Attention-Setting and Human Mental Function
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Pieces of History
1.2. Attention-Setting
- Theory and Evidence behind the Example
- A comprehensive theory of visual search has been developed by Wolfe and colleagues and provides details on many important visual mechanisms—Guided Search Version 6.0 [34]. The theory integrates well-supported details of sensory coding channels and the priority map, and the paper provides useful further references. Zelinsky, Chen, Ahn, and Adeli [35] provide an amazing catalog of computational search models, with an emphasis on the general problem and real-world scenes. Zelinsky et al. treat eye fixations, which are closely linked to attention; they provide a complementary theoretical approach to top-down influences (see also [36]).
- Attentional templates are included in most search models, and many experiments measure the set-up of the templates. Priority maps are also central constructs; they combine top-down knowledge and visual features from bottom-up parallel processing (e.g., [34,37,38]). Priority maps are used to guide the search toward likely target locations and away from unlikely locations [39]. The trade-offs in energetic resources between different tasks is a long-standing topic in basic and applied research (e.g., [27,40]). Internal attention, such as turning attention into one’s memory, is becoming a distinct research topic (e.g., [41,42]). Unconscious problem solving is a growing area of research. The idea that processes will be modified over time, through interaction with the world, was proposed by Neisser [1] as the perceptual cycle. We will develop the idea below as a useful framework for understanding attention over the time scale of seconds.
1.3. Relations to Other Major Concepts and Processes
1.4. The Present Approach
2. Attention-Setting and Gorilla Missing
Gorilla Missing with More Control, and Bottom-Up Capture
3. Attention-Setting in Time
3.1. Information and Attention over Seconds
3.2. Evidence from the Seconds Time Scale
- Details and Illustration of Method
- Before the test period, participants were trained with the four tasks and their distinct object-types. Only one token was shown at a time during training. Observers learned each task to near perfection. Then testing began, with many tokens on each trial. The tasks are described next, but first we describe a typical trial. At the start of a trial, the four (empty) quadrants were shown until the observer pressed a spacebar. Then, object tokens started to appear, one at a time but several per second, until reaching the maximum of 12 active tokens. Each token was offset at the end of its lifetime of 4 sec, and a replacement token would soon appear. During the 60 sec trials, there was a total of 136 distractor tokens and 8 target tokens, distributed throughout the quadrants. Observers were instructed to respond only to targets. On single-task trials, observers would see only one object-type and one task in all four quadrants (e.g., Figure 1a). There were two multi-task conditions used in the experiment. During the first type, there was one type of a different task in each quadrant (4 tasks in total; Figure 1b) but otherwise the same timing parameters as single-task trials. The second type (Figure 1c) will be explained below.
- The four tasks were thought to require different processes in the brain, and they were distinguished by their distinct object tokens. The color task had square tokens changing from green-yellow to yellow and back, as mentioned. The shape task had red tokens changing in concavity, from fat-diamonds to concave (star-like) and back. The location task had grey squares moving linearly (and bouncing off walls); targets passed through the central square outline (“more” is defined as the proximity to the central square). Finally, in the motion task, the blue squares moved left to right with up/down deviations; the targets moved up or down more, as if drunk.
4. The Biological Attention-Setting Machine
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Neisser, U. Cognition & Reality; W.H.Freeman & Co Ltd.: New York, NY, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- James, W. Chapter XI: Attention. In The Principles of Psychology; Holt: New York, NY, USA, 1890. [Google Scholar]
- Neisser, U. Cognitive Psychology; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Yantis, S. Control of visual attention. In Attention; Pashler, H., Ed.; Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis: Hove, UK, 1998; pp. 223–256. [Google Scholar]
- Tsotsos, J.K. When We Study the Ability to Attend, What Exactly Are We Trying to Understand? Unpublished manuscript.
- Tsotsos, J.K.; Abid, O.; Kotseruba, I.; Solbach, M.D. On the control of attention processes in vision. Cortex 2021, 137, 305–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Angell, J.R.; Pierce, A.H. Experimental research upon the phenomena of attention. Am. J. Psychol. 1892, 4, 528–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swift, E.J. Disturbance of the Attention during simple Mental Processes. Am. J. Psychol. 1892, 5, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jersild, A.T. Mental Set and Shift. Arch. Psychol. 1927, 89, 5–82. [Google Scholar]
- Broadbent, D.E. Failures of attention in selective listening. J. Exp. Psychol. 1952, 44, 428–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Broadbent, D. Perception and Communication; Pergamon Press: London, UK, 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Deutsch, J.A.; Deutsch, D. Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychol. Rev. 1963, 70, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schneider, W.; Shiffrin, R.M. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 1–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D.; Treisman, A. Changing views of attention and automaticity. In Variants of Attention; Parasuraman, R., Davies, D.R., Beatty, J., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1984; pp. 29–61. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn, T. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 1st ed.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Johnston, W.A.; Heinz, S.P. Flexibility and capacity demands of attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1978, 107, 420–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lavie, N. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 75–82. [Google Scholar]
- Massaro, D.W. Experimental Psychology and Information Processing; Rand McNally College Publishing Company: Chicago, IL, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer, S.E. Visual perception and world knowledge: Notes on a model of sensory-cognitive interaction. Explor. Cogn. 1975, 279–307. [Google Scholar]
- Franconeri, S.L. The Nature and Status of Visual Resources. In Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology; Reisberg, D., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Geng, J.J.; Leber, A.B.; Shomstein, S. Attention and Perception: 40 reviews, 40 views. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019, 29, v–viii. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lavie, N. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1995, 21, 451–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sanocki, T.; Sulman, N. Complex, dynamic scene perception: Effects of attentional set on perceiving single and multiple event types. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2013, 39, 381–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tsotsos, J.K. Analyzing vision at the complexity level. Behav. Brain Sci. 1990, 13, 4233–4469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norman, D.A.; Bobrow, D.G. On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cogn. Psychol. 1975, 7, 44–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wahn, B.; König, P. Is Attentional Resource Allocation across Sensory Modalities Task-Dependent? Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2017, 13, 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wickens, C.D. Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci. 2002, 3, 159–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folk, C.L.; Remington, R.W.; Wright, J.H. The structure of attentional control: Contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1994, 20, 317–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Most, S.B.; Scholl, B.J.; Clifford, E.R.; Simons, D.J. What You See Is What You Set: Sustained Inattentional Blindness and the Capture of Awareness. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 112, 217–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dehaene, S.; Changeux, J.P.; Naccache, L.; Sackur, J.; Sergent, C. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Desimone, R.; Duncan, J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1995, 18, 193–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fan, J.; McCandliss, B.D.; Fossella, J.; Flombaum, J.I.; Posner, M.I. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 2005, 26, 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Franconeri, S.L.; Alvarez, G.A.; Cavanagh, P. Flexible cognitive resources: Competitive content maps for attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013, 17, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wolfe, J.M. Guided Search 6.0: An updated model of visual search. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2021, 28, 1060–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelinsky, G.; Chen, Y.; Ahn, S.; Adeli, H. Changing perspectives on goal-directed attention control: The past, present, and future of modeling fixations during visual search. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020; pp. 231–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rolfs, M. Attention in Active Vision: A Perspective on Perceptual Continuity Across Saccades. Perception 2015, 44, 900–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Eckstein, M.P.; Drescher, B.A.; Shimozaki, S.S. Attentional cues in real scenes, saccadic targeting, and Bayesian priors. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 973–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torralba, A.; Oliva, A.; Castelhano, M.; Henderson, J.M. Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: The role of global features in object search. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 766–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ng, G.J.P.; Patel, T.N.; Buetti, S.; Lleras, A. Prioritization in Visual Attention Does Not Work the Way You Think It Does. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2021, 47, 252–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kramer, A.F.; Wiegmann, D.A.; Kirlik, A. Attention: From Theory to Practice; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Chun, M.M.; Golomb, J.D.; Turk-Browne, N.B. A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 73–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Logan, G.D.; Cox, G.E.; Annis, J.; Lindsey, D.R.B. The episodic flanker task: Memory retrieval as attention turned inward. Psychol. Rev. 2021, 128, 397–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, C.M. Inattentional blindness: Perception or memory and what does it matter? Psyche 2001, 7, 178–194. [Google Scholar]
- Burgoyne, A.P.; Engle, R.W. Attention Control: A Cornerstone of Higher-Order Cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 29, 624–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Baldauf, D.; Deubel, H. Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping. Vis. Res. 2010, 50, 999–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Eppinger, B.; Goschke, T.; Musslick, S. Meta-control: From psychology to computational neuroscience. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2021, 21, 447–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, T.W.; Graziano, M.S. The attention schema theory: A mechanistic account of subjective awareness. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Duncan, J.; Chylinski, D.; Mitchell, D.J.; Bhandari, A. Complexity and compositionality in fluid intelligence. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 5295–5299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barsalou, L.W.; Prinz, J.J. Mundane creativity in perceptual symbol systems. In Creative Thought: An Investigation of Conceptual Structures and Processes; Ward, T.B., Smith, S.M., Vaid, J., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1997; pp. 267–307. [Google Scholar]
- Seel, N.M. Mental Models and Creative Invention. In Encyclopedia of Creativity, Invention, Innovation and Entrepreneurship; Carayannis, E.G., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Ward, T.B.; Smith, S.M.; Vaid, J.E. Creative Thought: An Investigation of Conceptual Structures and Processes; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1997; ISBN 978-1-55798-906-2. [Google Scholar]
- Pearson, J.; Keogh, R. Redefining Visual Working Memory: A Cognitive-Strategy, Brain-Region Approach. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 28, 266–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malmberg, K.J.; Raaijmakers, J.G.W.; Shiffrin, R.M. 50 Years of Research Sparked by Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968). Mem. Cogn. 2019, 47, 561–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Logan, G.D. Automatic control: How experts act without thinking. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 125, 453–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Posner, M.I.; Rothbart, M.K.; Tang, Y.Y. Enhancing attention through training. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 4, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenberg, M.D.; Finn, E.S.; Scheinost, D.; Constable, R.T.; Chun, M.M. Characterizing Attention with Predictive Network Models. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2017, 21, 290–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Simons, D.J.; Chabris, C.F. Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 1999, 28, 1059–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koch, I.; Poljac, E.; Müller, H.; Kiesel, A. Cognitive structure, flexibility, and plasticity in human multitasking—An integrative review of dual-task and task-switching research. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Monsell, S. Task switching. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 134–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandierendonck, A.; Liefooghe, B.; Verbruggen, F. Task switching: Interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 601–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pashler, H. Attention; Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis: Hove, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Most, S.B.; Simons, D.J.; Scholl, B.J.; Jimenez, R.; Clifford, E.; Chabris, C.F. How not to be seen: The contribution of similarity and selective ignoring to sustained inattentional blindness. Psychol. Sci. 2001, 12, 9–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mack, A.; Rock, I. Inattentional Blindness; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Moray, N. Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1959, 11, 56–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treisman, A.M. Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychol. Rev. 1969, 76, 282–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gronau, N.; Cohen, A.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Dissociations of Personally Significant and Task-Relevant Distractors Inside and Outside the Focus of Attention: A Combined Behavioral and Psychophysiological Study. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2003, 132, 512–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ahissar, M.; Hochstein, S. The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004, 8, 457–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paap, K.R.; Newsome, S.L.; McDonald, J.E.; Schvaneveldt, R.W. An activation–verification model for letter and word recognition: The word-superiority effect. Psychol. Rev. 1982, 89, 573–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tsotsos, J.K.; Culhane, S.; Cutzu, F. From Theoretical Foundations to a Hierarchical Circuit for Selective Attention, Visual Attention and Cortical Circuits; Braun, J., Koch, C., Davis, J., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; pp. 285–306. [Google Scholar]
- Bar, M. From objects to unified minds. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 30, 129–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Theeuwes, J. Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Percept. Psychophys. 1992, 51, 599–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Theeuwes, J. Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: Selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1994, 20, 799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gronau, N. To Grasp the World at a Glance: The Role of Attention in Visual and Semantic Associative Processing. J. Imaging 2022, 7, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bacon, W.F.; Egeth, H.E. Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Percept. Psychophys. 1994, 55, 485–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dreisbach, G.; Haider, H. That’s what task sets are for: Shielding against irrelevant information. Psychol. Res. 2008, 72, 355–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folk, C.L.; Remington, R.W.; Johnston, J.C. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1992, 18, 1030–1044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wyble, B.; Folk, C.; Potter, M.C. Contingent attentional capture by conceptually relevant images. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2013, 39, 861–871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Luck, S.J.; Gaspelin, N.; Folk, C.L.; Remington, R.W.; Theeuwes, J. Progress toward resolving the attentional capture debate. Vis. Cogn. 2021, 29, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cunningham, S.J.; Vogt, J.; Martin, D. Me first? Positioning self in the attentional hierarchy. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2022, 48, 115–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schäfer, S.; Wentura, D.; Frings, C. Creating a network of importance: The particular effects of self-relevance on stimulus processing. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2020, 82, 3750–3766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Elgendi, M.; Kumar, P.; Barbic, S.; Howard, N.; Abbott, D.; Cichocki, A. Subliminal priming—State of the art and future perspectives. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van den Bussche, E.; Van den Noortgate, W.; Reynvoet, B. Mechanisms of masked priming: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 452–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Raymond, J.E.; Shapiro, K.L.; Arnell, K.M. Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1992, 18, 849–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chun, M.M.; Potter, M.C. A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1995, 21, 109–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dux, P.E.; Marois, R. The attentional blink: A review of data and theory. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2009, 71, 1683–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olivers, C.N.L.; van der Stigchel, S.; Hulleman, J. Spreading the sparing: Against a limited-capacity account of the attentional blink. Psychol. Res. 2007, 71, 126–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Einhäuser, W.; Koch, C.; Makeig, S. The duration of the attentional blink in natural scenes depends on stimulus category. Vis. Res. 2007, 47, 597–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tang, M.F.; Ford, L.; Arabzadeh, E.; Enns, J.T.; Troy, A.W.V.; Mattingley, J.B. Neural dynamics of the attentional blink revealed by encoding orientation selectivity during rapid visual presentation. Nat. Commun. 2020, 11, 434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wyble, B.; Callahan-Flintoft, C.; Chen, H.; Marinov, T.; Sarkar, A.; Bowman, H. Understanding visual attention with RAGNAROC: A reflexive attention gradient through neural AttRactOr competition. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 127, 1163–1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bransford, J.D.; Johnson, M.K. Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1972, 11, 717–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wiley, J.; Rayner, K. Effects of titles on the processing of text and lexically ambiguous words: Evidence from eye movements. Mem. Cogn. 2000, 28, 1011–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bransford, J.D.; Brown, A.L.; Cocking, R.R. How People Learn; National Academy Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2004; Volume 11. [Google Scholar]
- Loschky, L.C.; Hutson, J.P.; Smith, M.E.; Smith, T.J.; Magliano, J.P. Viewing Static Visual Narratives through the Lens of the Scene Perception and Event Comprehension Theory (SPECT). In Empirical Comics Research: Digital, Multimodal, and Cognitive Methods; Laubrock, J., Wildfeuer, J., Dunst, A., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 217–238. [Google Scholar]
- Grant, E.R.; Spivey, M.J. Eye movements and problem solving: Guiding attention guides thought. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 462–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rouinfar, A.; Agra, E.; Larson, A.M.; Rebello, N.S.; Loschky, L.C. Linking attentional processes and conceptual problem solving: Visual cues facilitate the automaticity of extracting relevant information from diagrams. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1094–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hegarty, M.; Canham, M.S.; Fabrikant, S.I. Thinking about the weather: How display salience and knowledge affect performance in a graphic inference task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2010, 36, 37–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Horrey, W.J.; Wickens, C.D.; Consalus, K.P. Modeling drivers’ visual attention allocation while interacting with in-vehicle technologies. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2006, 12, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strayer, D.L.; Cooper, J.M.; Turrill, J.; Coleman, J.R.; Hopman, R.J. Talking to your car can drive you to distraction. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2016, 1, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kunar, M.A.; Watson, D.G. Visual Search in a Multi-Element Asynchronous Dynamic (MAD) World. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2011, 37, 1017–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Crittenden, B.M.; Mitchell, D.J.; Duncan, J. Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. Elife 2015, 4, e06481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fedorenko, E.; Duncan, J.; Kanwisher, N. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 16616–16621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Shashidhara, S.; Mitchell, D.J.; Erez, Y.; Duncan, J. Progressive Recruitment of the Frontoparietal Multiple-demand System with Increased Task Complexity, Time Pressure, and Reward. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 31, 1617–1630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marr, D. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information; W.H. Freeman: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Hillman, C.H.; Erickson, K.I.; Kramer, A.F. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Milham, M.P.; Erickson, K.I.; Banich, M.T.; Kramer, A.F.; Webb, A.; Wszalek, T.; Cohen, N.J. Attentional control in the aging brain: Insights from an fMRI study of the Stroop task. Brain Cogn. 2002, 49, 277–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Prakash, R.S.; Voss, M.W.; Erickson, K.I.; Lewis, J.M.; Chaddock, L.; Malkowski, E.; Alves, H.; Kim, J.; Szabo, A.; White, S.M.; et al. Cardiorespiratory fitness and attentional control in the aging brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 4, 229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sahakian, B.J.; Langley, C.; Kaser, M.; University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK. How chronic stress changes the brain—And what you can do to reverse the damage. Personal communication, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Berman, M.G.; Jonides, J.; Kaplan, S. The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 1207–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schertz, K.E.; Berman, M.G. Understanding nature and its cognitive benefits. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 28, 496–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goleman, D.; Davidson, R.J. Altered Traits: Science Reveals How Meditation Changes Your Mind, Brain, and Body; Penguin: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Dahl, C.J.; Wilson-Mendenhall, C.H.; Davidson, R.J. The plasticity of well-being: A training-based framework for the cultivation of human flourishing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 32197–32206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sanocki, T.; Lee, J.H. Attention-Setting and Human Mental Function. J. Imaging 2022, 8, 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/jimaging8060159
Sanocki T, Lee JH. Attention-Setting and Human Mental Function. Journal of Imaging. 2022; 8(6):159. https://doi.org/10.3390/jimaging8060159
Chicago/Turabian StyleSanocki, Thomas, and Jong Han Lee. 2022. "Attention-Setting and Human Mental Function" Journal of Imaging 8, no. 6: 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/jimaging8060159
APA StyleSanocki, T., & Lee, J. H. (2022). Attention-Setting and Human Mental Function. Journal of Imaging, 8(6), 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/jimaging8060159