Next Article in Journal
Measuring Industrial Health Using a Diminished Quality of Life Instrument
Next Article in Special Issue
Comparisons of Predictive Power for Traffic Accident Involvement; Celeration Behaviour versus Age, Sex, Ethnic Origin, and Experience
Previous Article in Journal
Consequences of Increases in Wild Boar-Vehicle Accidents 2003–2016 in Sweden on Personal Injuries and Costs
Previous Article in Special Issue
Correction: Bishop, H. et al. Driving among Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Safety 2018, 4, 40
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Perceived Safety Benefits of Aftermarket Driver Support Systems: Results from a Large Scale European Field Operational Test (FOT)

Loughborough Design School, Loughborough University, Loughborough LE11 3TU, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Safety 2018, 4(4), 54; https://doi.org/10.3390/safety4040054
Submission received: 4 September 2018 / Revised: 5 November 2018 / Accepted: 14 November 2018 / Published: 20 November 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Traffic Safety and Driver Behaviour)

Abstract

:
A field operational test (FOT) is a technique used within traffic safety to evaluate the overall value of in-vehicle information systems (IVISs) under normal operating conditions. In this study, a pan-European FOT was used to evaluate Navigation, Speed Information/Alert, Traffic Information, and Green Driving Support functions together with participants’ perceptions of safety’ before, during, and after using the functions. Through utilization and adherence to the FOT methodology, data were collected over a period ranging from 8 to 16 months in five European countries in order to assess the driver pre-conceived ideas and subsequent subjective and objective experiences with the IVIS functions. Several analyses of data were conducted, and this paper describes the results relating to the ‘user-experience’ as evaluated through subjective responses. The study showed that before the FOTs started, overall participants expected a higher safety benefit through using Speed Alert compared to the other functions. This function was also perceived to offer the highest safety benefit after the FOT had been completed. Perceptions of safety were found to be lowest for the green-driving function. The results offer insights into public expectations of IVIS functions and how these change with experience and overall; they suggest that, in some cases, the perception to safety benefits could be somewhat misplaced.

1. Introduction

Modern vehicles have many systems that offer the driver information that facilitate the driving task or provide details of the rapidly changing road environment in the interests of enhanced mobility. Many such systems are now available on an after-market or ‘nomadic’ device that can be transported into and out of the vehicle when required. Increasingly, however, the functions are also becoming available on a smartphone and the market penetration of these has been increasing rapidly (Statista) [1]. Such proliferation of technology has, however, led to many questions relating to safety and efficiency. One such issue is the potential of information systems to cause distractions while driving. The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA (https://www.nhtsa.gov/risky-driving/distracted-driving)) observes that “distracted driving is any activity that diverts attention from driving, including interacting with the navigation system”. When considering driver control of the vehicle, Young and Regan [2] reviewed several studies showing that drivers consciously or unconsciously attempt to reduce their workload and thereby moderate their exposure to crash risk through distraction while interacting with in-vehicle devices. According to Young and Regan, drivers try to achieve this through a number of means including speed reduction (Alm and Nilsson; Burns, Parkes, Burton, Smith and Burch; Haigney et al.; Rakauskas, Gugerty and Ward) [3,4,5,6], increasing car-following distance (Jamson, Westerman; Strayer and Drews; Strayer, Drews and Johnston) [7,8,9], moderating the level of attention given to the driving and non-driving tasks as the road environment changes (Brookhuis, de Vries, and de Waard; Chiang Brooks and Weir) [10,11], and ultimately accepting reduced overall driving performance (e.g., by checking mirrors and instruments less frequently—Brookhuis et al.; Harbluk, Noy, and Eizenmann) [10,12]. However, there appears to be a lack of research relating to individual drivers’ own perceptions of safety whilst interacting with in-vehicle information systems (IVISs). Of particular relevance is whether use of such systems increases the risk of driver distraction and therefore reduces drivers’ overall perceptions of safety. The study described in this paper aims to address this lack of research.
Field operational tests (FOTs) are being increasingly used in studies of road transportation and safety research. A definition of an FOT was made in the Field Operational Test Support Action (FESTA) handbook in 2008, now in its fifth revision (FESTA, 2016) [13], which defines FOTs as “studies undertaken to evaluate a function or functions under normal operating conditions in environments typically encountered by the host vehicle(s) using quasi-experimental methods”. Essentially, therefore, an FOT involves a large-scale program that aims to assess the efficiency, quality, robustness, and acceptance of information and communication technology (ICT) solutions for smarter, safer, cleaner, and more comfortable transport and travel. Such ICT technologies include, for example, navigation and traffic information systems, advanced driver support systems, and co-operative vehicle-highway systems.
The TeleFOT project (Field operational tests of aftermarket nomadic devices in vehicles) involved a number of large-scale pan-European FOTs (LFOT) with the collective aim to assess the impacts of driver support functions provided by nomadic devices on the driving task as well as on the transportation process as a whole (testing and evaluation strategy reference). These functions were Navigation, Traffic Information, Speed Information/Alert, and Green Driving Support.
Whilst much of the safety analysis (Welsh, Morris et al.) [14] within the TeleFOT project uses data based upon ‘facts’ logged in either travel diaries or collected using simple data acquisition systems placed in the vehicle, participants were also asked to judge the impact they ‘perceived’ the support function to have had upon their safety. This was established before, during, and at the end of the FOT. This paper focuses upon these perceptions and how they changed by comparing the pre-study expectations to the post-study evaluation. The paper also considers which of the functions were perceived to have the most potential in respect of increasing safety.

2. Method

Participants were recruited into the TeleFOT project based upon sampling criteria that related to age (25–55 years), number of years driving experience (>3 years), and number of kilometers driven per year (>10,000 km). In addition, it was intended that participants should have either no or very little/only occasional prior experience with the functions to be evaluated within the FOT. It is, however, acknowledged that some of the participants may have had more experience than they indicated, but it is not possible to verify this. Participants were invited to local briefing sessions where the systems and functions were demonstrated and the assistance they could expect to be provided by the function was explained. Participants had the opportunity to ask any questions concerning the systems/functions prior to undertaking the FOT and at any point during their participation.
During their involvement in the TeleFOT field operational tests, participants were asked on four occasions to indicate how their perception of safety was affected by using the specific support function under test (Navigation, Traffic Information, Speed Information/Alert, or Green Driving Support). At the outset, prior to using the function, they were asked ‘Do you think that your safety while driving will change with your access to the support system?’ This was then assessed at two further points during the FOT and then again finally once the FOT was completed. The post-study questionnaire asked ‘Do you find that your safety whilst driving has changed due to your access to the support system?’ Responses were given on a five-point Likert scale as follows: 1 = Radical Decrease (in safety), 2 = Slight Decrease, 3 = No Change, 4 = Slight Increase, 5 = Radical Increase.
Of the FOTs that took place during TeleFOT, those included in the analysis presented in this paper are listed in Table 1 together with the FOT sample size, the duration of the FOT, the functions tested with the system used, and the analysis sample size. Only participants who completed both the pre- and post-FOT questionnaires are included in the analysis (which follows a repeated measures design).
The diver support offered by the functions was predefined via use cases that specified the functionality required for the FOTs. Navigation preference could be set by the participant and utilized both verbal and visual instructions with the options for fastest or shortest routing. Traffic Information was received while driving, via connection of the system being used with real-time updated databases with a warning displayed on the device each time a critical situation was detected along the pre-set (Navigation) path to be followed. For Speed Information/Alert, the actual speed limit at any point was displayed on the device screen, either continuously or each time the speed limit changed to alert the driver of the change. While driving, the system continuously checked the actual vehicle speed (either via the CAN-bus (Controller Area Network) or the GPS (Global Positioning System) speed) in comparison with the actual speed limit at every point. When the vehicle speed exceeded the speed limit by a predetermined percentage or amount, an alert was given to the driver by a visual/acoustical warning. The device screen displayed the actual speed as well as the speed limit. The Green Driving function collected information on fuel use and CO2 emissions. The system then informed the driver about, for example, excessive fuel use and driving habits (e.g., excessive acceleration) to give an overview of how ‘greenly’ the participant was driving and how they could improve.
The results focus first upon how the perception of safety at the end of the trial met the pre-trial expectation for each of the different functions. The Likert scale data is classified as ordinal data and each participant gave a response pre-FOT and post-FOT, thus resulting in paired data sets over a longitudinal study. Wilcoxon’s Signed Rank test is appropriate to establish significant differences in responses for paired ordinal data using the following null and alternative hypotheses:
H0 There is no difference between the pre and post-test perception of safety.
HA There is a difference between the pre and post-test perception of safety.
The second phase of the analysis considers, of all the functions and irrespective of FOT, which was perceived to have the most safety benefit prior to the study and then which was perceived to have been the most beneficial for safety at the end of the study. For this analysis, the responses were grouped into three categories (Decrease in Safety, No Change, and Increase in Safety) by combining the radical and slight categories together. To test for significant differences across the different functions, the Kruskal-Wallis test was used to test the following null and alternative hypotheses (both pre- and post-FOT):
H0 There is no difference in the perception of safety offered by the different functions.
HA There is a difference in the perception of safety offered by the different functions.
All statistical tests were carried out at the 95% significance level, with α = 0.05. Thus, significant results are obtained when sig(p) < 0.05. Significant results infer that that any observed differences in the TeleFOT sample data are highly likely (more than 95% chance) to be also observed in the population as a whole, whereas non-significant results are less likely to be representative.

3. Results

3.1. Navigation

Navigation was assessed in five FOTs undertaken in Greece, Italy, Spain, Sweden, and the UK. Considering all participants across the five countries, 41% expected the safety of their driving to improve when they used navigation. The pre- and post-questionnaire responses per country are shown in Figure 1 and the results of the Wilcoxon signed rank tests are presented in Table 2.
For all countries, the modal value before the FOT commenced was 3 (‘No Change’) expected in safety while driving with the use of navigation. Drivers in Greece had the highest expectation (rating 4 and 5 combined) of safety improvements prior to the study (50%) followed by those in the UK (48%). For the other countries, safety benefits were generally anticipated by around a third of participants. Overall, in comparing pre-test with post-test responses, no changes in the distribution of responses were observed for Sweden, Italy, or Spain. However, in the UK and in Greece there was an overall reduction in the rating, with a shift towards a more neutral and slight perceived negative effect upon safety compared to original expectations.

3.2. Speed Information/Alert

Speed Information/Alert featured in FOTs in the UK, Italy, and Spain. Combining all participants across the three FOTs, 62% anticipated an improvement in safety (response 4 or 5) with the use of Speed Information/Alert. The pre- and post-questionnaire responses per country are shown in Figure 2 and the results of the Wilcoxon signed rank tests are presented in Table 3.
The modal response was ‘Slight Increase’ in each country in the pre-FOT assessment, with 68% of Italian, 56% of Spanish, and 55% of UK participants expecting at least a slight increase in safety with the use of Speed Information/Alert. Upon completion of the FOT, there were significant differences in the perceived safety in the UK and Italy with a move towards ‘No Change’. There was no significant difference in the pre- and post-FOT responses overall for Spain. Very few participants either anticipated or reported through experience after the FOT that Speed Information/Alert had a negative impact on safety whilst driving.

3.3. Traffic Information

FOTs from Sweden and Greece contributed to the analysis relating to Traffic Information. In Sweden, 58% of participants expected Traffic Information to give a safety benefit and in Greece the figure was 45% (50% when countries are combined). The distribution of pre- and post-FOT questionnaire responses per country are shown in Figure 3 and the results of the test of differences comparing pre- and post FOT are shown in Table 4.
In Sweden, the modal pre-FOT response anticipated a slight increase in safety whilst in Greece the modal pre-FOT response anticipated ‘No Change’. There was a significant difference in the post-FOT response for Sweden with a distinct move towards perceiving no change in safety. There was a similar pattern in the data from Greece; however, in this case the result does not indicate a significant difference (although the result is marginal and would be significant for a directional test).

3.4. Green Driving

Green Driving Support was assessed as a function in the Swedish FOT. Just over 20% of the drivers anticipated an increase in safety whilst driving associated with the Green Driving Support. The vast majority anticipated no change (75%) and this increased post-FOT (88%), as shown in Figure 4; however, the difference between pre- and post-FOT responses is not significant for the two-tailed hypothesis stated (see Table 5). It would, however, be significant for a one-tailed test, had a direction been assigned to the alternative hypothesis.

3.5. Difference in Perceptions across Functions

Kruskal-Wallis Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) show that there is a significant difference in the rating of both the perceived expected safety impact (pre-FOT) with use across the different functions (p < 0.001) and also in the rating of the perceived safety impact (post-FOT, p < 0.001). Figure 5 and Figure 6 show the percentage of ratings according to ‘Decrease’, ‘No Change’, and ‘Increase’ according to function for the pre-FOT and post-FOT evaluation, respectively.
Pre-FOT, Speed Information/Alert had the highest proportion of participants anticipating a benefit upon safety whilst Navigation had the lowest proportion. At the end of the FOTs, a higher proportion of participants still perceived Speed Information/Alert functions to have a greater safety benefit than the other functions; however, the proportion had fallen by 10% from the pre-FOT evaluation. The greatest fall in rating was for Green Driving, where participants rated the perceived post-FOT benefit the lowest, with a 35% fall in the proportion feeling they drove safer whilst using the function.

4. Discussion

Overall, driving can be considered as one example of human–machine interaction (HMI) in which the human (in this case, the driver) interacts with a machine (in this case, the vehicle; Harvey et al.) [15]. As well as interacting with the vehicle control functions, such as steering, accelerating, braking, and gears, the driver also performs secondary tasks within the vehicle, and more and more frequently this will involve interacting with an in-vehicle information system (IVIS). These functions are generally aimed at enhancing the driving experience. To successfully design and evaluate the performance of these systems, a thorough understanding of user acceptance and user experience is required.
Such feedback allows for the design of interfaces that are acceptable to the driver and positively enhance the overall experience (Hassenzahl and Tractinsky) [16].
In this study, user experience was specifically defined in terms of ‘perceptions of safety’ and the FOT methodology was used to evaluate whether such perceptions changed as a result of increasing exposure through user experience in comparison to pre-conceived ideas about the IVISs, which may have been formulated without any prior user-experience.
Some interesting findings are evident. The perceptions of safety relating to Navigation were evaluated in Greece, Italy, Spain, Sweden, and the UK. Overall, 41% expected that the Navigation function would be of benefit in terms of safety prior to the commencement of the study. However, exposure to the Navigation function did nothing to improve the perception of safety in any of the participating countries, with the exception of Spain, where a marginal increase in perceived safety benefit was found. In the other participating countries, the participants most commonly reported that navigation would not alter their general perception of safety both before and after the FOT. In some cases, the perception of safety shifted towards being negative, although the reasons for this are unclear.
The perception of safety of the Speed Information/Alert feature was tested in the UK, Italy, and Spain. Across these three test sites, 62% anticipated an improvement in safety. However, in both Italy and the UK, the perceptions shifted towards a more neutral (no change) viewpoint, although in Spain, perceptions of safety increased slightly post-test. Again, the explanation is not immediately obvious but may indicate that pre-conceived ideas relating to this function could have been misplaced and that it failed to live up to expectations.
The Traffic Information system was only evaluated in Sweden and Greece and prior to commencement of the FOT most respondents showed a neutral or positive perception of safety. However, experience with this function showed that perceptions of safety did not improve in both countries and became more neutral. Some participants reported that this function led to a slightly negative perception of safety after exposure to the function, but this was not statistically significant. The Green Driving Support function was only assessed in Sweden. Just over 20% of the drivers anticipated an increase in safety prior to the FOT, with the majority anticipating no change. After experience with the function, more respondents reported that it would not change their perception of safety. However, the results for this function are also not statistically significant.
Overall, the results suggest that the perceptions of safety for the tested functions did not increase with exposure to them. Again, this may reflect ambiguous or unrealistic expectations of what such functions can deliver in terms of safety. On the other hand, participants may feel that the functions do not offer safety benefits because they are forced to look at them in order to gain necessary information, which in turn causes them to divert their visual attention from the road, thereby increasing crash risk. Objective analyses of data relating to distraction through Navigation use as part of the same study (Morris et al.) [17] suggest that whilst participants do indeed change their visual behavior in response to the introduction of the function (so that they spend relatively more time looking at the function), individual glances are not detrimental to safety, since they are overwhelmingly short (mostly <1 s at a time.) Therefore, whilst participants may anticipate that an IVIS causes distraction, this is not widely supported in the literature.
Alternatively, the functions tested in the study may simply have not met with expectations and preconceived notions as to what they can deliver. For example, Smith [18] identified different groups in terms of technology usage and the groups differed in terms of their expectations. Although the study was designed to ensure recruitment of a random sample of participants at each test site, the sampling did not discriminate in terms of views and experiences regarding technologies.
The study also represents a reasonably robust and positive experience with the FOT methodology. Overall, the methodology works well and gives convincing results given the longitudinal nature of the tests and the amount of detail that is collected during each test (both subjective and objective.) With that said, some issues are immediately obvious (e.g., participant drop-outs, reliability of data-logging, participant vigilance in completing questionnaires), but these issues did not present a special barrier to the results and we are confident that the study is reasonably repeatable and representative. It may, however, be the case that participants had more prior experience with the functions, particularly Navigation, than they were prepared to state upon recruitment, and this may have had an effect on the results for Navigation. As the vehicle industry progresses towards automation, it is likely that IVISs will become more and more prominent as the driver’s role switches from (essentially) ‘controller’ to ‘monitor’. The user experiences with such systems have to be fully evaluated before there can be general public confidence in, and acceptance of, the safety of autonomous vehicles. Studies such as this are relevant as they provide the evidence-base in this regard.

5. Conclusions

  • Driver information systems are becoming more prevalent in vehicles, although their full effect on driver performance and safety is still unclear.
  • The study described used FOT methodology to evaluate drivers’ perceptions of safety when using driver information systems (Navigation, Speed Information/Alert, Traffic Information, and Green Driving Support) and participants were tested over a period ranging from 8 to 16 months in five European countries.
  • When looking at results, 41% of participants expected the safety of their driving to improve when they used a Navigation system (Greece, Italy, Spain, Sweden, and the UK); 62% anticipated an improvement in safety with the use of a Speed Information/Alert system (UK, Italy, and Spain); 50% anticipated an improvement in safety with the use of Traffic Information (Sweden and Greece); and 20% of the drivers anticipated an improvement in safety whilst driving with Green Driving Support (Greece only).
  • For Navigation, upon comparing pre-test with post-test responses, no changes in responses were observed for Sweden, Italy, or Spain. However, in the UK and in Greece there was a shift towards perceived negative effects upon safety compared to original expectations.
  • For Speed Information/Alert, at the end of testing, there were significant differences in the perceived safety in the UK and Italy, with a move towards ‘No Change’—but there was no significant difference in the pre- and post-FOT responses in Spain.
  • For Traffic Information, there was a significant difference in the post-FOT response for Sweden, with a distinct move towards perceiving no change in safety. There was a similar pattern in the data from Greece; however, this was not a statistically significant change.
  • For Green Driving Support in Greece, the numbers of drivers reporting ‘No Change’ in perception of safety increased post-FOT to 88%, although this was not statistically significant.
  • The results suggest that perceptions of safety for the tested functions do not necessarily increase with exposure to them, as might be expected.

Author Contributions

R.W. collected the data for the UK LFOT (Large-Scale Field Operational Test) and undertook the analysis of the data for all countries included. A.M. contributed to the experimental design, the discussion and the conclusions.

Funding

This research was funded by the European Commission under Framework Program FP7 of Information and Communication Technologies FP7-ICT-2007-2.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Statista. Number of Mobile Phone Users Worldwide from 2013 to 2019. The Statistics Portal. 2017. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/274774/forecast-of-mobile-phone-users-worldwide/ (accessed on 3 February 2017).
  2. Young, K.; Regan, M. Driver distraction: A review of the literature. In Distracted Driving; Faulks, I.J., Regan, M., Stevenson, M., Brown, J., Porter, A., Irwin, J.D., Eds.; Australasian College of Road Safety: Sydney, Australia, 2007; pp. 379–405. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alm, H.; Nilsson, L. The effects of a mobile telephone task on driver behaviour in a car following situation. Accid. Anal. Prev. 1995, 27, 717–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Burns, P.C.; Parkes, A.; Burton, S.; Smith, R.K.; Burch, D. How Dangerous Is Driving with a Mobile Phone? Benchmarking the Impairment to Alcohol; TRL Report TRL547; TRL Limited: Berkshire, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  5. Haigney, D.E.; Taylor, R.G.; Westerman, S.J. Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone use and driving performance: Task demand characteristics and compensatory processes. Transp. Res. Part F 2000, 3, 113–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Rakauskas, M.E.; Gugerty, L.J.; Ward, N.J. Effects of naturalistic cell phone conversation on driving performance. J. Saf. Res. 2004, 35, 453–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Jamson, A.H.; Westerman, S.J.; Hockey, G.R.J.; Carsten, M.J. Speech-based email and driver behaviour: Effects of an in-vehicle message system interface. Hum. Factors 2004, 46, 625–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Strayer, D.L.; Drews, F.A.; Johnston, W.A. Cell-phone induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2003, 9, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Strayer, D.L.; Drews, F.A. Profiles of driver distraction: Effects of cell phone conversations on younger and older drivers. Hum. Factors 2004, 46, 640–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Brookhuis, K.A.; de Vries, G.; de Waard, D. The effects of mobile telephoning on driving performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 1991, 23, 309–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Chiang, D.P.; Brooks, A.M.; Weir, D.H. An Experimental Study of Destination Entry with an Example Automobile Navigation System; Special Publication SP-1593; Society of Automotive Engineers: Warrendale, PA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  12. Harbluk, J.L.; Noy, Y.I.; Eizenman, M. The Impact of Cognitive Distraction on Driver Visual Behaviour and Vehicle Control; TP No. 13889 E; Transport Canada: Ottowa, ON, Canada, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  13. Field Operational Test Networking and Methodology Promotion FETSA Handbook (FOT_NET). 2016. Available online: http://fot-net.eu/wp-content/uploads/sites/7/2017/01/FESTA-Handbook-Version-6.pdf (accessed on 19 November 2018).
  14. Welsh, R.; Morris, A.P.; Reed, S.; Wallgren, P.; Innamaa, S.; Rama, A.; Perez, O.M.; Birrell, S.; Roberto Brignolo, D.W.; Guidotti, L. TeleFOT [Field Operational Tests of Aftermarket and Nomadic Devices in Vehicles]. D4.3.3 Vol. 0.4 Impacts on Safety—Results and Implications; European Commission 7th Framework Programme; TeleFOT: Espoo, Finland, 2012; pp. 1–131. [Google Scholar]
  15. Harvey, C.; Stanton, N.; Pickering, C.; McDonald, M.; Zheng, P. Vehicle Information Systems to Meet the Needs of Drivers. Int. J. Hum. -Comput. Interact. 2011, 27, 505–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Hassenzahl, M.; Tractinsky, T. User experience—A research agenda. Behav. Inf. Technol. 2006, 25, 91–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Morris, A.P.; Welsh, R.H.; Reed, S.; Brown, L.; Birrell, S. The Distraction Effects of Navigation and Green-Driving Systems—Results from Field Operational Tests (FOTs) in the UK. Eur. Transp. Res. Rev. 2015, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Smith, A. Older Adults and Technology Use. Pew Research Center Internet and American Life Project. 2014. Available online: http://www.pewinternet.org/2014/04/03/older-adults-and-technology-use/ (accessed on 19 November 2018).
Figure 1. Navigation: perception of safety pre- and post-field operational test (FOT).
Figure 1. Navigation: perception of safety pre- and post-field operational test (FOT).
Safety 04 00054 g001
Figure 2. Speed Information/Alert: perception of safety pre- and post-FOT.
Figure 2. Speed Information/Alert: perception of safety pre- and post-FOT.
Safety 04 00054 g002
Figure 3. Traffic Information: perception of safety pre- and post-FOT.
Figure 3. Traffic Information: perception of safety pre- and post-FOT.
Safety 04 00054 g003
Figure 4. Green Driving Support: perception of safety pre- and post-FOT.
Figure 4. Green Driving Support: perception of safety pre- and post-FOT.
Safety 04 00054 g004
Figure 5. Pre-FOT: expected safety impact per function.
Figure 5. Pre-FOT: expected safety impact per function.
Safety 04 00054 g005
Figure 6. Post-FOT: perceived safety impact per function.
Figure 6. Post-FOT: perceived safety impact per function.
Safety 04 00054 g006
Table 1. TeleFOT field operational tests included in the “safety” perception analysis.
Table 1. TeleFOT field operational tests included in the “safety” perception analysis.
FOTFunctions TestedSystem UsedDurationFOT Sample SizeAnalysis Sample Size
UK LFOTNavigation
Speed Info/Alert
NDrive12 months80Navigation—42
Speed Info/Alert—39
Sweden LFOT2Navigation
Traffic Information
Green Driving
Garmin Nuvi 205wt with EcoRoute8 months96Navigation—68
Traffic Information—60
Green Driving—62
Greece LFOTNavigation
Traffic Information
Samsung OMNIA 1111 months148Navigation—138
Traffic Information—116
Italy LFOTNavigation
Speed Info/Alert
Acer beTouchE101 smart phone16 months168Navigation—111
Speed Info/Alert—110
Spain LFOT1Navigation
Speed Info/Alert
NDrive13 months120Navigation—95
Speed Info/Alert—94
Table 2. Navigation: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
Table 2. Navigation: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
CountrySig(p)Conclusion
UK0.03Decrease in perceived safety post-FOT based on negative ranks
Sweden0.896No change in perceived safety post-FOT
Greece<0.001Decrease in perceived safety post-FOT based on negative ranks
Italy0.118No change in perceived safety post-FOT
Spain0.719No change in perceived safety post-FOT
Table 3. Speed Information/Alert: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
Table 3. Speed Information/Alert: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
CountrySig(p)Conclusion
UK0.02Decrease in perceived safety post-FOT based on negative ranks
Italy<0.001Decrease in perceived safety post-FOT based on negative ranks
Spain0.173No change in perceived safety post-FOT
Table 4. Traffic Information: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
Table 4. Traffic Information: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
CountrySig(p)Conclusion
Sweden<0.001Decrease in perceived safety post FOT based on negative ranks
Greece0.058No change in perceived safety post-FOT
Table 5. Green Driving Support: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
Table 5. Green Driving Support: Wilcoxon test for differences in pre-/post-FOT responses.
CountrySig(p)Conclusion
Sweden0.108No change in perceived safety post-FOT

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Welsh, R.; Morris, A. Perceived Safety Benefits of Aftermarket Driver Support Systems: Results from a Large Scale European Field Operational Test (FOT). Safety 2018, 4, 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety4040054

AMA Style

Welsh R, Morris A. Perceived Safety Benefits of Aftermarket Driver Support Systems: Results from a Large Scale European Field Operational Test (FOT). Safety. 2018; 4(4):54. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety4040054

Chicago/Turabian Style

Welsh, Ruth, and Andrew Morris. 2018. "Perceived Safety Benefits of Aftermarket Driver Support Systems: Results from a Large Scale European Field Operational Test (FOT)" Safety 4, no. 4: 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety4040054

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop