Next Article in Journal
The Social and the Psychological Impact of Endometriosis on the Romanian Urban Population
Previous Article in Journal
The Relationship Between Spiritual Health and Social Trust Among Students
 
 
Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences is published by MDPI from Volume 12 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Valparaiso University (ValpoScholar).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Representations and Narratives of F to M Gender Dysphoric Subjects: Self, Body and Psycho-Traumatology in Comparison with M to F Transsexualism

by
Emanuele Maria Merlo
Department of Adult and Childhood Human Pathology Gaetano Barresi, University of Messina, Italy
J. Mind Med. Sci. 2021, 8(1), 108-119; https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.81.P108119
Submission received: 11 October 2020 / Revised: 14 January 2021 / Accepted: 14 January 2021 / Published: 8 April 2021

Abstract

:
The present study was aimed at extending the method previously used with M to F gender dysphoric patients, to F to M individuals. In particular, the aim of the study was to highlight global and particular representations of the subjects. The study involved 10 gender dysphoric subjects in F to M transition, aged between 18 to 26 years old. Rorschach and the Thematic Apperception Test were administered with particular attention to the detection of psycho-traumatic indexes. The data reached through the extension of the method to F to M subjects revealed a continuity between the two groups. Referring to Self-representation, the subjects produced mainly objectual responses, indicative of a global representation affected by the current physical condition. In terms of Body representation, the subjects provided answers demonstrating the presence of partiality and fragmentation, indicative of a body representation supporting the term dysphoria. Approximately 30% of the subjects’ responses indicated the presence of consistent psycho-traumatology. In comparative terms, F to M subjects provided quantitatively and quantitatively similar answers to M to F subjects, with a greater number of fragmentary responses and a lower presence of psycho-traumatic indexes.

Introduction

From our experience with Gender Dysphoric patients, the first interesting fact refers to the diagnostic question. In this specific case, the request is double and refers to the recognition of the condition and the exclusion of active psychopathology.
The psychopathological issue is fundamental, since the condition is no longer considered a disorder, DSM 5 [1]. The request to exclude the presence of psychopathology makes sense since the patients will then have to undergo endocrinologic and surgical treatments [2]. This refers to the different conditions arising in the clinical field with LGBT subjects, since socio-demographic variables and age represent relevant aspects [3].
Therefore, the need to consider the patients’ associated conditions provides different outcomes. The first refers to the recognition of the condition of Gender Dysphoria, with no presence of psychopathology. The second case refers to the coexistence of Gender Dysphoria and psychopathological factors whose compensation appears as possible.
Primarily, our work requires the avoidance of all disturbing factors that prevent clinicians from being objective. The most common phenomena are known in the light of lack of knowledge, together with minority stress, resilience and general health status [4,5,6].
Phenomenology allows us to grasp the phenomena in the way they manifest themselves [7] and to consider them in the light of effective and objective clinical reasoning related to the subjects’ world projects [8]. With particular reference to psychic objects [9], our study took into consideration intentionality, with logical reference to Brentano [10]. The way in which the subjects are directed towards objects and their nature informs us about personality structures and possible deviations from the adaptation process. This must take place with the application of a method based on the suspension of judgment (epochè), phenomenological reduction and inference as fundamental factors.
From our perspective, the derivation of the used assessment instruments can be considered both phenomenological and dynamic, for the following reasons: it makes the applications of clinical psychology possible, addressing the various fields of application in healthcare and hospital contexts [11], in consideration with the existentially known and unknown possibilities of the subjects [12].
Pareidolia [7,13,14] represents the basic phenomenon through which it is possible to access the subjects’ representations [15] of the things. This is the case of the Rorschach method, which is one of the most used psychodiagnostic methods in the study of personality structures. The study of the inner phenomena allows us to take into account inner structures and to consider horizontal dynamics with respect to the unconscious [16].
The contributions to the study of Rorschach’s pareidolia provided by Nina Rauch de Traubenberg [17,18] makes us get in touch with both corporeality and affectivity. This fact is of fundamental importance, since the functioning of the subjects under examination could be considered to be characterized by a peculiar form of mind- body dualism.
This emerging data can be compared with non- dysphoric conditions, such as binary sexual identity [19]. We therefore consider the images as a form of knowledge [20], that can witness the subjects’ adaptation- maladaptation processes. The fundamental difference between perception and representation is taken into account through the representations by means of which we can access the unconscious dynamics of the subjects.
The second phenomenon taken into consideration by this study is apperception. This transversal phenomenon is understood given the studies of Herbart [21], Lange [22], Stout [23]. It can be defined as a process through which every new experience is assimilated and transformed on the basis of the entire previous experience. The set of experiences that tend to filter subsequent perceptions and representations is called the apperceptive mass.
It is therefore possible to grasp the dynamics of the apperceptive mass, on the basis of the new experiences just lived. The best-known apperception-based psychodiagnostic method is Murray’s Thematic Apperception Test [24]. The theoretical and methodological particularities underlying the methodology adopted will be discussed below.
Our research is linked to the dynamics expressed by previous studies that investigated the corporeality of the subjects. It is necessary to clarify that the dynamics related to corporeality have undergone a theoretical evolution. Fuchs [16], suggested the fundamental concept of body memory, that in our case refers to the fixation of habitual styles, considered in the light of an existential style [25,26]. This style is built on the basis of all fixations and sedimentations, with reference to corporeality, psychological and cognitive experiences as implicit memory [27].
In the case of the Fuchs studies [28,29,30,31], it was possible to identify precise, habitual, traumatic, situational, inter- corporeal and incorporative styles. These styles were considered in previous research on Dysphoric patients of M to F Gender [26], which reported Sabrine C. Koch’s original definition [32].
Our contribution proposed a methodological and theoretical integration in which fundamental, representational and apperceptive moves were extended following the categories of Body Memory, Representation of the Self and the Body.
The transversal factor is therefore corporeality in relation with the dualistic mind-body dynamics, through which it is possible to infer the experience of our subjects. The present research was aimed at extending the following methodology from M to F Gender Dysphoric patients [26,33] to F to M patients, in order to highlight possible common figures and differences between the two groups.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The observational group was composed by 10 subjects in F to M transition, aged between 18 and 26 years old (the average age being 23.3). All subjects had signed the informed consent before the interviews and the test administration. The research was conducted in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration.

Research methods and instruments

Rorschach
Rorschach [17] and the Thematic Apperception Test [24] were administered in order to study object relations and the patients’ representations due to Self [17], Body [18] and Body Memory [16,31,32].
The main theme of object relations in terms of phenomenology and dynamic psychology is meant as the relationship between subject and object [7], considering the necessity to work through epochè, phenomenological inference, phenomenological reduction [34] and to consider the subjects’ intentionality [10].
The studies of Rausch de Traubenberg on Rorschach representations were considered in order to highlight the Self and Body representations of the subjects [17,18].
Self-representation dynamics was highlighted on the basis of Rorschach contents, distinguishing Human, Animal and Objectual representations as indicators of mature/immature organization of the Self. It is fundamental to clarify that several factors can interfere in the constitution of global Self-Representations, so that pathological factors are well known as intervening variables.
The second datum considered refers to Body representations together with affectivity. It is possible to understand this dynamic on the basis of the provided images (responses to Rorschach inkblots). The responses were also considered through the Grid of Affective Dynamics and the Body Image, as stated by Rausch de Traubenberg [18] and reported by Settineri, Frisone and Merlo [33]:
-
Integrate, for images that appear as complete representations, providing clear indications of a good quality answer;
-
Reached/ Attested, for those representations that, although starting from a partial tendency, reach an integrity that must take origin into account;
-
Partial, whose expressiveness remains linked to the lack of entirety with the related diagnostic implications;
-
Fragmentary, understandable not only as not integrated, but also as part of a set whose function or existential sense cannot ignore the integrative contact between the parts, in the specific cases separated as for spaltung.
The last reference is to Gravenhoost’s studies [35] reported issues related to the psycho-traumatic role of inner images. These traumatic aspects are due to the same above- mentioned declared discrepancy, non-conforming experience of corporeal images as psycho-traumatic disturbing figures.
Thematic Apperception Test (T.A.T.)
With reference to the study of the patients’ apperception, in addition to Murray’s T.A.T methodological practice, Bellak’s Systematic study was used [36,37].
Bellak’s Systematic study considered apperception- projective phenomena on the basis of the difference between the original perception (the first contact with the object) and the apperception distortion (apperceptions due to the comparison with previous experienced objects, objectual experiences).
As proposed by previous research, the different types of projections can be [26]:
-
Inverted projection: those processes in which the apperceptive distortion is at the highest levels, tracing paranoid levels.
-
Simple projection: in this mechanism, the inversion and the rigidity of the previous one is missing, it foresees a state of tension derived from experiences of sanction, whose reiteration is independent from the fact by which the situation is known to the others.
-
Sensitization: hyper-receptivity to certain stimuli, due to an increased receptive alertness, occurs when an object (stimulus) affects a predisposing pattern of a given reaction requiring high levels of excitement.
-
Exteriorization: refers to processes of preconscious order, which the subject could dispose of without breaking from deep mental states with rigidity.
-
Cognitive perception: the simple phenomenal detection of graphic traits and manifest meanings.
The projection forms provided by Bellak, are meant as a passage from pure representations to pure perceptions [36].
The reference to the Ego, involved in the apperception process, is meant for the consideration of its role [26,36]:
-
organizes and controls the mode of perception;
-
maintains the projective barriers against disturbing external/ internal stimuli;
-
warns the reality (conative action) and emits warning signs (anxiety);
-
has the ability to self-regulate by promoting tolerance to frustration, adaptation and defenses;
-
has autonomous functions;
-
has the capacity for partial and general self-exclusion.
It is possible to recognize Theme’s levels, Anxieties- Emotions, Defenses, Projection types.
On the basis of the aforementioned methods and phenomena, the Rorschach and T.A.T protocols of the 10 F to M patients were analyzed, as reported by the following results.
All the subjects had signed the informed consent before the interviews and the test administration. The research was conducted in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration.

Results

Results of comparison between TAT F to M and TAT M to F (Current 10 F to M protocols).
In Table 1, the complete analysis related to the subjects’ Rorschach protocols was reported. The analysis involved Rorschach Self and Body representations, respectively represented by human, animal and objectual contents for Self and integrate, reached, partial and fragmentation of the Body.
Moreover, since most of Rorschach responses resulted as psycho-traumatic, in accordance with the previous studies, the psycho-traumatic indexes were provided.
As it follows (Table 2), the data were reported within a summary table, in order to highlight the significance of emerged indexes.
Abbreviations. Self-representation, H: Human; A: Animal; Obj: Object; Body representation, I: Integrate; R: Reached; P: Partial; F: Fragmentation.
In terms of Self representation, a prevalence of objectual contents has emerged. The data will be compared to previous M to F studies. It was possible to notice the relevance of affected Self representation, whose inference would be linked to the declared dichotomy and discrepancy. In the specific case, the affective investment towards human objects resulted low, so that the data explained the strong request of adhering to reassignment programs. In continuity with Self representation, Body dynamics highlighted the presence of fragmentation and partial contents. The continuity is meant as a high level of unexpected contents, due to just treated dynamics. Body contents was represented by partial and fragmented objects, suggesting a state of uncertainty and intolerability of the current body status.
The comparison between the two groups (F to M and M to F) highlighted an important continuity about the results. For both Self and Body representations, the indexes were paired as reported in Table 3.
The Body representation indexes respected the above- mentioned continuity, but rather for the M to F fragmentation index. In this particular case, a prevalence of M to F fragmented representations has emerged. The global tendency is close to continuity, but solely for the F index.
As reported in Table 5, a prevalence in psycho- traumatic indexes emerged within the comparison between F to M and M to F indexes. With particular reference to F to M subjects, the index pointed out the 30% of the traumatic representations. In terms of comparison, there was a prevalence of psycho-traumatic images for M to F subjects (40%). Finally, it was possible to demonstrate a higher presence of psycho-traumatology in M to F subjects.
The analysis carried out on the Thematic Apperception Test indexes was reported in Table 6. In particular, three T.A.T. tables were selected (I, III, XIII), so that the subjects’ responses were analyzed with reference to Bellack Systematic study [36], including themes, emotions, anxieties and projection types. In addition to Bellak’s analysis, the type of body memory was studied to point out the exact equivalence with the relative studies. The following summary tables (Table 7 and Table 8) report the specific indexes and result as emergent from the T.A.T. analysis.
With reference to themes, a prevalence of symbolic and interpretative movements has emerged. This fact highlighted the necessity of our subjects to operate a signification process on body contents, useful to distance themselves from phenomenal perception issues related to the current body form. Several emotions and anxiety issues have emerged with a marked prevalence of boredom, mourning contents, sadness and depressive contents. These issues must be considered related to the body, since the selected tables were included on the basis of their property to evoke corporeal representations. The prevalence of sensitization and exteriorization in terms of projection types, was in line with the significance of changes made in diagnostic terms.
Since Gender Dysphoria (GD) is no longer considered a mental disorder (GID), it is possible to notice how the subjects’ knowledge impacts their consciousness. Sensitization and exteriorization represent dynamics related to consciousness, so that the current knowledge about gender dysphoria influences their psychological paths. As for the M to F subjects, our observation group of F to M subjects, expressed an absolute prevalence of traumatic body memory. Compared to previous Rorscharch indexes, it was indicative of high disturbance about corporeal themes, as testified by the term dysphoria itself.

Discussions

The data presented in the results concerned numerous indexes, which require synthesis and discussion before operating an inferential process to the population. The first reference was to Rorschach, with particular attention to the data concerning the global constitution of the Self [17]. It was interesting to note a continuity between F to M and M to F individuals, since there was a congruence in the percentage of human, animal and object representations. It is therefore possible to infer that the general data persist in gender dysphoric subjects. In particular, we noted a prevalence of object responses ranging from 57 to 58%.
This datum informed us about the constitution of the Self for gender dysphoric patients, which is mediated by representations mainly distinct from the human contents. The maturity of the Self-representation is configured on the basis of a great number of human responses and, in general, it is evidenced by prevalent vital objects [17,18,26,33].
In the case of our gender dysphoric patients, this did not happen, so that it was important to understand the degree of maturity of their global Self representation. Rausch de Traubenberg’s studies clarified this maturation process. The current state of our patients is characterized by a strong desire for sexual reassignment, so that the global representation is under construction, projected towards an integrity that is supposed to be reached through physical reassignment. Specifically, the current general and particular perceptions are inconsistent with internal images, that tend to represent the desire to belong to the opposite sexual identity. From here, we can confirm what has been premised as declared discontinuity, discrepancy and, above all, a unique form of apparent body-mind separation. Following the individuals’ declarations, the sense of unity arising from the need to unify body and mind was experienced as impossible. It would be essential to follow patients prospectively, in order to verify the possible occurring variations of their expressions.
This process would have just begun, since our observations represent the preliminary step for obtaining the authorization to proceed with hormonal therapies and sexual reassignment surgery. At present, it is possible to infer poorly adaptive dynamics related to the Self. The second important fact concerns the global body representation, studied in depth by Rausch de Traubenberg’s [18]. The patients examined by means of the Rorschach method produced indexes representing a similar continuity with the previous M to F study, albeit variant in terms of fragmentary representations. It would be possible to state that an important number of partial and fragmentary representations were present, not foreseen in mature bodily representations. Partial responses were greater among F to M patients and the fragmentary representations were greater among M to F subjects. The results were similar, indicating the presence of fragmentation and partiality that was usually found in Gender Dysphoric subjects depending on a peculiar corporeal discomfort. The psycho-traumatic question referring to gender dysphoric subjects emerged through characteristic features. A high number of representations proved to be maladaptive for the subjects, having a disturbing psycho-traumatic role. This form of representational traumaticity is in line with what has been reported so far. There were two traumatic indexes that seem to be affecting the subjects.
The first referred to the psycho-traumatic indices of Gravenhost [35] and the second to the body experience and sedimentations, due to the above-mentioned studies of Fuchs and Koch [29,30,31,32]. Both indices proved high, indicating a traumatic role of the current images, different from those that would plausibly take place depending on the reassignment process. The traumatic indexes referable to the Rorschach method revealed a consistent presence of psycho-traumatology among F to M subjects, although less than among M to F subjects. In terms of body memory, the transposition operated allowed us to detect an absolute prevalence of traumatic issues. This was evident in the presence of mainly interpretative and symbolic issues, indicative of prospective representation processes for reassignment. The prevalent apperceptive tendencies, in line with the conscious desire for sexual reassignment, were sensitization and externalization. Therefore, the processes of apperception appeared characterized by the full awareness of the condition (GD), by the need for symbolic variation of the current contents and by predominant negative emotions.
Overall, what has emerged revealed the intolerability of the condition and the aspects of bad endurance (dysphoria). It had been stated that, based on the previous studies and current results, gender dysphoria could be considered with particular reference to the Body-Mind Problem. The need to overcome the distinction between body and mind in our experience with gender dysphoria proves to be maintained. Although efforts to express the need for integration between the concepts of mind and body have a long history, in many approaches this conception persists. The latest developments in psychosomatic medicine seem to have sensitized this perspective [38]. The indication is very clear and undeniable. As suggested by Flumini [39] regarding the phenomenology of language, a phenomenological opening that takes into account human ontology is necessary. It is possible to avoid metaphysical drifts and eliminativism through epochè. As it has reported by the author, several contributions have defined the categorical error [40,41] which does not exclude the propensity towards one of the two terms (i.e., Body or Mind) [42,43,44,45]. Several studies have considered the Body-Mind Problem, and it is interesting to compare the emerging results with the approaches in the literature. In a study published in 2015, the authors suggested an interrelation between two specific units, without which the study of mental facts would be ineffective [46]. These subunits are internal-mental existence/ entity and internal-mental reality, fundamental and related phenomena. The two phenomena were considered impossible to be studied independently, to the point that their relationships would have been preliminary to the approach of the Body-Mind Problem. Specifically, the first psychological/physiological subunit (internal-mental existence), consists of human specific mental existence; the second represents the process of introjection of external reality. The authors suggested that the separation of the two subunits would represent a counterproductive fact in terms of study.
The authors stated: “The internal-mental entity is different from the physical body, the internal-mental reality is different from environmental reality, and the mental interaction is also different from physical interaction. In other words, this model would suggest that there is actually no “mind-body problem”. In our case, it was considered interesting to propose a particular functioning mode, which seems to be committed to affirming the existence of a Body Mind Problem. On the surface, the solution to the problem would coincide with the request for sexual reassignment. The results demonstrated the existence of basic phenomena, independent from the possibility of identifying the discrepancy usually declared linguistically by the subjects. It would be interesting if the scientific debate addressed this domain on the basis of possible solutions. It would be possible to say that, in accordance with what Motofei and Rowland proposed [46], the subjective experience of gender dysphoric subjects plays an important role in mediating a picture of the situation with respect to the Body-Mind Problem. Statements that seem to affirm a clear split between mind and body are common with gender dysphoric patients.
What is reported by patients, however, must be examined keeping in mind their lack of knowledge and all the linguistic issues, which in most cases are far from clinicians’ theoretical frameworks. This does not mean that the linguistic contents of the patients is not important. Indeed, this study would suggest that a linguistic analysis is necessary within the diagnostic process. The real discrepancy seems to exist between the typical expressions of the subjects and the basic phenomena that we detect clinically. This can be easily understood depending on the impact that dualistic conceptions have had in cultural and therefore linguistic terms. It all depends on the clinical question. In most cases, as stated in the introduction, the request is very specific. A linguistic analysis of typical expressions should take into account the difference between what a statement is and what the subject’s structure is.
In a second contribution [47], the authors reported previous literature as a useful path for reaching a final statement on issues such as material-immaterial phenomena [48,49,50,51,52,53,54]. What the authors have stated, refers precisely to the inconsistency of the body-mind issue. In fact, the authors suggest that the limited current resources in psychophysiological terms do not coincide with the possibility of speculating on an issue considered improper. These statements have been part of the phenomenological perspective from the very beginning. If we thoroughly analyzed the issue of gender dysphoria and the statements referring to the discrepancy between the mind and body of the patients, we would find that the contents reveal much more. In particular, the request for sexual reassignment is not formulated in line with an evident discrepancy, but rather referring to a specific gender. From the point of view of gender identity, the subjects declare themselves with absolute conviction already men and women regardless of the current primary and secondary sexual characteristics. We, therefore, find expressions that would be inconsistent. On the one hand, the subjects present themselves as afflicted by an intolerable discrepancy, they tell facts in line with this split between an unwanted body and a well- oriented mind in terms of gender [55].
From a second point of view, men or women are declared with absolute certainty, regardless of their sexual characteristics. The above leaves space for the possibility of considering discrepancies and divisions, in an “as if” perspective. In the “results” section, discrepancy and dichotomy have always been expressed as “declared”. The paradox would consist in the subjects’ request to cancel a body-mind dichotomy, which would not exist. It would be interesting to study the genesis of this linguistic structuring. In this case, the subjects declare that they experience an intolerable split between their sexual and gender identities, referring to gender identity as the only relevant domain, they require the modification of sexual characteristics.
No linguistic expression or ideation, even if for a long time considered as abstract and immaterial, would escape the evidence of their corporeal allocation. In these terms, the expressions supporting the perspectives become part of different approaches, both from a historical and conceptual point of view. In one of the previously considered articles [26], the integrative approach reported language as a biologically oriented mental property.
Moreover, as reported by Flumini [39] according to Merelau-Ponty [25,42], language and the specific self and of world consciousness are shown as a further reorganization of the human organism, which therefore exists on three levels, so that the physical, biological and conscious levels can never be separated from one another. In our opinion, it is essential to devote ourselves to the study of the linguistic expressions of subjects with gender dysphoria, since the components just reported are always interconnected. The necessity to resort to projective methods and phenomenology arises from the need to recognize the basic phenomena and to grasp what they represent regardless of the possible misunderstandings due to the expressive desire of the subjects. Even if the subjects present themselves through a declared discrepancy between representations and perceptions, the results do not present Spaltung and phenomenology indicating the presence of inter-psychic split or inter-psychic conflicts as for psychosis. In this work, reference is always made to the declared discrepancies, to clarify that the basic phenomena differ from the expressive will of the subjects. It is therefore necessary to distinguish expressive tendencies and the contents of gender dysphoric subjects starting from the basic phenomena. In our case, Self and Body provided representations were clear indicators of bad endurance of the condition. The data explicitly demonstrated the existence of a condition of suffering, for example suggested by negative affectivity, psycho-traumatology, traumatic somatic sedimentations, in any case, modalities that testified a conscious condition (such as sensitization and externalization). From the emerging data, the question of the awareness of the condition has eventually found an answer; instead, what was not satisfying concerned the expression of a body-mind dichotomy peculiar to gender dysphoria. More correctly, we noticed a construction of language which does not testify the need to better know the body, but the desire to confirm the subjects’ opinion about a private Self deprived of the correspondence between a possible reality (perception) and representation.

Conclusions

Further studies are needed in order to distinguish between peculiar expressive modalities, which would let clinicians think of a particular experience confirming a body-mind functioning and the real basic phenomena that remain independent from the attempts of expression.

Conflict of interest disclosure

There are no known conflicts of interest in the publication of this article. The manuscript was read and approved by all authors.

Compliance with ethical standards

Any aspect of the work covered in this manuscript has been conducted with the ethical approval of all relevant bodies and that such approvals are acknowledged within the manuscript.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. DSM 5. American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Available online: https://www.psychiatry.org/psychiatrists/practice/dsm.
  2. Vitelli, R.; Scandurra, C.; Pacifico, R.; Selvino, M.S.; Picariello, S.; Amodeo, A.L.; et al. Trans identities and medical practice in Italy: Self-positioning towards gender affirmation surgery. Sexologies. 2017, 26, e43–e51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Scandurra, C.; Mezza, F.; Bochicchio, V.; Valerio, P.; Amodeo, A.L. LGBT elders health from the minority stress perspective. Literature review & research recommendations. Psicol Della Salut. 2017, 2, 70–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Scandurra, C.; Amodeo, A.L.; Valerio, P.; Bochicchio, V.; Frost, D.M. Minority stress, resilience, and mental health: A study of Italian transgender people. Journal of Social Issues. 2017, 73, 563–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Scandurra, C.; Picariello, S.; Valerio, P.; Amodeo, A.L. Sexism, homophobia and transphobia in a sample of Italian pre-service teachers: The role of socio- demographic features. Journal of Education for teaching. 2017, 43, 245–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Scandurra, C.; Bacchini, D.; Esposito, C.; Bochicchio, V.; Valerio, P.; Amodeo, A.L. The influence of minority stress, gender, and legalization of civil unions on parenting desire and intention in lesbian women and gay men: Implications for social policy and clinical practice. Journal of GLBT Family Studies. 2019, 15, 76–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Jaspers, K. Allgemeine Psychopathologie; Springer (ed. or. 1913): Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1959; trad. it. Psicopatologia gen-erale, Roma, Il pensiero scientifico, 1964. [Google Scholar]
  8. Vitelli, R. Binswanger, Daseinsanalyse and the issue of the unconscious: An historical reconstruction as a preliminary step for a rethinking of Daseinsanalytic psychotherapy. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology. 2018, 49, 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Husserl, E.; Franzini, E. L’idea della fenomenologia: Cinque lezioni; Bruno Mondadori: Milano, Italy, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brentano, F. La psicologia dal punto di vista empirico, 1973; trad. Giovanni Gurisatti, orig. F. Brentano (1873), Psychologie vom empirischen Standpunkt.
  11. Merlo, E.M. Opinion Article: The role of psychological features in chronic diseases, advancements and perspectives. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology 2019, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Merlo, E.M. Adolescent phobia as a “mask object”. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology 2019, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Fyfe, S.; Williams, C.; Mason, O.J.; Pickup, G.J. Apophenia, theory of mind and schizotypy: Perceiving meaning and intentionality in randomness. Cortex. 2008, 44, 1316–1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lanczik, M. Karl Ludwig Kahlbaum (1828-1899) and the emergence of psychopathological and nosological research in German psychiatry. Hist Psychiatry. 1992, 3, 53–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Freud, S. L’interpretazione delle afasie, Milano, Sugarco (ed. or. 1891). 1989. [Google Scholar]
  16. Fuchs, T. Body Memory and the Unconscious, in Founding Psychoanalysis Phenomenologically; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 69–82. [Google Scholar]
  17. Rausch de Traubenberg, N.; Sanglade, A. Self- representation and object relationships on the Rorschach: Scaling of self-representation: A comparative analysis of the results of mentally disturbed adolescents and somatic patients. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée. 1984, 34, 41–57. [Google Scholar]
  18. Rausch de Traubenberg, N.; Bloch-Laine, F.; Boizou, M.F.; Duplant, N. Modalités d’analyse de la dynamique affective au Rorschach. Grille d’analyse de la dynamique affective. Revue de psychologie appliquée 1990. [Google Scholar]
  19. Scandurra, C.; Amodeo ALBochicchio, V.; Valerio, P.; Frost, D.M. Psychometric characteristics of the Transgender Identity Survey in an Italian sample: A measure to assess positive and negative feelings towards transgender identity. International Journal of Transgenderism. 2017, 18, 53–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sartre, J.P. L’immaginario. Psicologia fenomenologica dell’immaginazione (ed. or. 1940); Einaudi: Torino, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  21. Herbart, J.F. Lehrbuch zur Psychologie (1816). Psychologie als Wissenschaft 1825. [Google Scholar]
  22. Lange, K. Über Apperzeption. Eine psychologisch-pädagogische Monographie, Plauen, F.E. Neupert, 1895. 1899. [Google Scholar]
  23. Stout, G.F. Analytic Psychology; Swan Sonnenschein: London, UK, 1896; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
  24. Murray, H.A. Thematic Apperception Test; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1943. [Google Scholar]
  25. Merleau-Ponty, M. Phenomenology of Perception; Routledge & Kegan Paul: London, UK, 1962; ISBN 0–415–27841–4. [Google Scholar]
  26. Settineri, S.; Merlo, E.M. The Phenomenology of the Style of Existence in Gender Dysphoric Patients. A Bridge Between Apperception and Body Memory. Reti, Saperi, Linguaggi. 2019, 6, 171–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Schacter, D.L. Implicit Memory: History and Current Status. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition. 1987, 13, 501–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Fuchs, T. Das Gedächtnis des Leibes. Phänomenologische Forschungen. 2000, 5, 71–89. [Google Scholar]
  29. Fuchs, T. Leib und Lebenswelt. Neue philosophisch-psychiatrische Essays; Die Graue Edition: Kusterdingen, Germany, 2008; ISBN-10: 3906336514. [Google Scholar]
  30. Fuchs, T. Leibgedächtnis und Lebensgeschichte. In Der Text im Körper; Friedrich, F.A., Fuchs, T., Koll, J., Krondorfer, B., Martin, G.M., Eds.; Leibgedächtnis, Inkarnation und Bibliodrama; EB-Verlag: Hamburg, Germany, 2008; pp. 10–40. [Google Scholar]
  31. Fuchs, T. Body Memory and the Unconscious. In Founding Psychoanalysis Phenomenologically; Lohmar, D., Brudzinska, J., Eds.; Phaenomenologica (Published Under the Auspices of the Husserl-Archives); Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2012; Volume 199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Koch, S.C. Testing Fuchs’ Taxonomy of Body Memory: A Content Analysis of Interview Data. In Body Memory, Metaphor and Movement; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 171–186. [Google Scholar]
  33. Settineri, S.; Frisone, F.; Merlo, E.M. Psychotraumatology of images in gender dysphoria. The Open Psychology Journal. 2018, 11, 222–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Husserl, E. Logica, psicologia e fenomenologia: Gli Oggetti intenzionali e altri scritti. Il melangolo. 1999. [Google Scholar]
  35. Gravenhorst, M.C. Rorschach psychodiagnosis of psychic trauma in sexually abused children. Rorschachiana. 2002, 25, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Bellak, L.; Pasqoarelly, B.A.; Braverman, S. The use of the thematic apperception test in psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1949, 110, 51–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Imbasciati, A.; Ghilardi, A. Manuale clinico del TAT: La diagnosi psicoanalitica; Giunti: Firenze, Italy, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  38. Settineri, S.; Frisone, F.; Alibrandi, A.; Merlo, E.M. Emotional Suppression and Oneiric Expression in Psychosomatic Disorders: Early Manifestations in Emerging Adulthood and Young Patients. Front Psychol. 2019, 10, 1897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Flumini, A. La fenomenologia della parola di Maurice Merleau-Ponty: Per un ritorno al” soggetto parlante”. Rivista italiana di filosofia del linguaggio. 2009, 100–120. [Google Scholar]
  40. Damasio, A.R.; Macaluso, F. L’errore di Cartesio: Emozione, ragione e cervello umano; Adelphi: Garden City, NY, USA, 1995; ISBN 10: 8845911810. [Google Scholar]
  41. Ryle, G. Lo spirito come comportamento-Einaudi. 1955. [Google Scholar]
  42. Merleau-Ponty, M. La struttura del comportamento, tr. it. di GD Neri.; Bompiani: Milano, Italy, 1963. [Google Scholar]
  43. Edmund, H. Philosophie als strenge Wissenschaft. In La filosofia come scienza rigorosa; Logos, trad. it.; Laterza: Bari, Italy, 2005; pp. 3–4. [Google Scholar]
  44. Searle, J.R. Dell’intenzionalità, trad. it. di D. Barbieri, Bompiani, Milano. 1985. [Google Scholar]
  45. Searle, J.R. La riscoperta della mente, trad. it. di S. Ravaioli, Torino, Bollati Boringhieri. 1994. [Google Scholar]
  46. Motofei, I.G.; Rowland, D.L. Solving the mind-body problem through two distinct concepts: Internal-mental existence and internal mental reality. J Mind Med Sci. 2015, 2, 128–141. [Google Scholar]
  47. Motofei, I.G.; Rowland, D.L. The mind-body problem; three equations and one solution represented by immaterial-material data. J Mind Med Sci. 2018, 5, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Motofei, I.G.; Rowland, D.L. Structural dichotomy of the mind; the role of sexual neuromodulators. J Mind Med Sci. 2016, 3, 131–140. [Google Scholar]
  49. Motofei, I.G.; Rowland, D.L. The mind body problem, part three: Ascension of sexual function to cerebral level. J Mind Med Sci. 2016, 3, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  50. Rowland, D.L.; Motofei, I.G. The mind and sexuality: Introduction to a Psychophysiological Perspective. J Mind Med Sci. 2015, 2, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Motofei, I.G. A dual physiological character for sexual function: Libido and sexual pheromones. BJU Int. 2009, 104, 1702–1708. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rowland, D.L.; Motofei, I.G. Psycho-physiologic emergentism; four minds in a body. J Mind Med Sci. 2017, 4, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Motofei, I.G. A dual physiological character for cerebral mechanisms of sexuality and cognition: Common somatic peripheral afferents. BJU Int. 2011, 108, 1634–1639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Motofei, I.G. The etiology of premature ejaculation starting from a bihormonal model of normal sexual stimulation. Int J Impot Res. 2001, 13, 49–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Merlo, E.M.; Frisone, F.; Settineri, S. A Reflection on the Subjective Transgender Experience in Healthcare Context: Emotion and Identity as Figures of Life Course. PuntOorg International Journal. 2019, 4, 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Rorschach indexes for Self, Body and Psycho-traumatology of F to M subjects.
Table 1. Rorschach indexes for Self, Body and Psycho-traumatology of F to M subjects.
Jmms 08 00014 i001
Table 2. Indexes summary.
Table 2. Indexes summary.
Jmms 08 00014 i002
Table 3. Rorschach Self indexes comparison F to M and M to F patients.
Table 3. Rorschach Self indexes comparison F to M and M to F patients.
Jmms 08 00014 i003
Table 4. Rorschach Body indexes comparison F to M and M to F patients.
Table 4. Rorschach Body indexes comparison F to M and M to F patients.
Jmms 08 00014 i004
Table 5. Psycho-traumatology summary.
Table 5. Psycho-traumatology summary.
Jmms 08 00014 i005
Table 6. T.A.T. narratives; Bellak systematic indexes and Body memory.
Table 6. T.A.T. narratives; Bellak systematic indexes and Body memory.
Jmms 08 00014 i006
Jmms 08 00014 i007
Jmms 08 00014 i008
Jmms 08 00014 i009
Jmms 08 00014 i010
Table 7. General Thematic Apperception Test indexes and results.
Table 7. General Thematic Apperception Test indexes and results.
Jmms 08 00014 i011
Table 8. Tat indexes comparison for F to M and M to F patients.
Table 8. Tat indexes comparison for F to M and M to F patients.
Jmms 08 00014 i012

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Merlo, E.M. Representations and Narratives of F to M Gender Dysphoric Subjects: Self, Body and Psycho-Traumatology in Comparison with M to F Transsexualism. J. Mind Med. Sci. 2021, 8, 108-119. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.81.P108119

AMA Style

Merlo EM. Representations and Narratives of F to M Gender Dysphoric Subjects: Self, Body and Psycho-Traumatology in Comparison with M to F Transsexualism. Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences. 2021; 8(1):108-119. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.81.P108119

Chicago/Turabian Style

Merlo, Emanuele Maria. 2021. "Representations and Narratives of F to M Gender Dysphoric Subjects: Self, Body and Psycho-Traumatology in Comparison with M to F Transsexualism" Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences 8, no. 1: 108-119. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.81.P108119

APA Style

Merlo, E. M. (2021). Representations and Narratives of F to M Gender Dysphoric Subjects: Self, Body and Psycho-Traumatology in Comparison with M to F Transsexualism. Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences, 8(1), 108-119. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.81.P108119

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop