Next Article in Journal
Wine Aging Technologies
Previous Article in Journal
Coconut Neera—A Vital Health Beverage from Coconut Palms: Harvesting, Processing and Quality Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effect of the Tactile Attributes of a Container on Mineral Water Perception

1
Department of Psychology and NeuroMi, Center for Neuroscience, University of Milan-Bicocca, P.zza dell’Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126 Milan, Italy
2
SCHI Lab, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RH, UK
3
Department of Mechanical Engineering, Politecnico di Milano, 20156 Milan, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Beverages 2019, 5(1), 23; https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages5010023
Submission received: 29 December 2018 / Revised: 9 February 2019 / Accepted: 27 February 2019 / Published: 5 March 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Drinking Water)

Abstract

:
The present study investigates the effect of container texture on people’s perception of four characteristics (i.e., freshness, pleasantness, level of carbonation, lightness) of mineral water (i.e., still or carbonated). Water was served in three commercial cups covered with a layer of sandpaper, satin, or the same material of the cup (plastic). The blindfolded participants were asked to evaluate the mineral water using visual analogue scales. The results showed that mineral water was perceived as fresher and more pleasant when contained in plastic cups than when it was contained in cups covered with sandpaper or satin. Moreover, mineral water was perceived as lighter when contained in plastic cups than when it was contained in cups covered with sandpaper. These results suggest that people’s perception of some characteristics of mineral water can be modulated by the texture of the container in which the liquid is served.

1. Introduction

As any professional chef would know, our experience of food is not solely related to the sensations arising from the activation of a few chemical receptors on our tongue (see [1,2] for reviews). Just as it occurs for other aspects of our environment, beverage and food perception is determined by the complex interactions taking place among the different sensorial aspects of the stimuli and of their background, as well as by the way in which the brain integrates them into a whole experience.
A number of studies in the last two decades have started to investigate these important aspects [2]. However, very few studies have specifically investigated taste–tactile interactions in beverage perception. Among these, Szczesniak [3], somehow unsurprisingly, has shown that beverages containing lumps or hard particles make them unacceptable or inappropriate for participants. This effect is likely due to the fact that they provoke the fear of choking. Schifferstein [4] used cups made of different materials (i.e., translucent plastic, opaque plastic, melamine, glass, ceramic) and asked participants to evaluate two different beverages: soda and hot tea. The participants also rated the empty cups with respect to a set of characteristics related to affective dimensions such as ‘pleasant–unpleasant’ or ‘good–bad’, as well as sensory perception across dimensions such as ‘heavy–light’ and ‘thick–thin’. The author found that the drinking experience followed the experience of the cups (e.g., the more pleasant the cup was perceived, the more pleasant the drink was perceived). Similarly, Krishna and Morrin [5] investigated the effect of the firmness of a plastic cup on the taste perception of a mixed drink. The results showed that the firmer a cup was, the higher the participants evaluated the quality of the drink inside. Finally, Tu et al. [6] investigated the effect of different packaging materials on taste perception of a traditional Chinese cold tea beverage. The results showed that people rated the sense of ice (a sub-dimension of the scale measuring sweetness) of the beverage as significantly higher when tasted by means of a glass cup with respect to paper or organic plastic cups.
It is worth noting here that touch plays a very important role for our emotional wellbeing [7,8,9] and that tactile sensations can convey emotions [10], just as the visual properties of stimuli do [11]). Consequently, the emotional valence of the container textures [12] during the manipulation might be transferred to the perception of the liquid contained in it, thus influencing its perceptual evaluation (e.g., more pleasant textures might lead to more pleasant evaluations of the water). Recently, Wang and Spence [13] demonstrated that the same juice was rated as sweeter when presented together with visual and auditory positive-valenced stimuli (emotional faces and soundtracks harmonised with consonant music intervals) than when presented together with negatively-valenced stimuli. Importantly, as pointed out by the authors, to date the neural and cognitive correlates at the basis of these sensation transference effects (from the container to the content) are not completely clear.
The container texture effect on food might also be driven by people’s expectations based on their memory of previous experiences (exposure effects) with water [14,15]. In this case, the more the container deviates from a number of features perceived as ‘standard’ in water containers, the more the water was perceived as being less pleasant and lower in quality. Hence, one might expect that often-used containers, such as plastic glasses, lead to more positive water evaluations than novel—differently textured—containers [16,17,18]. It is worth noting that in the studies reported so far, the container textures were all somehow congruent and compatible with participants’ previous drinking experiences. Consequently, it would seem difficult to test the hypothesis of an exposure effect [14,15,16,17,18]. In fact, the extant literature lacks studies with novel or unusual container textures. Note, however, that companies are constantly searching for novel materials (possibly eco-sustainable) in order to enhance consumer experiences. Moreover, most previous studies used beverages characterised by specific odours, making it difficult to separate the effects determined by the multisensory interaction between taste and touch from those related to olfactory perception. In our paradigm, we tried to bypass these problems by using an odour-free liquid, namely water, and by using materials such as sandpaper and satin textures not commonly used for the production of water containers or packaging.
In our experiment, participants’ judgments on the liquid were expressed by means of four different scales: freshness, pleasantness, level of carbonation, and lightness. The same scales were previously used in other studies by our research group [19,20]. These scales were chosen among the descriptors of the categories used to classify the mouthfeel sensations of liquids [21]. As previously mentioned, the choice of mineral water was due to the fact that it is odourless so that smell did not interfere with the evaluation of the features of the liquid (conversely to the beverages used in previous experiments on this matter; e.g., Tu et al. [6]). By using this particular stimulus, we could directly analyse the effect of tactile qualities of containers on people’s taste perception of liquids, without the influence of other sensorial attributes of the stimuli. It is important to mention that in the present study, we wanted to exclude the so-called basic tastes (sweet, sour, bitter, salt) and odours perceived both retronasally and orthonasally from participants’ taste experience. The specific aim was to investigate tactile and trigeminal components of the flavour network in isolation [22,23,24].
Given that water is often defined as tasteless by people (i.e., with the exception of highly trained ‘hydrosommeliers’), we asked our participants to assess the characteristics that are more closely linked to thirst or to the advertising campaign related to mineral water [20,25,26] rather than the basic tastes (sweet, sour, salty, bitter, umami). If people’s perception of mineral water is affected by the emotional value of the stimuli, we would expect that certain characteristics of the water, such as its pleasantness, freshness, and lightness, would be enhanced by the tactile pleasantness of materials such as satin. By contrast, if people’s evaluations were mainly determined by exposure-related factors, we would expect that materials that deviate from the common drinking experience, such as satin and sandpaper, would result in negative effects: lower pleasantness, freshness and lightness.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Forty-eight participants with a mean age of 23.61 years (SD = 4.75, 38 females) took part in the experiment; they were graduate and undergraduate students. The students received course credits for their participation in the study. All the participants gave written consent prior to their participation. The experiment was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 2008 Declaration of Helsinki, and it was approved from the Bicocca University Ethics Committee. The experimental session lasted for approximately 40–50 min. This duration included an initial overview of the experimental setup, an explanation of the instructions, and a final debrief of 10 min regarding the main aims of the study.

2.2. Stimuli

Two different types of mineral water were used: sparkling and still mineral water (S. Benedetto® sparkling and still mineral water, 0.5 l bottle). Common plastic cups produced by ®DOpla S.p.A and varying only in terms of the material used to cover them (sandpaper, satin, and plastic) were used to serve the water. The textured covers (plastic, satin and sandpaper; see Figure 1) were selected on the basis of a previous study showing that across a range of ten different materials, people perceive sandpaper as very rough and unpleasant and satin as very smooth and pleasant [12]. Each cover was cut in such a way that only the participant’s hands could come in contact with the material. The textures were glued around the container, leaving uncovered only the part of the cup where the participant’s lips had to be placed in order to drink the content. Each plastic cup used was filled with 20 ml of water and the bottles used were always kept at constant room temperature (19–22 °C).

2.3. Procedure

The procedure followed a within-participants design. The study was conducted in an experimental room fitted with a laptop computer (screen resolution: 1024 × 768 pixels; screen size: 23 cm height × 30.6 cm width; refresh rate: 60 Hz). The computer was placed on a desk directly in front of the participants. The participants sat comfortably on a chair, approximately 50 cm from the laptop screen. In each trial, a plastic cup filled with water was placed on the desk in front of the participants by the experimenter at about 25–30 cm from the participant’s body. The individuals were blindfolded and instructed to grasp the cup when the experimenter placed it close to their fingers and to drink as much water as they wished. Given that the textures used were glued around the same plastic container that contained the water, the chemical properties of the liquid to be tasted remained constant for all experimental conditions. Immediately after the tasting session, the participants were asked to remove the blindfold and rate the water along four dimensions (freshness (In Italian the word ‘freschezza’, (freshness) has different meanings; in this case, we used the term with the meaning of thirst-quenching/refreshing capacity rather than of the water temperature itself.), pleasantness, carbonation and lightness) by means of horizontally-oriented, 150-mm-long visual analogue scales (VASs) anchored with the terms ‘not at all’ and ‘very much’ and presented on the centre of the PC screen. A mark on the VAS could be moved to the left or right by means of a computer mouse accordingly to the participant’s judgment. Participants were not informed that during the experiment the same two kinds of mineral water (still and sparkling) were always presented. At the end of the experimental session, people evaluated the pleasantness of the three textured cups using the same VASs of the previous part of the experiment. The participants were also requested to indicate what kind of mineral water they generally preferred to drink. Each type of water was presented 3 times in each different cup for a total of 18 (2 waters × 3 textures × 3 repetitions) samples to be evaluated. The order of presentation of each kind of water–texture combination was randomised across the entire experiment. The data were analysed with STATISTICA 6.0 (StatSoft, Padova, Italy).

3. Results

The participants’ mean ratings were submitted to a repeated measures ANOVA with the factors of texture of the plastic cup (i.e., sandpaper, satin, plastic), type of water (i.e., still, carbonated) and scales (i.e., freshness, pleasantness, level of carbonate, lightness). The results of the analysis revealed a significant main effect of the scale (F(3,141) = 13.96, p < 0.001) and of texture (F(2,94) = 5.42, p = 0.006), but not of water (F(1,47) = 3.68, p = 0.061). The interaction between scale and texture (F(6,282) = 2.37, p = 0.03), as well as the interaction between scale and water (F(3,141) = 118.43, p < 0.001), resulted to be significant. The interaction between texture and water (F(2,94) = 0.06, p = 0.94) and between scale, texture and water (F(6,282) = 1.14, p = 0.34) were not significant. Newman–Keuls post hoc tests were performed on all the significant effects. The main effect of scale showed that on average, participants provided quantitatively lower evaluations (i.e., evaluations closer to the ‘not at all’ endpoint of the scale) for the intensity of carbonation scale than for freshness (p < 0.001), pleasantness (p < 0.001) and lightness (p < 0.001) scales. A post hoc test on the significant effect of texture demonstrated that people on average provided quantitatively lower evaluations (i.e., evaluations closer to the ‘not at all’ endpoint of the scale) for sandpaper (p = 0.01) and satin (p = 0.01), as compared to the plastic texture. A post hoc test on the interaction between scale and texture revealed that participants perceived mineral water as fresher when served in a plastic cup as compared to a cup covered with sandpaper (p = 0.049) or satin (p = 0.028) (see Figure 2).
The post hoc test also revealed that water was perceived as more pleasant when served in a plastic cup as compared to water served in cups covered by sandpaper (p = 0.01) or satin (p = 0.04) (see Figure 3). Finally, the results showed that the mineral water was perceived as lighter when served in a plastic cup than when served in a cup covered by sandpaper (p = 0.054) (see Figure 4). No significant differences were found on the scale of carbonation intensity. A post hoc test on the interaction between the factors of scale and water revealed that participants perceived still water as fresher (p = 0.01), more pleasant (p < 0.001) and lighter (p < 0.001) than carbonated water. Not surprisingly, carbonated mineral water was perceived as more carbonated than still water (p < 0.001).
A univariate ANOVA was performed on the participants’ pleasantness evaluations of the three textures. The results of the analysis revealed a significant main effect of texture (F(2,141) = 13.06, p < 0.001). A Newman–Keuls post hoc test showed that participants rated sandpaper as less pleasant than the satin (p < 0.001) and plastic (p < 0.001). Finally, people’s responses regarding their ability to recognise the textures and their preference for the mineral water were calculated: 47 individuals recognised the plastic (98%), 41 recognised the sandpaper (82%) and 2 recognised the satin (4%). As far as the evaluations of the water preferences are concerned, 36 participants, in general, preferred to drink still water (75%), 10 carbonated water (21%), and 2 slightly carbonated water (4%).
A linear regression analysis was performed to evaluate the possible presence of a relationship between the perception of mineral water and the pleasantness of the textures on each of the four scales. The four dimensions investigated freshness (r = 0.11, p = 0.93), pleasantness (r = 0.35, p = 0.77), carbonation intensity (r = 0.93, p = 0.23) and lightness (r = 0.55, p = 0.63) of the water and did not show any significant correlation with the perceived pleasantness of the textures.

4. General Discussion and Conclusions

The results of the present study revealed that the perception of certain attributes/qualities of mineral water was affected by the different textures used to cover the cups in which the liquid was served. In particular, participants perceived both still and carbonated mineral water as fresher and more pleasant when the liquid was tasted in a plastic cup as compared to conditions where the cups were covered by sandpaper or satin. On the dimensions of lightness, we found that people perceived mineral water as lighter when the liquid was tasted in plastic cups as compared to conditions where the cups were covered by sandpaper. The dimension of carbonation intensity was not modulated by the different container textures.
As far as the result on the dimension of freshness is concerned, it is important to highlight that in Italian, this word is usually referred to the thirst-quenching characteristics of a drink more than its temperature. As a matter of fact, the water was served at constant room temperature (19–22 °C) and it is unlikely that the participants could perceive it as really ‘fresh’. Therefore, it is unlikely that by holding a plastic cup or holding a cup covered by satin or sandpaper, they could have perceived a different temperature of the liquid inside. Therefore, it is difficult to argue that participants’ water taste perception in our experiment was only affected by the different surface temperature of the three different cups adopted. However, we believe that temperature variables of the container might be able to contribute to people’s perception of the content. This aspect should be further investigated in the future.
Similar considerations also apply to the lightness dimension. That is, although the cups were covered by different materials, their physical weight was unlikely perceived as different by the participants (variations were in terms of fractions of milligrams). Moreover, if the effect found was due to weight differences between the glasses, probably such difference should have also involved the cup covered with satin, where instead no effect was reported.
It might be speculated that the degree of the appropriateness of the container to the whole drinking experience could explain the effect reported in the present study. In fact, it is possible that the participants, as a function of their previous experiences, considered the plastic cup—i.e., that one covered by the same material used to make the cup—as the more appropriate container from which to drink mineral water. In fact, this condition might be considered more similar to the typical beverage drinking experience stored in memory within a given culture. This experience-based interpretation agrees with Woods et al.’s [18] model, suggesting that flavour sensations fluctuate in order to harmonise with the expectations generated from actual and past food/beverage experiences. It might also be possible that participants transferred to the water some of the characteristics of the texture (such as the presence of rough particles of sandpaper and of fabrics), thus diminishing the level of acceptance of the water [3,27].
It is important to mention here that the effects found could also be related to emotional responses to the stimuli. Specifically, with reference to the affective ventriloquist effect [28]), the emotional response elicited by a tactile aspect of the container might affect the overall evaluation of the drinking experience. That is, containers covered with sandpaper and satin may have caused here an unpleasant negative feeling regarding the experience of drinking mineral water that is usually served in glass or plastic containers. Such a hypothesis could be strengthened by the fact that also the pleasantness dimension was decreased by the presence of unusual textures such as satin and sandpaper.
The perceptions of males and females were not evaluated in the current study.
In conclusion, the results of the present study show that the perception of mineral water can be modulated by the texture of the materials used to cover the cups in which the liquid is served. In particular, unusual container textures—at least those adopted in the present study—can lower people’s perception of certain mineral water characteristics. Further investigations should be directed to assess whether the effects found in the present study are consistent over time or fluctuate depending on people’s repeated exposure to specific textures and materials. Moreover, future research will also need to assess how different types of liquids interact with the same characteristics of the textures used to cover the containers. For example, one might study the effect of sandpaper-coated cups on the taste perception of grainy beverages such as pear juice. From an applied point of view, the results of the present study could help researchers in the field of environmentally-friendly materials. That is, it will be really important in the future to determine the perceptual effects of new and more eco-sustainable materials on people’s expectations, consumption and choice of food and beverage. Finally, our results might also suggest that unusual container textures could perhaps be used to contribute to reducing the consumption of beverages potentially harmful to health, for example, alcoholic and/or drinks with a high sugar content [2].

Author Contributions

Conceptualization P.R. and A.G.; Data curation P.R., R.E., A.G.; Investigation P.R., E.M.; Methodology P.R., E.M., R.E., A.G.; Project administration P.R., A.G.; Supervision E.M., R.E., A.G.

Funding

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Spence, C.; Piqueras-Fiszman, B. The Perfect Meal: The Multisensory Science of Food and Dining; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2014; ISBN 978-1-118-42082-2. [Google Scholar]
  2. Spence, C. Gastrophysics: The New Science of Eating; Viking, Penguin Random House: London, UK, 2017; ISBN 024127009X. [Google Scholar]
  3. Szczesniak, A.S. Texture is a sensory property. Food Qual. Prefer. 2002, 13, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Schifferstein, H.N.J. The drinking experience: Cup or content? Food Qual. Prefer. 2009, 20, 268–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Krishna, A.; Morrin, M. Does touch affect taste? The perceptual transfer of product container haptic cues. J. Consum. Res. 2008, 34, 807–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Tu, Y.; Yang, Z.; Ma, C. Touching tastes: The haptic perception transfer of liquid food packaging materials. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 124–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gallace, A.; Spence, C. The science of interpersonal touch: An overview. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 34, 246–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Hertenstein, M.J.; Verkamp, J.M.; Kerestes, A.M.; Holmes, R.M. The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: A review and synthesis of the empirical research. Genet. Soc. General Psychol. Monogr. 2006, 132, 5–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Walker, S.C.; McGlone, F.P. The social brain: Neurobiological basis of affiliative behaviors and psychological well-being. Neuropeptides 2013, 47, 379–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Hertenstein, M.J.; Keltner, D.; App, B.; Bulliet, B.A.; Jaskolka, A.R. Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotions 2006, 6, 528–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Paradiso, S.; Johnson, D.; Andreasen, N.C.; O’Leary, D.S.; Watkins, G.L.; Boles Ponto, L.L.; Hichwa, D. Cerebral blood flow changes associated with attribution of emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral visual stimuli in a PET study of normal subjects. Am. J. Psychiatry 2014, 156, 1618–1629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Etzi, R.; Spence, C.; Gallace, A. Textures that we like to touch: An experimental study of aesthetic preferences for tactile stimuli. Conscious Cogn. 2014, 29, 178–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Wang, Q.; Spence, C. “A sweet smile”: The modulatory role of emotion in how extrinsic factors influence taste evaluation influence taste evaluation. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 32, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Gallace, A.; Spence, C. A memory for touch: The cognitive science of tactile memory. In Touch in Museums: Policy and Practice in Object Handling; Berg: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 163–186. ISBN 978-1-84788-238-7. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gallace, A.; Spence, C. The cognitive and neural correlates of tactile memory. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 380–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Anderson, R.E. Consumer dissatisfaction: The effect of disconfirmed expectancy on perceived product performance. J. Mark. Res. 1973, 10, 38–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Woodruff, R.B.; Cadotte, E.R.; Jenkins, R.L. Modelling consumer using experience-based Norms. J. Mark. Res. 1983, 20, 286–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Woods, A.T.; Poliakoff, E.; Dijksterhuis, G.B.; Thomas, G.B. Flavor expectation: The effect of assuming homogeneity on drink perception. Chemosens. Percept. 2010, 3, 174–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Maggioni, E.; Risso, P.; Olivero, N.; Gallace, A. The effect of the weight of the container on people’s perception of mineral water. J. Sens. Stud. 2015, 30, 395–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Risso, P.; Gallace, A.; Olivero, N.; Gallace, A. The association between the colour of a container and the liquid inside: An experimental study on consumer’s perception, expectations and choices regarding mineral water. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 44, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Szczesniak, A.S. Classification of textural characteristics. J. Food Sci. 1979, 28, 385–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Auvray, M.; Spence, C. The multisensory perception of flavor. Conscious. Cogn. 2008, 3, 1016–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Spence, C. Oral referral: On the mislocalization of odours to the mouth. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 50, 117–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Spence, C.; Smith, B.; Auvray, M. Confusing tastes with flavours. In Perception and Its Modality; Oxford University: Oxford, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Dietrich, A.M. Aesthetic issues for drinking water. J. Water Health 2006, 4, 11–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Lucchiari, C.; Pravettoni, G. The effect of brand on EEG modulation. A study on mineral water. Swiss. J. Psychol. 2012, 71, 199–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Spence, C.; Wan, X. Beverage perception and consumption: The influence of the container on the perception of the contents. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 131–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Spence, C.; Gallace, A. Multisensory design: Reaching out to touch the consumer. Psychol. Mark. 2011, 3, 267–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The coatings used to cover the plastic cups (without the container inserted in them). Starting from the left: plastic, sandpaper and satin.
Figure 1. The coatings used to cover the plastic cups (without the container inserted in them). Starting from the left: plastic, sandpaper and satin.
Beverages 05 00023 g001
Figure 2. The participants’ mean judgments on the scale measuring freshness perception for both kinds of water as a function of the texture of the glass in which the liquid was served. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent Newman–Keuls significant differences at p < 0.05.
Figure 2. The participants’ mean judgments on the scale measuring freshness perception for both kinds of water as a function of the texture of the glass in which the liquid was served. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent Newman–Keuls significant differences at p < 0.05.
Beverages 05 00023 g002
Figure 3. The participants’ mean judgments on the scale measuring pleasantness perception for both kinds of water as a function of the texture of the glass in which the liquid was served. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent Newman-Keuls significant differences at p < 0.05.
Figure 3. The participants’ mean judgments on the scale measuring pleasantness perception for both kinds of water as a function of the texture of the glass in which the liquid was served. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent Newman-Keuls significant differences at p < 0.05.
Beverages 05 00023 g003
Figure 4. The participants’ mean judgments on the scale measuring lightness perception for both kinds of water as a function of the texture of the plastic glass in which the liquid was served. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent Newman–Keuls significant differences at p < 0.05.
Figure 4. The participants’ mean judgments on the scale measuring lightness perception for both kinds of water as a function of the texture of the plastic glass in which the liquid was served. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks represent Newman–Keuls significant differences at p < 0.05.
Beverages 05 00023 g004

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Risso, P.; Maggioni, E.; Etzi, R.; Gallace, A. The Effect of the Tactile Attributes of a Container on Mineral Water Perception. Beverages 2019, 5, 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages5010023

AMA Style

Risso P, Maggioni E, Etzi R, Gallace A. The Effect of the Tactile Attributes of a Container on Mineral Water Perception. Beverages. 2019; 5(1):23. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages5010023

Chicago/Turabian Style

Risso, Paola, Emanuela Maggioni, Roberta Etzi, and Alberto Gallace. 2019. "The Effect of the Tactile Attributes of a Container on Mineral Water Perception" Beverages 5, no. 1: 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages5010023

APA Style

Risso, P., Maggioni, E., Etzi, R., & Gallace, A. (2019). The Effect of the Tactile Attributes of a Container on Mineral Water Perception. Beverages, 5(1), 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/beverages5010023

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop