height of 164.1 cm (ranging from 156 to 172 cm) and a mean weight of 59.8 kg (ranging from 51.0 to 70.0 kg). The subjects in G3 were aged 20–24 years (mean age: 21.8 years), with a mean height of 174.1 cm (ranging from 168.8 to 182.3 cm) and a mean weight of 64.1 kg (ranging from 47.6 to 83.3 kg). Other anthropometric data on the subjects’ hands are summarized in Table 1.
The subjects were requested to refrain from caffeine consumption for 4 h prior to and during the testing. All of the subjects underwent an explanation of the test procedure and gave their written informed consent to participate in this study. The experimental protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Board of the National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health, Japan (application number 2020N-1-17).

2.2. Apparatus

A vibrotactile perception meter (Type AU-06; Rion Inc., Tokyo, Japan) that satisfies the requirements specified in the international standard ISO 13091-1 (2021) [12] was used in this study. The specifications of the vibrotactile perception meter are summarized in Table 2. The diameter of the probe used in this vibrotactile perception meter is 4.0 mm, which yields a contact area at the fingertip of 4.0 ± 2.0 mm in diameter. The contact condition between the probe and the fingertip is classified into method B as specified in ISO 13091-1. The gap between the stimulating probe and its surroundings was 1.5 ± 0.6 mm.
Vibration perception measurements were performed at two test frequencies of 125 Hz and 31.5 Hz. The test frequencies of 125 and 31.5 Hz fall into the response range of FA II and FA I mechanoreceptors, respectively. The selection of the test frequencies was based on results obtained from previous experimental studies showing that daily shifts in fingertip VPTs measured at 125 Hz and 31.5 Hz are likely to be induced remarkably more than those at other frequencies [13,14,15,16]. Moreover these test frequencies have often been used in other studies, making the comparison of data obtained in this study with in the findings of other studies possible.
Vibrotactile perception was measured on the distal phalanx of the index finger of the right hand. Subjects were instructed to position the tip of the index finger of their right hand so that the center of the distal phalanx coincided with the center of the probe. The subjects were required to hold the response button in their left hand and instructed to press the response button as soon as they perceived a vibration sensation and to release the response button as soon as they did not perceive the vibration. Presentation of a test stimulus was based on the von Békésy algorithm, which automatically ascended or descended the magnitude of the test stimulus at 2.5 dB/s.

2.3. Experimental Condition

A vibrational signal was designed in the frequency range of 6.3 to 1250 Hz to form a pseudorandom vibrational signal segment where the power spectrum density (PSD) was constant at 1.0 (m/s2)2/Hz in this frequency range, corresponding to the frequency-weighted acceleration magnitude [7] of 5.9 m/s2 (r.m.s.). This vibrational signal segment was then reconstructed to form a rescaled signal segment, analogous to the former in terms of frequency contents, so that the weighted r.m.s. acceleration magnitude corresponded to 5.3 m/s2. The spectra of pseudorandom vibrational signals used in this study are shown in Figure 1.
The selection of vibrational acceleration magnitudes in this study was based on a previous report that surveyed the vibrational acceleration magnitudes of hand tools supplied in the domestic market. According to this report, several popular hand tools—including pneumatic impact wrenches, sanders, hammer drills, chainsaws, and so on—showed average weighted vibrational acceleration magnitudes falling between 5.0 and 6.0 m/s2.
Eighteen vibrational signal segments, which consisted of the same vibrational magnitudes (5.9 or 5.3 m/s2), were sequentially connected to construct two series of hand–arm vibration (HAV) test signals. The duration of each vibrational signal segment was 30 s, which was followed by a 5-second interval. Therefore, the subjects were intermittently exposed to each series of HAV, the total exposure time of which was 9 min. The signal sequence was designed to simulate the intermittent operation of a handheld tool such as can often be observed on manufacturing lines.
Prior to the experiments on VPT measurements, each subject was exposed to HAV by grasping an experimental handle fastened to a single-axis HAV test rig [17]. The test rig was powered by an electrodynamic shaker (VE-100S; IMV Corporation, Osaka, Japan) aligned to vibrate horizontally, to which the experimental handle was mounted. The handle was vertically oriented, and its geometry was cylindrical, with a size of 40 mm in diameter and of 100 mm in grip length. The handle consisted of the handle base and the measuring cap, between which two piezoelectric single-axis force sensors (9212; Kistler Inc., Winterthur, Switzerland) were sandwiched on the handle’s centerline to measure the grip force. An accelerometer (356A12; PCB Piezotronics Inc., New York, NY, USA) was fixed to the center of the measuring cap to measure the vibrational acceleration at the handle in the fore–aft direction. The resonance frequency of the handle was beyond the range up to 1250 Hz. A force plate was secured on the floor to measure the reaction force acting horizontally between the subjects’ feet and the plate surface. This reaction force was balanced with the push force acting at the handle.

2.4. Experimental Procedure

The experiments were conducted at a room temperature controlled to 23 ± 2 °C. The subjects were required to wear earplugs (NRR31) to exclude the background noise of the laboratory, the main causes of which were the noise related to an air-conditioning unit in the laboratory and the noise related to the signal processing unit connected to a PC to control the vibrations. Prior to the experiment, the background noise level measured around the necks of the subjects using a sound level meter (Type 2250, Brüel & Kjær Inc., Virum, Denmark) was 80 dB(A). Thus, the noise level to which the subjects with the earplugs were exposed was expected to be less than 70 dB(A).
The posture condition applied to subjects in the HAV exposure task was based on the ISO 10819 test protocol [18]. As shown in Figure 2, the subjects were required to stand up straight in front of the shaker and then to grasp the experimental handle connected horizontally to the shaker shaft with their right hand. The subjects were also advised to keep their right forearm oriented horizontally and to maintain their elbow angle within 90 ± 10 degrees and their wrist angle between 0 and 40 degrees. Additionally, the upper arm was not allowed to touch the subject’s body. During 30 seconds of HAV exposure, the subjects were required to control their grip force within 30 ± 5 N and the feed force on the handle within 50 ± 8 N. At five-minute intervals in between HAV exposure segments, the subjects were allowed to release their grip on the handle and to relax their right hand and arm.
Immediately after the vibration exposure, the subjects were asked to sit on a chair with a backrest with their right forearm placed and relaxed on an arm fixture. Then, the subjects were advised to place the index finger of their right hand so that the center of the probe was located between the center of the fingerprint whorl and the distal corners of the fingernail. A set of VPT measurements was performed on the exposed index finger at test frequencies of 125 Hz and 31.5 Hz. The VPT measurements were conducted in descending order of frequency. The acute neurosensory response was continuously measured for the first 90 s immediately after the HAV exposure task, which was followed by 90 s of measurements every 5 min for 30 min. At every interval in a series of VPT measurements, the subjects were allowed to remove their index finger from the probe and to relax their finger.

2.5. Modeling of Vibrotactile Response to Vibrational Stimulus

The term vibrotactile perception threshold (VPT) has been defined as the skin surface acceleration level at which there is a 50% positive response rate for detecting a pure-tone oscillatory stimulus in the psychometric function according to ISO 13091-1 [12]. However, the mechanical contact condition for measuring ascending thresholds is quite different from that for measuring descending thresholds. Descending thresholds are obtained when stimuli of successively descending vibrational intensity are applied to the skin of the fingertip until the stimulus is detected. In contrast, ascending thresholds are obtained when stimuli of successively increasing vibrational intensity are applied to the skin until the stimulus is detected. Therefore, the contact conditions between the fingertip and the tip of a vibrating indenter are quite different from one another. Averaging of data makes sense only when these data are measured under the same conditions. In this respect, conventional arithmetic averaging of three pairs of ascending and descending thresholds does not yield a 50% positive response rate based on psychophysical consideration.
In this study, the author proposes the hysteresis model shown in Figure 3, forming a loop-like relationship between vibrotactile perception and vibrational stimuli. As indicated by the solid lines in Figure 3, the subjective response represented by vibrotactile perception under an increasing vibrational stimulus differs from that under a decreasing vibrational stimulus, which results in different thresholds Ta1 and Td1 for the ascending and descending processes, respectively. Under another different condition, the subjective response changed differently, as shown by the dashed lines in Figure 3, resulting in different ascending and descending thresholds Ta2 and Td2, respectively. Although ascending and descending thresholds are apparently affected by environmental conditions such as the ambient temperature and by acute effects of the physiological and biomechanical conditions of the fingertip, the difference between these thresholds denoted by VPTW may be less affected by the aforementioned conditions. The same analogy can be seen in elastic hysteresis observed in the stress–strain relationships of viscoelastic materials.

2.6. Data Analysis

VPTs were calculated from the arithmetic mean of three pairs of ascending and descending threshold values. In contrast, the arithmetic mean of three differences between ascending and descending threshold values was calculated and defined as the vibrotactile perception threshold width (VPTW):
VPTW f = 1 3 i = 1 3 | T a ,   f ( i ) T d , f ( i ) |
where T a ,   f ( i )   is the i th ascending threshold at a test frequency of f , while T d , f ( i )   is   the   i th descending threshold at a test frequency of f . The first peak–trough pair was excluded from the calculation of VPTs and VPTWs.
A temporary threshold shift (TTS) of a VPT at time t was calculated from the following equation:
T T S f ( t ) = V P T f ( t ) V P T f , 0
where t is the elapsed time after the cessation of exposure to HAV; V P T f , 0 is a VPT value measured with a test frequency of f prior to exposure to HAV, which corresponds to the baseline VPT peculiar to an individual subject; and V P T f ( t ) is a VPT value measured at time t for a test frequency of f.
VPT recovery is modeled as a first-order system in this study [19,20]. The recovery of VPTs was evaluated by an exponential model [21] given in the following equation:
T T S f ( t ) = T T S m · exp [ t / τ ]
where T T S m is the temporal shift of VPT at time 0, and τ is a time constant. Previously, several studies modeled TTS recovery changing linearly as a function of the logarithm of elapsed time [15]. However, this modeling has a drawback in that the extrapolation of TTS recovery in the vicinity of time 0 results in infinity. The exponential model enables prediction of a TTS with a finite value in the vicinity of time 0.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

A univariate general linear model analysis of variance (ANOVA) for the experimental data was carried out to elucidate significant factors. All ANOVAs in this study were performed by using the statistical software SPSS (version 14.0). The results of the analysis were considered significant at a level of p < 0.05. Pursuant to the results of the ANOVA, multiple post hoc comparisons between VPTW data measured at different elapsed times were also carried out using Tukey’s method.
According to a preliminary ANOVA performed for the baseline TTSs, anthropometric data on subjects’ height, weight, and other parameters related to hand size did not significantly affect the baseline TTSs. Thus, these anthropometric terms were excluded from variables in the main ANOVA.

3. Results

Figure 4 shows the relationship between subjects’ age and steady-state VPTs measured at the tip of the right index finger at test frequencies of 125 Hz (Figure 4a) and 31.5 Hz (Figure 4b). In Figure 4a,b, the 15th, 50th, and 85th percentile VPTs of healthy males, predicted as a function of age using data shown in annex A of ISO 13091-2 (2003) [22], are shown by dotted, solid, and dashed lines, respectively. VPTs obtained at 125 Hz and at 31.5 Hz in G3 are distributed near the 50th percentile line. In contrast, VPTs at 125 Hz in G2 are distributed in a range up to the 50th percentile line. VPTs at 31.5 Hz in G1 belonged to the upper half of the percentile region.
The mean baseline VPTs preliminarily measured at the steady state prior to the HAV exposure experiment are summarized for each subject group in Table 3. The main effects of age and exposure experience were observed at 125 Hz (p < 0.05). The mean baseline VPT at 125 Hz in G1 was significantly larger than that at 125 Hz in G2 (p < 0.05) and in G3 (p < 0.05). The main effect of exposure experience was observed at 31.5 Hz (p < 0.01). However no main effect of age was observed at 31.5 Hz (p = 0.13). The mean baseline VPT at 31.5 Hz in G1 was significantly larger than that at 31.5 Hz in G2 (p < 0.01) and in G3 (p < 0.001). No significant difference in VPTs at 31.5 Hz was observed between G2 and G3 (p = 0.20).
Figure 5a,b show the mean TTSs of vibrotactile perception measured every 5 min after exposure to high HAV at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz and those after exposure to low HAV, respectively. TTS recovery was observed under both HAV exposure conditions in each group. TTSs of nearly zero were observed for the low-HAV condition in G3. The results of mixed-design ANOVAs performed for TTSs at 125 Hz are summarized in Table 4. All of the main effects of vibrational intensity, exposure experience, time elapsed, and age were significant (p < 0.05). Significant interaction effects were observed between vibrational intensity and age (p < 0.05) and between vibrational intensity and exposure experience (p < 0.05).
The mean TTSs of vibrotactile perception measured every 5 min after exposure to high HAV at a VPT test frequency of 31.5 Hz are shown in Figure 6a, while those after exposure to low HAV are shown in Figure 6b. TTS recovery after exposure to HAV was not observed in any of the subject groups. Regardless of the elapsed time, the mean TTSs for G2 and G3 were smaller than those for G1. Negative TTS values showing a lower TTS than the baseline were sometimes observed for the low-HAV condition in G3. The results of the mixed-design ANOVAs performed for TTSs at 31.5 Hz are summarized in Table 5. The main effects of vibrational intensity, exposure experience, and age were significant (p < 0.05). The main effect of time elapsed was not significant (p = 0.39). No significant interaction effect was observed at a VPT test frequency of 31.5 Hz.
Table 6 summarizes sets of the time constant, T T S m , and correlation coefficient (r) calculated from regression analysis using Equation (3) on TTS recovery after exposure to high HAV for the three subject groups. A marked TTS recovery at 125 Hz was observed for each group (see Figure 5). As a result, the correlation coefficients at 125 Hz for the three groups were relatively high, ranging from 0.78 to 0.88. In contrast, the TTS recovery at 31.5 Hz was not observed at first glance (see Figure 6). Thus, the correlation coefficients for the three groups were far from 1.0.
Mean VPTWs measured at every 5 minutes after exposure to high HAV at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz are shown in Figure 7a and those after exposure to low HAV in Figure 7b. Mean VPTWs for G2 and G3 nearly unchanged over time after exposure to HAV. In contrast, mean VPTWs for G1 gradually increased over time after exposure to HAV. Mean VPTWs for G1 were always larger than those for G2 and G3.
Mixed-design ANOVAs were performed on the VPTWs using the variables age (young, old), exposure experience (exposed, non-exposed), vibrational intensity (high, low), and the repeated measure of elapsed time (every 5 min from just after exposure up to 30 min). According to the results of ANOVA performed for VPTWs at a test frequency of 125 Hz, as summarized in Table 7, the main effect of exposure experience was significant (p < 0.001). No significant main effect was observed for age (p = 0.768) or time elapsed (p = 0.073). In contrast, the main effect of vibrational intensity was nearly significant (p = 0.059). A significant interaction effect was observed only between vibrational intensity and exposure experience (p < 0.05).
The mean VPTWs measured every 5 min after exposure to high HAV at a VPT test frequency of 31.5 Hz are shown in Figure 8a, while those measured after exposure to low HAV are shown in Figure 8b. Regardless of the HAV exposure intensity, the mean VPTWs for G2 and G3 remained nearly unchanged over time after exposure to HAV. In contrast, the mean VPTWs for G1 fluctuated over time. The mean VPTWs for G1 were always larger than those for G2 and G3. According to the results of ANOVAs performed for VPTWs at a test frequency of 31.5 Hz, the main effect of exposure experience was significant (p < 0.001). No significant main effect was observed for age (p = 0.611), vibrational intensity (p = 0.300), or time elapsed (p = 0.793). No significant interaction effect was observed in any combination of factors.

4. Discussion

The results obtained in this study support the hypothesis that the VPTW is a kind of physical quantity that is independent of individual neurosensory characteristics at the fingertips. The VPTWs measured particularly for non-exposed subjects were almost invariant regardless of the time elapsed shortly after exposure to HAV, suggesting that VPTWs are free from acute effects of exposure to HAV (Figure 6 and Figure 7). Moreover, the VPTWs were not affected by age.
VPTWs can be a screening parameter that detects potential patients with only neurosensory components observed as early signs of HAVS. According to the results obtained in this study, the VPTWs of old subjects occupationally exposed to HAV were consistently larger than those of non-exposed old subjects, suggesting that VPTWs are affected by the long-term effects of exposure to HAV, e.g., occupational exposure by using handheld power tools. Moreover, the features of the VPTWs observed only for the exposed old subjects were such that the VPTWs at 125 Hz gradually increased with the increase in the time elapsed after exposure to HAV. A future study is needed to elucidate the reason(s) why the VPTWs at 125 Hz gradually increased with the increase in elapsed time. In any case, these two features observed for VPTWs in the old subjects who were occupationally exposed to HAV can help us in identifying potential patients with only neurosensory components as early symptoms of HAVS.
The baseline VPT measured at the fingertip is one of the screening parameters widely used to detect workers who have developed HAVS. However, the baseline VPT cannot be used to detect potential patients who only have neurosensory components of HAVS, known as an early symptom of HAVS. According to the results obtained in this study, the mean baseline VPTs of the three subject groups classified by the age and experience of occupational exposure to HAV tend to increase with increasing age and the amount of daily exposure to HAV. As shown in Table 3, the main effects of age and exposure experience were observed for the mean baseline VPTs measured at 125 Hz (p < 0.05). Moreover, the mean baseline VPT at 125 Hz in G1 (the old exposed group) was significantly larger than that at 125 Hz in G2 (the old non-exposed group) (p < 0.05) and in G3 (the young non-exposed group) (p < 0.05). However, the individual baseline VPTs were distributed in a wide range (see Figure 4). This dispersion is affected by individual characteristics—such as neurosensory, physiological, and biomechanical characteristics, among others—which are also known to be influenced by environmental conditions represented by the finger skin temperature.
According to a preliminary ANOVA performed for the baseline VPTs for subjects in the three groups, anthropometric data on subjects’ height, weight, and other parameters related to hand size did not significantly affect the baseline VPTs. A previous study performed for workers occupationally exposed to organic solvents reported that height was positively associated with the baseline VPTs [23]. In contrast, another previous study did not find a significant positive correlation between height and the baseline VPTs in a population exposed to lead [24]. The inconsistency observed in the relationship between subjects’ height and baseline TTSs might be attributed to the difference in the average height of the subjects participating in these studies.
The TTS recovery at 125 Hz appears to follow a first-order model that can be characterized by a maximum amplitude of TTSm, a time constant τ, and a residual TTS at a certain elapsed time. In contrast, the correlation coefficients at 31.5 Hz were 0.51, 0.46, and 0.73 for G1, G2, and G3, respectively, showing low correlation coefficients compared to those at 125 Hz. TTSs and their recovery after exposure to HAV are influenced by the exposure amplitude and the test frequency. The results suggest that a set of parameters given in the prediction of a TTS recovery at 125 Hz after exposure to HAV are more useful than those at 31.5 Hz in evaluating the residual TTS after exposure to HAV. However, the recovery parameters obtained from a time-series change in TTSs varied significantly between the subjects and with their baseline VPTs. Thus, TTS recovery parameters at 125 Hz should be used in combination with an effective screening parameter for VPTWs to detect potential patients with only neurosensory components of HAVS.
A test frequency of 125 Hz is more suitable than 31.5 Hz for the measurement of vibrotactile perception at the fingertip. The frequency of vibration used in an HAV exposure task ranges from 10 Hz to 1.250 Hz, which covers the response range of FA II and FA I mechanoreceptors. Therefore, FA II and FA I mechanoreceptors embedded in the fingertip are inevitably affected by exposure to HAV, resulting in hypoesthesia and, hence, an increasing VPT at the fingertip. However, no TTS recovery was observed for VPTs measured at 31.5 Hz, while a TTS recovery was observed for VPTs at 125 Hz. This is probably related to the difference in the sensitivity of these two types of mechanoreceptors. A previous study reported that the most sensitive frequency in VPT measurements at the fingertip was 125 Hz [25].
The limitation of predicting a TTS recovery using a curve-fitting technique with the first-order model is that the existence of negative values in TTS data causes poor agreement between predicted and measured TTSs, resulting in a rather low correlation coefficient. This is because the prediction model given in Equation (3) in this study is defined only in the positive domain of the response variable. The TTS of a vibrotactile perception observed after exposure to low HAV has been reported to be small and sometimes shows a negative value because of measurement errors and variation in a subject’s response.

5. Conclusions

This study proposes an alternative method using vibrotactile perception thresholds (VPTs) at the fingertip for detecting early signs of neurosensory components in HAVS. Unlike the conventional method that uses the baseline value of VPTs measured at the steady state, free from the effect of exposure to HAV, the proposed parameter of vibrotactile perception threshold width (VPTW) is based on the difference between ascending and descending vibrotactile thresholds measured during the recovery process after exposure to HAV, which was shown to be essentially independent of individual neurosensory characteristics at the fingertip. VPTWs can be used as a screening parameter that detects potential patients with only neurosensory components observed as early signs of HAVS. Future studies are required to examine the effects of repetitive exposure to HAV on VPTWs and residual TTSs at a certain time elapsed after exposure to HAV.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Data Availability Statement

Experimental data are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The author wishes to thank Sachiko Yamaguchi-Sekino for her support in the acquisition of data on vibrotactile perception thresholds.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Report of Advisory Council on Safety and Health of Elderly Workers for the Era of 100-Year Life; Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare: Tokyo, Japan, 2020. (In Japanese)
  2. Bovenzi, M. Medical aspects of the hand-arm vibration syndrome. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 1990, 6, 61–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Pelmear, P.; Wasserman, D. Hand-Arm Vibration, 2nd ed.; OEM Press: Beverly, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  4. Pelmear, P.L.; Taylor, W. Hand-arm vibration syndrome: Clinical evaluation and prevention diagnostic criteria and staging of hand-arm vibration syndrome in the United Kingdom. J. Occup. Med. 1991, 33, 1144–1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. McGEOCH, K.L.; Lawson, I.J.; Burke, F.; Proud, G.; Miles, J. Diagnostic criteria and staging of hand-arm vibration syndrome in the United Kingdom. Ind. Health 2005, 43, 527–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Taylor, W.; Pelmear, P.; Pearson, J. Raynaud’s Phenomenon in Forestry Chain Saw Operators. In Vibration Syndrome; Taylor, W., Ed.; Academic Press: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1974; pp. 121–139. [Google Scholar]
  7. ISO5349-1; Mechanical Vibration—Measurement and Evaluation of Human Exposure to Hand-Transmitted Vibration—Part 1: General Requirements. International Organization for Standardization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2001.
  8. Johanning, E.; Stillo, M.; Landsbergis, P. Powered-hand tools and vibration-related disorders in US-railway maintenance-of-way workers. Ind. Health 2020, 58, 539–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Nilsson, T.; Wahlstrom, J.; Burstrom, L. Hand-arm vibration and the risk of vascular and neurological diseases—A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0180795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Ye, Y.; Griffin, M.J. Assessment of thermotactile and vibrotactile thresholds for detecting sensorineural components of the hand-arm vibration syndrome (HAVS). Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2018, 91, 35–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Takemura, S.; Yoshimasu, K.; Tsuno, K.; Fukumoto, J.; Kuroda, M.; Miyashita, K. Associations between anthropometric factors and peripheral neuropathy defined by vibrotactile perception threshold among industrial vibrating tool operators in Japan. J. Occup. Health 2016, 58, 145–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. ISO13091-1; Mechanical Vibration—Vibrotactile Perception Thresholds for the Assessment of Nerve Dysfunction—Part 1: Methods of Measurement at the Fingertips. International Organization for Standardization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2021.
  13. Bovenzi, M.; Apostoli, P.; Alessandro, G.; Vanoni, O. Changes over a workshift in aesthesiometric and vibrotactile perception thresholds of workers exposed to intermittent hand transmitted vibration from impact wrenches. Occup. Environ. Med. 1997, 54, 577–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Burström, L.; Lundström, R.; Hagberg, M.; Nilsson, T. Vibrotactile perception and effects of short-term exposure to hand-arm vibration. Ann. Occup. Hyg. 2009, 53, 539–547. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Harada, N.; Griffin, M.J. Factors influencing vibration sense thresholds used to assess occupational exposures to hand transmitted vibration. Br. J. Ind. Med. 1991, 48, 185–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Malchaire, J.; Rodriguez Diaz, L.S.; Piette, A.; Goncalves Amaral, F.; De Schaetzen, D. Neurological and functional effects of short-term exposure to hand-arm vibration. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 1998, 71, 270–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Shibata, N.; Hosoya, N.; Maeda, S. Establishment of one-axis vibration test system for measurement of biodynamic response of human hand-arm system. Ind. Health 2008, 46, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. ISO10819; Mechanical Vibration and Shock—Hand-Arm Vibration—Method for the Measurement and Evaluation of the Vibration Transmissibility of Gloves at the Palm of the Hand. International Organization for Standardization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2013.
  19. Nishiyama, K.; Taoda, K.; Yamashita, H.; Watanabe, S. The temporary threshold shift ofvibratory sensation induced by hand-arm vibration composed of fourone-third octave band vibrations. J. Sound Vib. 1997, 200, 631–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Nishiyama, K.; Watanabe, S. Temporary threshold shift of vibratory sensation after clasping a vibrating handle. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 1981, 49, 21–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Malchaire, J.; Piette, A.; Diaz, L.S.R. Temporary threshold shift of the vibration perception threshold following a short duration exposure to vibration. Ann. Occup. Hyg. 1998, 42, 121–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. ISO13091-2; Mechanical Vibration—Vibrotactile Perception Thresholds for the Assessment of Nerve Dysfunction—Part 2: Analysis and Interpretation of Measurements at the Fingertips. International Organization for Standardization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2003.
  23. Skov, T.; Steenland, K.; Deddens, J. Effect of age and height on vibrotactile threshold among 1663 U.S. workers. Am. J. Ind. Med. 1998, 34, 438–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Chuang, H.Y.; Schwartz, J.; Tsai, S.Y.; Lee, M.L.T.; Wang, J.D.; Hu, H. Vibration perception thresholds in workers with long term exposure to lead. Occup. Environ. Med. 2000, 57, 588–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Hong, C.; Xiangchun, Z.; Li, D.; Yongkui, M.; Jianxin, W. Equal vibrotactile sense thresholds of the fingers and its diagnostic significance for hand-arm vibration syndrome. Am. Ind. Hyg. Assoc. J. 1995, 56, 11–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Spectra of pseudorandom vibrational signals used in this study.
Figure 1. Spectra of pseudorandom vibrational signals used in this study.
Vibration 05 00047 g001
Figure 2. Subject’s posture condition in the grip task with exposure to HAV and at the measurement of vibrotactile perception.
Figure 2. Subject’s posture condition in the grip task with exposure to HAV and at the measurement of vibrotactile perception.
Vibration 05 00047 g002
Figure 3. Schematic illustration of the vibrotactile perception model in response to vibrational stimulus, taking hysteresis characteristics into account. Ta1 and Ta2 denote ascending thresholds at conditions 1 and 2, respectively. Td1 and Td2 denote descending thresholds at conditions 1 and 2, respectively.
Figure 3. Schematic illustration of the vibrotactile perception model in response to vibrational stimulus, taking hysteresis characteristics into account. Ta1 and Ta2 denote ascending thresholds at conditions 1 and 2, respectively. Td1 and Td2 denote descending thresholds at conditions 1 and 2, respectively.
Vibration 05 00047 g003
Figure 4. Baseline vibrotactile perception thresholds (VPTs) at frequencies of 125 Hz (a) and 31.5 Hz (b) measured prior to the experiments.
Figure 4. Baseline vibrotactile perception thresholds (VPTs) at frequencies of 125 Hz (a) and 31.5 Hz (b) measured prior to the experiments.
Vibration 05 00047 g004
Figure 5. Temporary threshold shifts (TTSs) of vibrotactile perception at a test frequency of 125 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Figure 5. Temporary threshold shifts (TTSs) of vibrotactile perception at a test frequency of 125 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Vibration 05 00047 g005
Figure 6. Temporary threshold shift (TTS) of vibrotactile perception at a test frequency of 31.5 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Figure 6. Temporary threshold shift (TTS) of vibrotactile perception at a test frequency of 31.5 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Vibration 05 00047 g006
Figure 7. Vibrotactile perception threshold width (VPTW) at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Figure 7. Vibrotactile perception threshold width (VPTW) at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Vibration 05 00047 g007
Figure 8. Vibrotactile perception threshold width (VPTW) at a VPT test frequency of 31.5 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Figure 8. Vibrotactile perception threshold width (VPTW) at a VPT test frequency of 31.5 Hz after exposure to high HAV (a) and low HAV (b). G1 denotes the exposed old group, G2 denotes the non-exposed old group, and G3 denotes the non-exposed young group.
Vibration 05 00047 g008
Table 1. Anthropometric data of the subjects participating in the experiment.
Table 1. Anthropometric data of the subjects participating in the experiment.
Subject GroupAge (years)Height (cm)Body Weight (kg)Hand Length 1 (cm)Palm Width 2 (cm)Palm Circumference 3 (cm)
G165.8170.076.018.89.522.2
(Old exposed)(2.3)(4.1)(10.2)(0.8)(0.3)(0.5)
G270.8164.159.818.39.320.7
(Old non-exposed)(3.3)(6.9)(8.2)(1.0)(0.6)(1.1)
G321.8174.164.119.39.120.6
(Young non-exposed)(1.5)(4.8)(12.1)(0.9)(0.7)(1.3)
Upper row: mean; lower row: SD. 1 Hand Length = the length measured from the tip of the middle finger to the crease at the wrist; 2 Palm Width = the breadth measured at the metacarpals; 3 Palm Circumference = the circumference measured at the metacarpals.
Table 2. Specifications of the vibrotactile perception meter used in this study.
Table 2. Specifications of the vibrotactile perception meter used in this study.
ContentsCondition
StimulationMechanoreceptorSA I, FA I, FAII
FrequencyIntermittent (3.1–160 Hz)
Rate of change2.5 dB/s
MeasurementControlAutomatic
algorithmUp–down method
Von Békésy method
Support Full length of forearm, hand, and finger; seat with back rest
ProbeDiameter4.0 mm
Contact typeMethod B (with surround)
Probe–surroundings gap1.5 ± 0.6 mm
Contact area4.0 ± 2.0 mm
Contact force0.5 ± 0.3 N
Surroundings–fingertip force1.0 ± 0.3 N
Table 3. Mean baseline VPTs in the three subject groups.
Table 3. Mean baseline VPTs in the three subject groups.
Subject GroupMean VPT (SD) (dB)
125 Hz31.5 Hz
G1115.9 (2.9)107.6 (1.9)
(Exposed old)
G2110.5 (7.3)103.3 (4.3)
(Non-exposed old)
G3104.6 (4.7)101.0 (3.2)
(Non-exposed young)
Table 4. ANOVA results of TTSs at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz.
Table 4. ANOVA results of TTSs at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz.
SourceSum of SquaresdfF-Valuep-Value
Main effects
  Vibrational intensity (V)745.820136.839<0.001
  Time elapsed (T)1997.060616.440<0.001
  Age (A)343.766116.980<0.001
  Exposure experience (E)239.193111.815<0.001
Interaction effects
  V × T193.57361.5940.148
  V × A101.81315.029<0.05
  V × E91.73514.531<0.05
  T × A43.92560.3620.903
  T × E127.68161.0510.392
Table 5. ANOVA results of TTSs at a VPT test frequency of 31.5 Hz.
Table 5. ANOVA results of TTSs at a VPT test frequency of 31.5 Hz.
SourceSum of SquaresdfF-Valuep-Value
Main effects
  Vibrational intensity (V)289.670117.843<0.001
  Time elapsed (T)102.07361.0480.394
  Age (A)69.05414.25<0.05
  Exposure experience (E)384.023123.655<0.001
Interaction effects
  V × T91.56160.9400.466
  V × A0.04910.0030.956
  V × E3.64010.2240.636
  T × A8.63760.0890.997
  T × E85.82660.8810.509
Table 6. Parameters of TTS recovery calculated from the vibrotactile perception test performed after exposure to high HAV.
Table 6. Parameters of TTS recovery calculated from the vibrotactile perception test performed after exposure to high HAV.
Subject GroupVPT Test Frequency
125 Hz31.5 Hz
T T S m     ( d B ) τ     ( m i n ) r T T S m     ( d B )   τ     ( m i n ) r
G1 (Exposed old)12.08.200.789.315.90.51
G2 (Non-exposed old)10.222.20.888.922.20.46
G3 (Non-exposed young)11.413.30.798.28.130.73
Table 7. ANOVA results of VPTW at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz.
Table 7. ANOVA results of VPTW at a VPT test frequency of 125 Hz.
SourceSum of SquaresdfF-Valuep-Value
Main effects
  Vibrational intensity (V)35.65313.6000.059
  Time elapsed (T)115.53661.9440.073
  Age (A)0.86010.0870.768
  Exposure experience (E)740.453174.764<0.001
Interaction effects
  V × T2.33360.0391.000
  V × A4.43410.4480.504
  V × E40.85214.125<0.05
  T × A11.27860.1900.980
  T × E88.14561.4830.182
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shibata, N. Vibrotactile Perception Thresholds following Short-Term Exposure to Hand–Arm Vibration: Application for Identifying Potential Workers at Risk of Neurosensory Disorders. Vibration 2022, 5, 803-816. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration5040047

AMA Style

Shibata N. Vibrotactile Perception Thresholds following Short-Term Exposure to Hand–Arm Vibration: Application for Identifying Potential Workers at Risk of Neurosensory Disorders. Vibration. 2022; 5(4):803-816. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration5040047

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shibata, Nobuyuki. 2022. "Vibrotactile Perception Thresholds following Short-Term Exposure to Hand–Arm Vibration: Application for Identifying Potential Workers at Risk of Neurosensory Disorders" Vibration 5, no. 4: 803-816. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration5040047

APA Style

Shibata, N. (2022). Vibrotactile Perception Thresholds following Short-Term Exposure to Hand–Arm Vibration: Application for Identifying Potential Workers at Risk of Neurosensory Disorders. Vibration, 5(4), 803-816. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration5040047

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop