Next Article in Journal
Isolation of Exopolysaccharide-Producing Yeast and Lactic Acid Bacteria from Quinoa (Chenopodium Quinoa) Sourdough Fermentation
Previous Article in Journal
Production of Probiotic Passion Fruit (Passiflora edulis Sims f. flavicarpa Deg.) Drink Using Lactobacillus reuteri and Microencapsulation via Spray Drying
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Subjective Sensations related to Food as Determinants of Snack Choice

1
Department of Food Science, Faculty of Science and Technology, Aarhus University, Agro Food Park 48, DK 8200 Aarhus N, Denmark
2
Sensory Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Food Science, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2020, 9(3), 336; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9030336
Submission received: 3 March 2020 / Accepted: 10 March 2020 / Published: 12 March 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Sensory and Consumer Sciences)

Abstract

:
Comprehending the complexity and determinants of food choices can help understand facets of the growing obesity epidemic. Focusing on consumers’ subjective sensations as determinants of food choices can provide essential insights into eating behaviors. We explored subjective sensations linked to appetite, desire, wellbeing and energy. This study aims to 1) quantify subjective temporal sensations, and 2) study the effects of these sensations on snack choice. Two-hundred and fifty-three participants (mean age 20.5) evaluated subjective sensations using a visual analogue scale. The choice of one of six snacks was offered to the participants; choices were recorded as implicit choice measures. The results demonstrated that especially sensory specific desire sensations (Salty, Fatty, Sweet desire) affected, either positively or negatively, snack choice. Furthermore, wellbeing sensations (Overall, Mental, Physical wellbeing) also showed significant effects for snack choice. Health-conscious females chose healthy snacks, and males chose unhealthy snacks. Importantly, this research indicates the relevance of subjective sensations in consumer studies that focus on diverse determinants of food choice. Sensory specific desires and wellbeing sensations were notably shown to be important determinants of snack choice. The contribution of different sensations to food choice is imperative, and helps us to understand aspects of snacking behavior. This could have broader implications concerning public health issues and obesity.

1. Introduction

We live in a multifaceted world, and the processes around food choices and eating behavior are complex and multifactorial [1,2,3,4]. Seeking to comprehend the complexity and determinants of food choices might help us understand and elucidate some of the underlying facets of the growing obesity epidemic [5,6,7]. Several approaches have been applied to model food choices [2,3,8]. One of the most widely used is the Theory of Planned Behavior (TBP), linking one’s attitudes and behavioral intentions with one’s behavior. TBP functions as a cognitive model and theorizes that we make rational and conscious decisions based on attitudes and intentions [1,3,5]. Yet, stronger attitudes are thought to be more predictive of food choice than weaker, ambivalent attitudes [3,8]. Contrariwise, it has been established that intuitive and subconscious decision-making also play important roles in food-related behavior [4,9]. Often, researchers find inconsistencies or weak associations between intention and actual eating behavior [5,10,11]. For instance, Weijzen et al. (2009) found that 24% of consumers with a healthy snack choice intention actually chose an unhealthy snack instead, showing a discrepancy between intent and action [5]. Psychologist and Nobel Prize winner Daniel Kahneman differentiates between two cognitive systems within decision-making. His groundbreaking two-system view is characterized into a System 1 and a System 2. System 1 is characterized as intuitive, fast and automatic thinking, whereas System 2 is characterized as reasoned, conscious and controlled thinking. It can be said that in basic behavior like eating and drinking, intuitive thinking is a rule rather than an exception [4,9,12]. Actual snack choices are often made impulsively, and maybe even instinctively or unconsciously [13,14]. The realization that much decision making happens unintentionally leads us to reconsider choice measures. More emphasis should be placed on research that shows real behavioral effects, for instance observational studies of actual choices [4]. Measuring actual behavior can be an advantage in food choice studies where the answers are found in people’s choice behaviors, rather than measuring choice based on intention or “imaginary on paper”. This indeed favors more observational and implicit approaches, which permits consumers to deal with food in a natural way, and this behavioral research can be considered closer to real life.
In order to explain food choices, we as researchers seek to explore various factors that could play important roles in eating behavior. Psychobiological perspectives highlight the relationship between our appetite control system and food choices. Here, homeostatic processes such as hunger and satiety sensations drive eating behavior, reflecting a need or motivation to eat or not to eat [15,16]. Non-homeostatic processes also influence food choices, for instance eating/choosing food for pleasure or because of desires in order to achieve a hedonic reward [17,18,19]. Exploring subjective appetite and diverse subjective sensations and their relationship to choice can provide essential insights into intake behaviors [20,21]. The Merriam-Webster Dictionary defines a “sensation” as a “state of consciousness due to internal bodily changes, e.g., a sensation of hunger” [22]. We therefore argue that subjective sensations relate to one’s interoceptive state. “Interoception” is defined as our perception of internal bodily signals, e.g., hunger, pain, heat, satiety, muscular and visceral sensations [23,24,25,26]. It is furthermore suggested that interoception is the basis for subjective feelings, sensations and self-awareness, representing a subjective evaluation of “how I feel” [18,23]. With reference to this, the study of subjective sensations might contribute important knowledge to the complex area of understanding the diversity in determinants of food choices.
Research in the area of subjective appetite has moved towards a broader understanding of the concept. It points towards employing a more diverse and holistic approach by which to better understand human eating behavior [26,27,28,29,30]. Specifically, understanding how subjective wellbeing relates to food can contribute to a better understanding of consumer choices [28,31]. Food-related wellbeing and quality of life can be significant additions when conducting consumer food studies [26,27,28,32,33]. Researchers bring in multiple variables when assessing sensations in relation to eating and health [30,34,35,36]. For instance, food studies have used variables such as psychological wellbeing, nausea, physical wellbeing, desire for other foods, sensory specific desires, energy level, sleepiness, relaxation and other subjective evaluations [30,35,37]. However, most of these studies do not relate the sensations to subsequent food choices. Focusing on consumers’ subjective sensations as determinants of food choice, therefore, offers a new basis upon which to predict food choice. However, the importance and implications of subjective sensations relative to food choices have scarcely been researched.
This research study thus seeks to unravel the complex area of determinants of food choice by including subjective sensations as important consumer evaluations for food choice. The overall objective of this study is to quantitatively measure consumers’ subjective sensations, and thereafter, relate these subjective sensations to actual snack choices. Snack choice is assessed with a real, actual choice, hereby applying an implicit behavioral measurement. We believe that this implicit observational measure validates and represents an actual daily snack choice very well. Specifically, the study aims to (1) quantify subjective temporal sensations such as energy, wellbeing, hunger, satiety and desires, and (2) study the effects of subjective sensations on consumers’ choice of specific snacks. Our research questions therefore include:
  • What subjective temporal sensations contribute to snack choice?
  • Do other factors such as gender, time of day and health consciousness affect consumers’ snack choice?

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants, Recruitment and Procedure

Participants (ntotal = 253) were recruited from the campus at University of Otago in Dunedin, New Zealand. Participants included undergraduate and postgraduate students ranging from 18−30 years old. Inclusion criteria involved being above 18 years old and in general good health. Exclusion criteria involved having any food allergies. Table 1 displays the characteristics from the 253 participants. Recruitment occurred between 8 am and 4 pm on campus. Students were approached in person and asked if they wanted to participate in a study about appetite sensations. After agreeing to partake, participants were given instructions and handed an iPad to fill in the online questionnaire. After completion of the questionnaire, the iPad was returned to the researcher, and the participants were given the option to choose one snack from a selection of six types snack products. The researchers registered all participants’ snack choices. All participants gave written consent prior to the commencement of the study. University of Otago Human Ethics Committee approved this research (19/018).

2.2. Questionnaire

The choice of sensation variables and the development of the questionnaire were based on existing scientific literature on appetite and sensations, and were also adapted from Duerlund et al. (2019) [26,29,30,36,38].
Sensation measurements were collected using a visual analogue line scale (VAS) via the Compusense® Cloud software (Compusense Inc., Guelph, Ontario, Canada) in randomized order. A continuous scale ranging from 0, i.e., “not at all” to 10, i.e., “very much”, was used to evaluate each sensation. Subsequently, all participants filled out a demographics and psychographics section including gender, age, weight, height, time since last food intake and health consciousness related questions. Health consciousness was evaluated by asking participants the extent to which they agree with the following two statements: “I choose food carefully to ensure good health” and “I think of myself as a health-conscious consumer”, adapted by Squirel et al. (2001) and used also by other researchers evaluating consumer health consciousness [39,40,41]. The responses were collected on a seven point Likert scale ranging from “completely disagree” to completely agree”. Table 2 presents all questionnaire variables including demographic and psychographic background questions.

2.3. Snack Samples

A selection of six different snack products which are commercially available in New Zealand was chosen for the study. All snacks were presented unpacked to avoid any brand knowledge related biases. The six snacks were divided into two categories, with each representing a healthy and unhealthy version of a sensory taste profile, sweet, salty or fatty (Table 3). The snacks were chosen based on two criteria: Firstly, from interviews with New Zealand students about their perception of snack products (‘What is a snack?’); and secondly, from screening the available snack products in supermarkets, both through actual physical observation and online research of display and availability. Emphasis was given to choosing snacks with similar levels of familiarity to New Zealand students, and for snacks to reflect a similar price range, so as to avoid any biased choices based on expensive versus cheaper options. To avoid any biases related to portion size, the snacks were presented in portions that were visually comparable and regarded as a proper snack amount. Therefore, the portion size of the snacks depended not on calorie content but on appearance.

2.4. Real Snack Choice

Participants were offered a snack of their own choice as a token of thanks for their participation. The researcher noted the real snack choice as an implicit choice measure. The snacks were placed in small containers to avoid the risk of poor hygiene contamination. All snacks were represented four times in a display at all times, so that the snacks were always equally represented to allow choice based on attractiveness rather than availability; see Figure 1. The display was consistently refilled and randomized in an unstructured way to ensure that each participant always had the same amount of choice options. An assistant was appointed to carry out this task with care.

2.5. Data Analysis

Data on self-reported weight and height were used to calculate body mass index (BMI): weight (kg)/(height (m))2. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed for each sensation (dependent variable) and all snack types (explanatory variable, six levels) to analyze significant snack sample differences for each sensation. p-values ≤ 0.05 were considered statistically significant, and a Tukey’s Honest Significant Differences (HSD) test was applied for pairwise comparisons between the six snack types. Effect sizes were examined using Cohen’s d values [42]. A Principal Component Analysis (PCA), including Pearson correlations, was undertaken to reveal the patterns of correlations among the sensation variables. Binomial Logistic regression models were applied separately for each snack choice response in order to examine the effect of the subjective sensation state on the specific choice behavior. Subjective sensation variables functioned as the explanatory variables, and the snack choice as the implicit binary response type (1/0, choice/no choice). Models were iteratively reduced in case of nonsignificant effects to produce more stable models. ANOVA, PCA and logistic regression models were carried out using XLSTAT by Addinsoft, version 2019.2. (XLSTAT, Long Island, NY, USA) [43].
Subsequently, the data were organized into three separate data matrices: X (sensation measures), Y (implicit choice measures) and Z (background data) in order to prepare for multivariate L-shaped Partial Least Square Regression (L-PLSR) analysis, as described by Martens et al. (2001) and applied by Giacalone et al. (2013) [44,45]. The X-matrix consisted of the significant subjective sensations as columns and each snack sample as rows. The Y-matrix consisted of the implicit consumer snack choice data (1/0, choice/no choice), with consumers as columns and snack samples as rows. The Z-matrix consisted of the background data, with consumers as columns and background information as rows. Data matrices X (sensations) and Z (background data) shared no dimensions but were connected via a data matrix Y. L-PLSR was employed to find the latent variables modelling the covariance structure between the three data matrices [46,47]. Prior to L-PLSR analysis, preliminary full cross-validated two-block PLSR analyses using Martens’ uncertainty test were carried out to validate the variance and significance of the different sets of variables [48]. The first preliminary PLSR model was performed with X (sensation measures) and Y (implicit choice) containing the actual snack sample choice observations. This two-block PLSR model was performed to reveal the sensations that were significant for explaining snack choice, using a so-called discriminant Partial Least Square Regression (D-PLSR). The second preliminary two-block PLSR model was performed on X (background data) and Y (implicit choice) to reveal the significant explanatory background variables for snack choice. Thus, only specifically chosen and significant variables were included in the L-PLSR analysis. All multivariate PLSR models (incl. L-PLSR) were carried out using Unscrambler® X, version 10.5.1. (CAMO software, Oslo, Norway).

3. Results

3.1. Snack Choice Differences

Across the actual snack choices, grapes were chosen most frequently, i.e., by 36% of the participants (n = 92). Jelly beans were chosen the least frequently, i.e., by 5% (n = 13). Potato chips and dark chocolate were both chosen by 17% of the participants (n = 43 and 44), whereas nuts and cookies were chosen by 16% (n = 39) and 9% (n = 22), respectively.
One-way ANOVA revealed that consumers’ levels of seven of the thirteen sensations were significantly different for the snack choices: Hunger (p = 0.009), Physical wellbeing (p = 0.030), Desire-to-eat (p = 0.023), Desire-to-snack (p = 0.001), Sweet desire (p < 0.0001), Fatty desire (p < 0.0001), and Salty desire (p < 0.0001). Table 4 shows all the details from all sensation variables with least squares means, standard deviations, p-values, F-values, and Tukey’s pairwise comparisons. Tukey’s (HSD) pairwise comparison revealed significant differences between the specific snack samples. Specifically, level of Hunger (p = 0.029, d = 0.5), Desire-to-eat (p = 0.011, d = 0.6), Desire-to-snack (p = 0.003, d = 0.7), Fatty desire (p < 0.0001, d = 1.1), and Salty desire (p < 0.0001, d = 0.9) were different between consumers choosing grapes compared to potato chips, with medium and large effect sizes [42,49]. Salty desire level was also different between consumers choosing potato chips compared to dark chocolate (p < 0.0001, d = 0.7), and fatty desire level was different between consumers choosing potato chips compared to nuts (p = 0.009, d = 0.5). Sweet desire level was different between consumers choosing potato chips compared to cookies (p = 0.011, d = 1.0), as well as compared to dark chocolate (p < 0.0001, d = 1.1). Importantly, this ANOVA output does not disclose which sensations were explanatory for the specific snack choice. This will be outlined via the binomial logistic regression analysis explained in Section 3.3.

3.2. Patterns of Correlation for Sensations

PCA revealed patterns of correlation for consumers’ subjective sensations. A total of 61.72% of the variance was explained in the first three factors. Factors 1, 2 and 3 explained 27.35%, 24.89% and 9.48% of the variance, respectively. The pattern of correlations of Factor 1 vs. Factor 2 (52.24% of explained variance) and Factor 1 vs. Factor 3 (36.83% of explained variance) is presented in Figure 2 and Figure 3, respectively. Results showed four main directions for subjective sensations. Factor 1 clearly separated appetite-related sensations, and Factor 2 clearly separated more vitality- and energy-related sensations, (Figure 2). Factor 3 was included because it revealed a different pattern than the first two factors, namely that Sweet desire mainly explained the variance for the third factor, (Figure 3). Sweet desire was hence not explained well in either Factor 1 or Factor 2, but rather in Factor 3. Sweet desire was positively correlated with Desire-to-snack (r = 0.37, p < 0.0001), Desire-to-eat (r = 0.27, p < 0.0001), and Sleepy (r = 0.14, p = 0.026).

3.3. Sensations’ Effect on Choice

In order to examine the effect of subjective sensations on choice behavior, and thereby, to link the specific snack choice to its explanatory sensations, we performed a binomial logistic regression analysis. Figure 4 shows the significant effect of sensations on the two large choice groups: grapes (chosen by 36% of the consumers) and potato chips (chosen by 17% of the consumer), by depicting standardized coefficient values (COEF) including 95% confidence intervals (CI).
After iteratively reducing the model to increase its stability, we found that three sensations had a significant negative effect for grape choice: Mental wellbeing (X2 = 3.818, COEF = −0.162, 95% CI [−0.324, 0.00], p = 0.050), Desire-to-snack (X2 = 5.693, COEF = −0.210, 95% CI [−0.382, −0.037], p = 0.017), and Salty desire (X2 = 8.002, COEF = −0.261, 95% CI [−0.441, −0.080], p = 0.005). Hence, sensing Mental wellbeing, Desire-to-snack, or a high Salty desire caused consumers not to choose grapes. Salty desire had the largest effect of the three sensations. No sensations positively drove grape choice. We found that five of the subjective sensations had an effect on potato chips choice. Sensing Overall wellbeing (X2 = 4.350, COEF = 0.312, 95% CI [0.019, 0.604], p = 0.037), Fatty desire (X2 = 11.383, COEF = 0.385, 95% CI [0.161, 0.609], p = 0.001), or Salty desire (X2 = 12.394, COEF = 0.389, 95% CI [0.173, 0.606], p < 0.0001) showed a positive effect on potato chips choice. On the other hand, sensing Physical wellbeing (X2 = 5.823, COEF = −0.358, 95% CI [−0.649, −0.067], p = 0.016) or Sweet desire (X2 = 13.288, COEF = −0.460, 95% CI [−0.708, −0.213], p < 0.0001) had a negative effect on potato chips choice. Hence, sensing Physical wellbeing or a high Sweet desire caused consumers not to choose potato chips, whereas sensing Overall wellbeing, a high Fatty desire or a high Salty desire caused consumers to choose potato chips. The three desires, i.e., Sweet, Fatty, and Salty, had the largest effects. Table 5 shows the details for all the snack choices as well their significant explanatory sensations with either a positive or a negative effect given by the standardized coefficient value (COEF).

3.4. Overall Structure by L-PLSR

The overall structure and relationship between subjective sensations, background variables and real snack choice were provided by the L-PLSR analysis. The preliminary full cross-validated two-block PLSR models revealed which variables were retained for the L-PLSR. Consequently, significant sensation variables were Salty desire, Fatty desire, and Desire-to-eat. Significant background variables were Health consciousness, gender female and gender male. The remaining non-significant variables were thus omitted from the L-PLSR analysis. Figure 5 shows the correlation loadings plot from L-PLSR, which visually summarizes the systematic covariation from the multiblock analysis. It illustrates the interrelationships of consumer snack choices (Y) explained by both subjective sensations (X) and background information (Z). Interpreting the plot in Figure 5, we see that consumers’ real choices (), as expected, are represented together with the snack samples given the nature of a choice or no-choice design (1/0 binary response). The consumers are thus grouped together for one snack choice. The results further show that both subjective sensations () and background information () bring important knowledge to the overall picture, and thus provide explanations for real snack choices. Factor 1 is explained by Health consciousness, depicted to the left in the plot, as well as desire sensations (Salty, fatty, Desire-to-eat), depicted to the right in the plot. Factor 2 is mainly explained by gender, with Females placed in the upper part of the plot and Males in the lower part. Clustering variables around choice, we observe that the choice of grapes was mainly explained by Health consciousness and gender Female. No sensations positively correlate and explain the choice of grapes. Around the upper left quadrant, we also see a cluster including the three presumed healthy snack options, i.e., grapes, dark chocolate, and nuts. In contrast, we see that the desire sensations and gender Male explain and cluster around the more unhealthy snack options, especially potato chips and partly cookies. Data matrix X explained in total 96% of the variance in the L-PLSR analysis, compared to data matrix Z, with 100% explained variance, and data matrix Y with 1.0% explained variance. This makes sense, considering that both the X- and Z-matrices bring new information to explain the choice and include quantitative variables that show variation amongst consumers, whereas the Y-matrix is a dummy matrix with binary variables that essentially indicates snack choice.

4. Discussion

4.1. Main Effects of Desires on Snack Choice

In relation to our first research question, i.e., which subjective temporal sensations contribute to snack choice, we found that subjective desires contribute the most, both positively and negatively. The results showed that especially consumers’ sensory specific desires (Salty, Fatty, Sweet desire) discriminated between the snack choices and also had the greatest effects in general. The theory behind Sensory Specific Desires (SSD) can be described as a general desire for certain tastes (e.g., sweet, salty, or fatty foods) and an intrinsic motivation to eat a food that contains that characteristic [19,50]. Both Salty and Fatty desires were explanatory for consumers’ choice of potato chips. Making a choice based on desires might be in line with the previously described impulsive decision-making [4,9,12]. For instance, Honkanen et al. (2012) found that impulsive snacking was characterized by the tendency to buy snacks without thinking [13,14]. This could perhaps reflect a behavior driven by an internal desire. Salty and Fatty desires also showed overall very high positive correlation with each other, as well as with Hunger. It is suggested that desires for specific sensory stimuli may be related to hedonic hunger, and function as a new and different dimension of appetite [26,36,51,52]. Hedonic hunger is driven by the need for pleasure rather than for nutrients, which is the case for homeostatic hunger [51]. We believe that in this research study, consumers’ desires (Salty, Fatty desire, and Desire-to-snack) and, thereby, hedonic hunger, drove choices, especially for the pleasurable hedonic snack choices such as potato chips and cookies, adding importance to non-homeostatic factors for actual snack choice.
Yet, Sweet desire seemed to not be related to the other desires, nor to Hunger nor Fullness, but revealed a different pattern than the other desire sensations, as illustrated in Figure 3. Sweet desire, hence, seemed to be disconnected from general appetite-related sensations. Sweet desire was not predictive of any of the presumed sweet snacks either, but instead, had a positive effect on dark chocolate choice and a negative effect on potato chips choice. Harington et al. (2016) suggest that our desire for sweet foods is partially disconnected from our appetite, hypothesizing a “dessert mentality” [53]. This could mean that there is always room for something sweet, regardless of other appetite sensations [26,52]. Harington et al. (2016) found that the desire for sweet was maintained for their whole study period of three hours, after eating two slices of bread, whereas participants’ desire for salty, fatty and savory decreased [53]. This suggests a disconnect of sweet desire from the other desires as well, which is in line with the results in this research study. Thus, including desires when studying food choice and eating behavior in general is highly relevant and provides valuable information. Sensory specific desires were revealed to be important determinants for actual snack choice. However, Sweet desire appeared to have its own agenda and showed different patterns. More detailed knowledge about the disconnect of Sweet desire from appetite and other desires is therefore needed.

4.2. Health Consciousness and Females as Explanatory for Healthy Snack Choice

Multivariate analyses revealed that not only consumers’ subjective sensation state or only consumers’ background information data explained snack choice. Both subjective sensations and background data brought important knowledge to the overall picture. This underlines that we live in a multifaceted world, and that the processes around food choice are complex [3,4]. For this snack study, and in relation to our second research question, Health consciousness was a driving factor for healthy choices, especially grapes. The three presumed healthy snack options in this study, i.e., grapes, nuts and dark chocolate, were mainly explained by gender Female and Health consciousness, whereas male consumers more frequently chose the unhealthy snacks. These results add to plentiful research showing that gender and health consciousness are linked to healthy eating, and that health consciousness itself is a substantial driver of healthy eating in general [5,14,41,54,55,56,57]. Consumers with high health consciousness tend to engage in behavior that improves their health as, well as follow health and dietary recommendations [41,58,59,60]. Additionally, according to Honkanen et al. (2012), reflective or deliberate snacking behavior (as opposed to impulsive) was characterized by an attitude towards unhealthy snacking, and furthermore strengthened by a strong food related self-control [14]. Her and Seo (2017) investigated the role of health-consciousness on the intention to order desserts, finding that highly health-conscious consumers reliably showed low intention [54]. Hartman et al. (2012) examined the associations between snack frequency and healthy versus unhealthy food choices in order to identify underlying patterns. They found that high frequency snacking happened in the context of both healthy and unhealthy lifestyle patterns. However, the unhealthy snacking consumer segments were less health conscious than the healthy snacking segments [61]. Moreover, they found that women chose fruit snacks and made healthier choices than men, while men more often chose unhealthy snacks such as sweets and savories [61]. Amongst adolescent consumers, Mielby et al. (2012), also found that boys chose baked savory and sweet snacks, whereas girls chose fruit snacks such as grapes more often [62]. Consumers in this study were between 18 and 30 years old and revealed similar tendencies. This raises the question of why female consumers choose more healthy snacks in general. Weijzen et al. (2009) found that the aforementioned discrepancy between intention and actual choice was less evident amongst female participants, and that females had a healthy snacking habit as well as strong self-control [5]. Hartman et al. (2012) found that the consumer segment with high frequency healthy snacking habits consisted mainly of females reporting high levels of health consciousness, as well as regular breakfast consumption and low consumption of alcohol in general [61]. Leaning on these findings, we might reason that health-conscious and high self-control females, more often than male consumers, do not follow their immediate desires, but tend to follow a more reflective choice pattern when choosing snacks. The results from this study could also be reflected in this reasoning and support the consistency between health attitudes, intention and actual choice behavior.

4.3. Differences in Subjective Wellbeing Sensations

For the included wellbeing sensations, i.e., Overall, Mental and Physical wellbeing, we observed that all three parameters were important and showed significant effects on snack choice, but in different ways. Overall, wellbeing and Physical wellbeing revealed opposite effects for potato chips choice, showing that high Overall wellbeing positively drove that choice, whereas high Physical wellbeing caused consumers not to choose potato chips. Furthermore, consumers that rated high Mental wellbeing showed a negative effect on grape choice. So, even though the three wellbeing sensations showed high positive correlations to each other independently of snack choice (Figure 2), it seems that consumers do distinguish (perhaps subconsciously) between these three sensations before choosing a snack, and that the three wellbeing sensations each bring important knowledge to the overall picture. Food-related wellbeing has gained a lot of interest in recent years [26,27,32,34,36,37,63], but its multifaceted nature proves very difficult to define and conceptualize [28,64,65]. Consequently, there is no one universal way of defining or measuring wellbeing; King et al. (2015) suggest that it includes five dimensions, namely physical, emotional, social, intellectual and spiritual, as encompassed in their developed WellSense Profile™ [34]. Wellbeing in food-related contexts has shown strong associations with physical health when eliciting consumer associations with wellbeing [31,33]. Andersen and Hyldig (2015) also found qualitative evidence that physical wellbeing functions as an important element in food satisfaction [35]. Food-related wellbeing is furthermore recognized as a comprehensive holistic concept that goes beyond just physical health [27,28], and is additionally associated with sensations such as calmness, happiness, satisfaction, joy and general positive emotions [26,33,66]. Psychological wellbeing was also found to be explanatory for food satisfaction in a study by Andersen et al. (2017) [38].
Regular healthy eating results in better wellbeing. Foods that are recognized as positive for wellbeing include fruits, vegetables, grains and dairy products, whereas foods high in fat, salt and sugar are perceived to be negative for one’s wellbeing [33,67]. This applies to the effect of food on wellbeing, but the direct effect of subjective wellbeing for healthy food choices is less clear. Gardner et al. (2014) found that mood influences the choice between healthy and indulgent foods, with positive mood leading to a preference for healthy foods and negative mood leading to a preference for indulgent foods [68]. Somewhat controversially, we found that Overall wellbeing had a positive effect for the presumed unhealthy potato chips snack choice, a snack which is high in salt and fat. Could this mean that if you feel general overall wellbeing, then you choose your snacks based on your desires and not your health attitudes? These findings add to the significance, importance and applicability of evaluating subjective wellbeing in consumer research. Exploring food-related wellbeing can help to clarify some of the factors influencing our food choices, and perhaps try to unravel the complex implications for choice, intake and eating behaviors overall [4,28].

4.4. Sleepiness Versus Energy

The vitality- and energy-related sensations such as Sleepy, Energized and Concentration did not have any significant effects on the actual snack choice behavior in this study. Thus, in this study and in relation to the first question, these subjective sensations were not important or contributed to actual snack choice. Nonetheless, independently of snack choice measures, we observed that these sensations functioned almost as opposites in consumer perceptions (Figure 2), with Sleepy being significantly negatively correlated to Energized and Concentration. In a previous study done by the authors [36], we observed that sleepiness and energized displayed almost opposite dynamic curves after the consumption of a breakfast meal, with an immediate decrease in sleepiness and an immediate increase in energized, suggesting that feeling energized and sleepy function as two opposites of one pole [36]. Energy level was demonstrated to be an important factor in driving consumers’ food satisfaction in a study by Andersen et al. (2017). They did not measure sleepiness, but found energy to be negatively correlated to nausea [38]. Energy sensations have been found to include both positive sensations such as concentration and feeling alert, but also more negative sensations such as a lack of focus, heaviness and sleepiness [26]. Boelsma et al. (2010) applied and concluded a measure of overall postprandial wellness, which included both pleasantness of sleepiness, physical energy, mental alertness, hunger and fullness. They thus explained these variables in a common underlying factor [30]. With regard to energy and sleepiness, research suggests a term called “post-lunch sleepiness” or a “post-lunch dip” in energy. It occurs, accordingly, after having lunch, and is a distinctive urge for sleep in the early afternoon [26,69,70,71]. According to Monk (2005), this “post-lunch-dip” in energy can occur even without having eaten lunch, or without being aware of the time of day [71]. In the present study, we did not serve any food to the participants prior to the subjective sensation measurements, nor did we allocate the data collection to a specific time of day. Taking the above into consideration, researching consumers’ energy levels in relation to food choices has several applications, including whether eating food influences the sensation of, e.g., sleepiness or energy, or whether these vitality- and energy-related sensations influence the next meal intake or even longer-term food choices. In this study, these sensations did not affect snack choice. Perhaps, if the study had extended data collection to the evening, these sensations would have shown significant effects on snack choice. The applications require more research for elaboration and new knowledge creation.

4.5. Research Contribution and Future Perspectives

The present results contribute to the multicomplex area of understanding determinants of food choices and eating behaviors. With this study, we add new knowledge about effects of subjective temporal sensations on actual snack choice. The contribution of the different sensations to snack choice is imperative, and helps to unravel and understand some aspects of snack eating behavior.
While this research study focused on snack choice, future studies should extend the investigation of the effect of subjective sensations on main meals and general food intake. The importance of subjective sensations in relation to predictions of longer-term food intake is, to date, unstudied. Therefore, additional and longer-term research is necessary to support our findings and to gain more in-depth knowledge about the importance of subjective sensations and their relevance to food choice behavior over time. Furthermore, researching subjective sensations with other and more diverse approaches such as including more implicit and indirect measures for both homeostatic and non-homeostatic sensations could add to the validation and application of predicting food choices. This entails interdisciplinary research, involving research areas such as psychology, neuroscience, human nutrition, biology and physiology. This could provide further insights into (over) eating, and have an impact in broader contexts with implications for public health issues and obesity.

4.6. Limitations

In the present study, the six snacks were divided into categories each representing a healthy and unhealthy version of a sensory taste profile, being sweet, salty, or fatty. It is important to consider that these categories might be a matter of interpretation, and that consumers might distinguish other sensory profiles than those that we categorized. For instance, we saw that Sweet desire showed an effect on dark chocolate, and that Fatty desire showed an effect on potato chips. Cookies represented an overall higher mean Sweet desire amongst participants than did the Fatty desire. Nuts represented an overall higher mean Salty desire than Fatty desire. These sensations do not necessarily describe the snacks by sensory attributes, but might be an indicator as to how consumers perceive them, given that desires were explanatory for specific snack choices. Furthermore, some consumers consider dark chocolates or nuts as healthy or unhealthy, or perhaps regard potato chips as overlapping in both fatty and salty. Other examples of researchers categorizing choices in consumer food studies include Muñoz-Vilches et al. (2019) [72], who used the concepts of vice and virtue products, characterized into hedonic or utilitarian categories using crisps and green smoothies as choice options. Her and Seo (2017) [54] categorized cake, cookie and ice cream as unhealthy as their excessive caloric value can lead to aversive health consequences. Hence, we see diverse categorizations, and for this reason, one should avoid making firm interpretations based only on category definitions.
Another aspect to consider when interpreting the results is the possibility that the choice outcome could be affected by the consumer’s own answers, e.g., “I answered high salty desire, therefore I should choose potato chips”. Moreover, the consumer group that chose jelly beans was the smallest of all, comprising only 5%. Therefore, any strong conclusions should not be drawn as to the effects for the choice of jelly beans. Furthermore, it is important to consider the nature and composition of the participants. All participants were relatively young adults associated with the university, whose choice behavior may differ from that of the general population. Therefore, more confirmatory studies are necessary with differing population groups in order to generalize the results from this study.

5. Conclusions

This research study provided new knowledge about the effects of subjective sensations on actual snack choice. We explored subjective sensations related to appetite sensations, such as hunger and fullness, but also focused on non-homeostatic subjective sensations such as desires, wellbeing and energy sensations. Sweet desire seemed to be a unique sensation not related to appetite nor other desire sensations. The results demonstrated that especially sensory specific desire sensations (Salty, Fatty and Sweet desire) affected, either positively or negatively, snack choice. Furthermore, wellbeing sensations (Overall, Mental, and Physical wellbeing) also showed significant effects on snack choice, each in different ways, with Physical wellbeing contributing positively to the choice of nuts and negatively to the choice of potato chips. The vitality- and energy-related sensations such as Sleepy, Energized and Concentration did not affect snack choice. Additionally, we found that both subjective temporal sensations and background factors brought important knowledge to the overall picture. Gender and Health consciousness drove snack choice as well, with health-conscious females choosing the presumed healthy snacks and males choosing the presumed unhealthy snacks.
Importantly, this study indicates the applicability of subjective sensations in food choice research that focus on diverse determinants. Notably, including desires and wellbeing sensations elucidates important knowledge. The contribution of the different sensations to food choice is thus imperative, and helps to unravel and understand some aspects of snack eating behavior. This could provide further insights into (over)eating, and have implications for public health issues such as unhealthy snacking and obesity. Future perspectives include interdisciplinary research involving implicit approaches to predict choice behavior over time.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.D., B.V.A., and D.V.B.; methodology, M.D. and B.V.A.; software, M.D.; validation, M.D., B.V.A., and N.A.; formal analysis, M.D., B.V.A., and N.A.; investigation, M.D.; obtaining ethics, M.D., M.P., resources, M.D., M.P.; data curation, M.D.; writing—original draft preparation, M.D.; writing—review and editing, M.D., B.V.A., N.A., M.P., and D.V.B.; visualization, M.D.; supervision, B.V.A., M.P., and D.V.B.; project administration, M.D., B.V.A, and D.V.B.; funding acquisition, B.V.A., and D.V.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

We thank Andreas Heinz Nielsen and members of the Sensory Laboratory at University of Otago (Jimmy Cahayadi, Hannah Browne, Hazel Potterton) for practical help with conducting of the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ajzen, I. From Intention to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior. In Action Control: From Cognition to Behaviour; Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  2. Furst, T.; Connors, M.; Bisogni, C.A.; Sobal, J.; Falk, L.W. Food Choice: A Conceptual Model of the Process. Appetite 1996, 26, 247–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Gorton, M.; Barjolle, D. Theories of Food Choice. In Food Consumer Science; Barjolle, D., Gorton, M., Đorđević, J.M., Stojanović, Ž., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2013; pp. 15–26. [Google Scholar]
  4. Köster, E.P. Diversity in the Determinants of Food Choice: A Psychological Perspective. Food Qual. Prefer. 2009, 20, 70–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Weijzen, P.L.G.; de Graaf, C.; Dijksterhuis, G.B. Predictors of the Consistency between Healthy Snack Choice Intentions and Actual Behaviour. Food Qual. Prefer. 2009, 20, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Butland, B.; Jebb, S.; Kopelman, P.; McPherson, K.; Thomas, S.; Mardell, J.; Parry, V. Foresight–Tackling Obesities: Future Choices—Project Report; Government Office for Science: London, UK, 2007.
  7. World Health Organization (WHO). European Food and Nutrition Action Plan 2015–2020; WHO Regional Office for Europe: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  8. Conner, M.; Armitage, C. Social Psychological Models of Food Choice. In The Psychology of Food Choice; Shepherd, R., Raats, M., Eds.; Open University Press: Wallingford, UK, 2006; pp. 41–58. [Google Scholar]
  9. Kahneman, D. A Perspective on Judgment and Choice: Mapping Bounded Rationality. Am. Psychol. 2003, 58, 697–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Fila, S.; Smith, C. Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior to Healthy Eating Behaviors in Urban Native American Youth. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2006, 3, 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Cahayadi, J.; Geng, X.; Mirosa, M.; Peng, M. Expectancy versus Experience—Comparing Portion-Size-Effect during Pre-Meal Planning and Actual Intake. Appetite 2019, 135, 108–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Farrar, Straus and Giroux: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  13. Wansink, B.; Sobal, J. Mindless Eating: The 200 Daily Food Decisions We Overlook. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 106–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Honkanen, P.; Olsen, S.O.; Verplanken, B.; Tuu, H.H. Reflective and Impulsive Influences on Unhealthy Snacking. The Moderating Effects of Food Related Self-Control. Appetite 2012, 58, 616–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Yeomans, M.R. Psychobiological Mechanisms in Food Choice. In Consumer-lLd Food Product Development; MacFie, H., Ed.; Woodhead Publishing: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2007; pp. 81–107. [Google Scholar]
  16. Rogers, P.J.; Blundell, J.E. Psychobiological Bases of Food Choice. BNF Nutr. Bull. 1990, 15, 31–40. [Google Scholar]
  17. Rogers, P.J.; Hardman, C.A. Food Reward. What It Is and How to Measure It. Appetite 2015, 90, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Møller, P. Satisfaction, Satiation and Food Behaviour. Curr. Opin. Food Sci. 2015, 3, 59–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Olsen, A.; Ritz, C.; Hartvig, D.L.; Møller, P. Comparison of Sensory Specific Satiety and Sensory Specific Desires to Eat in Children and Adults. Appetite 2011, 57, 6–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Yeomans, M.R. Measuring Appetite and Food Intake. In Methods in Consumer Research. Alternative Approaches and Special Applications; Ares, G., Varela, P., Eds.; Woodhead Publishing: Sawston, UK, 2018; Volume 2, pp. 119–149. [Google Scholar]
  21. Blundell, J.; Bellisle, F. Satiation, Satiety and the Control of Food Intake: Theory and Practice; Blundell, J., France, B., Eds.; Woodhead Publishing Limited: Cambridge, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  22. Merriam-Webster. Sensation. Available online: https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/sensation (accessed on 30 October 2019).
  23. Craig, A.D. How Do You Feel? Interoception: The Sense of the Physiological Condition of the Body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 655–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Simmons, W.K.; DeVille, D.C. Interoceptive Contributions to Healthy Eating and Obesity. Psychology 2017, 17, 106–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Stevenson, R.J.; Mahmut, M.; Rooney, K. Individual Differences in the Interoceptive States of Hunger, Fullness and Thirst. Appetite 2015, 95, 44–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Duerlund, M.; Andersen, B.V.; Grønbeck, M.S.; Byrne, D.V. Consumer Reflections on Post-Ingestive Sensations. A Qualitative Approach by Means of Focus Group Interviews. Appetite 2019, 142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Meiselman, H.L. Quality of Life, Well-Being and Wellness: Measuring Subjective Health for Foods and Other Products. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 54, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. McMahon, A.-T.; Williams, P.; Tapsell, L. Reviewing the Meanings of Wellbeing and Wellness and Their Implications for Food Choice. Public Health 2010, 130, 282–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Karalus, M.; Vickers, Z. Satiation and Satiety Sensations Produced by Eating Oatmeal vs. Oranges. A Comparison of Different Scales. Appetite 2016, 99, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Boelsma, E.; Brink, E.J.; Stafleu, A.; Hendriks, H.F.J. Measures of Postprandial Wellness after Single Intake of Two Protein-Carbohydrate Meals. Appetite 2010, 54, 456–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ares, G.; De Saldamando, L.; Giménez, A.; Deliza, R. Food and Wellbeing. Towards a Consumer-Based Approach. Appetite 2014, 74, 61–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Ares, G.; Giménez, A.; Vidal, L.; Zhou, Y.; Krystallis, A.; Tsalis, G.; Symoneaux, R.; Cunha, L.M.; de Moura, A.P.; Claret, A.; et al. Do We All Perceive Food-Related Wellbeing in the Same Way? Results from an Exploratory Cross-Cultural Study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 52, 62–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ares, G.; de Saldamando, L.; Giménez, A.; Claret, A.; Cunha, L.M.; Guerrero, L.; de Moura, A.P.; Oliveira, D.C.R.; Symoneaux, R.; Deliza, R. Consumers’ Associations with Wellbeing in a Food-Related Context: A Cross-Cultural Study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 40, 304–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. King, S.C.; Snow, J.; Meiselman, H.L.; Sainsbury, J.; Carr, B.T.; McCafferty, D.; Serrano, D.; Gillette, M.; Millard, L.; Li, Q. Development of a Questionnaire to Measure Consumer Wellness Associated with Foods: The WellSense ProfileTM. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 82–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Andersen, B.V.; Hyldig, G. Food Satisfaction: Integrating Feelings before, during and after Food Intake. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 43, 126–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Duerlund, M.; Andersen, B.V.; Byrne, D.V. Dynamic Changes in Post-Ingestive Sensations after Consumption of a Breakfast Meal High in Protein or Carbohydrate. Foods 2019, 8, 413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Andersen, B.V.; Byrne, D.V.; Bredie, W.L.P.; Møller, P. Cayenne Pepper in a Meal: Effect of Oral Heat on Feelings of Appetite, Sensory Specific Desires and Well-Being. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 60, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Andersen, B.V.; Mielby, L.H.; Viemose, I.; Bredie, W.L.P.; Hyldig, G. Integration of the Sensory Experience and Post-Ingestive Measures for Understanding Food Satisfaction. A Case Study on Sucrose Replacement by Stevia Rebaudiana and Addition of Beta Glucan in Fruit Drinks. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 58, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Squires, L.; Bettina Cornwell, T.; Juric, B. Level of Market Development and Intensity of Organic Food Consumption: Cross-cultural Study of Danish and New Zealand Consumers. J. Consum. Mark. 2011, 18, 392–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Tarkiainen, A.; Sundqvist, S. Subjective Norms, Attitudes and Intentions of Finnish Consumers in Buying Organic Food. Br. Food J. 2005, 107, 808–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Shin, J.; Mattila, A.S. When Organic Food Choices Shape Subsequent Food Choices: The Interplay of Gender and Health Consciousness. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2019, 76, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  43. Addinsoft. XLSTAT Statistical and Data Analysis Solution; Addinsoft: Long Island, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  44. Martens, H.; Anderssen, E.; Flatberg, A.; Gidskehaug, L.H.; Høy, M.; Westad, F.; Thybo, A.; Martens, M. Regression of a Data Matrix on Descriptors of Both Its Rows and of Its Columns via Latent Variables: L-PLSR. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 2005, 48, 103–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Giacalone, D.; Bredie, W.L.P.; Frøst, M.B. “All-In-One Test”(AI1): A Rapid and Easily Applicable Approach to Consumer Product Testing. Food Qual. Prefer. 2013, 27, 108–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Sæbø, S.; Almøy, T.; Flatberg, A.; Aastveit, A.H.; Martens, H. LPLS-Regression: A Method for Prediction and Classification under the Influence of Background Information on Predictor Variables. Chemom. Intell. Lab. Syst. 2008, 91, 121–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Martens, H.; Martens, M. Multivariate Analysis of Quality. An Introduction; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  48. Martens, H.; Martens, M. Modified Jack-Knife Estimation of Parameter Uncertainty in Bilinear Modelling by Partial Least Squares Regression (PLSR). Food Qual. Prefer. 2000, 11, 5–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Sawilowsky, S.S. New Effect Size Rules of Thumb. J. Mod. Appl. Stat. Methods 2009, 8, 597–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mela, D.J. Eating for Pleasure or Just Wanting to Eat? Reconsidering Sensory Hedonic Responses as a Driver of Obesity. Appetite 2006, 47, 10–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Lowe, M.R.; Butryn, M.L. Hedonic Hunger: A New Dimension of Appetite? Physiol. Behav. 2007, 91, 432–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Murray, M.; Vickers, Z. Consumer Views of Hunger and Fullness. A Qualitative Approach. Appetite 2009, 53, 174–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Harington, K.; Smeele, R.; Van Loon, F.; Yuan, J.; Haszard, J.J.; Drewer, A.; Venn, B.J. Desire for Sweet Taste Unchanged After Eating: Evidence of a Dessert Mentality? J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 2016, 35, 581–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Her, E.S.; Seo, S. Health Halo Effects in Sequential Food Consumption: The Moderating Roles of Health-Consciousness and Attribute Framing. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2017, 62, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Apaolaza, V.; Hartmann, P.; D’Souza, C.; López, C.M. Eat Organic—Feel Good? The Relationship between Organic Food Consumption, Health Concern and Subjective Wellbeing. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 63, 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Jeong, E.H.; Shawn Jang, S.C. Healthy Menu Promotions: A Match between Dining Value and Temporal Distance. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2015, 45, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Lee, K.; Conklin, M.; Cranage, D.A.; Lee, S. The Role of Perceived Corporate Social Responsibility on Providing Healthful Foods and Nutrition Information with Health-Consciousness as a Moderator. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2014, 37, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kraft, F.B.; Goodell, P. Identifying the Health Conscious Consumer. J. Health Care Mark. 1993, 13, 18–25. [Google Scholar]
  59. Wardle, J.; Steptoe, A. Socioeconomic Differences in Attitudes and Beliefs about Healthy Lifestyles. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2003, 57, 440–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Zizza, C.A. Healthy Snacking Recommendations: One Size Does Not Fit All. Physiol. Behav. 2014, 134, 32–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hartmann, C.; Siegrist, M.; Van Der Horst, K. Snack Frequency: Associations with Healthy and Unhealthy Food Choices. Public Health Nutr. 2013, 16, 1487–1496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Mielby, L.H.; Edelenbos, M.; Thybo, A.K. Comparison of Rating, Best-Worst Scaling, and Adolescents’ Real Choices of Snacks. Food Qual. Prefer. 2012, 25, 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ares, G.; Giménez, A.; Deliza, R. Methodological Approaches for Measuring Consumer-Perceived Well-Being in a Food-Related Context. In Methods in Consumer Research. Alternative Approaches and Special Applications; Ares, G., Varela, P., Eds.; Woodhead Publishing: Sawston, UK, 2018; Volume 2, pp. 183–200. [Google Scholar]
  64. Dodge, R.; Daly, A.P.; Huyton, J.; Sanders, L.D. The Challenge of Defining Wellbeing. Int. J. Wellbeing 2012, 2, 222–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Miller, G.; Foster, L.T. Critical Synthesis of Wellness Literature; University of Victoria, Faculty of Human and Social Development & Department of Geography: Victoria, BC, Canada, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  66. Diener, E.; Ryan, K. Subjective Well-Being: A General Overview. S. Afr. J. Phychol. 2009, 39, 391–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Spence, C. Comfort Food: A Review. Int. J. Gastron. Food Sci. 2017, 9, 105–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Gardner, M.P.; Wansink, B.; Kim, J.; Park, S.B. Better Moods for Better Eating?: How Mood Influences Food Choice. J. Consum. Psychol. 2014, 24, 320–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Reyner, L.A.; Wells, S.J.; Mortlock, V.; Horne, J.A. “Post-Lunch” Sleepiness during Prolonged, Monotonous Driving–Effects of Meal Size. Physiol. Behav. 2012, 105, 1088–1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Smit, H.J.; Finnegan, Y.E.; Rogers, P.J. Post-Lunch Dip? Get out and Stay Out! Appetite 2006, 47, 281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Monk, T.H. The Post-Lunch Dip in Performance. Clin. Sports Med. 2005, 24, 15–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Muñoz-Vilches, N.C.; van Trijp, H.C.M.; Piqueras-Fiszman, B. The Impact of Instructed Mental Simulation on Wanting and Choice between Vice and Virtue Food Products. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 73, 182–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Picture of the six snacks in a randomized display.
Figure 1. Picture of the six snacks in a randomized display.
Foods 09 00336 g001
Figure 2. Patterns of correlation for all sensation variables from Principal Component Analysis (PCA) displaying Factor 1 (27.35% explained variance) vs. Factor 2 (24.89% explained variance).
Figure 2. Patterns of correlation for all sensation variables from Principal Component Analysis (PCA) displaying Factor 1 (27.35% explained variance) vs. Factor 2 (24.89% explained variance).
Foods 09 00336 g002
Figure 3. Patterns of correlations for all sensation variables from Principal Component Analysis (PCA) displaying Factor 1 (27.35% explained variance) vs. Factor 3 (9.48% explained variance).
Figure 3. Patterns of correlations for all sensation variables from Principal Component Analysis (PCA) displaying Factor 1 (27.35% explained variance) vs. Factor 3 (9.48% explained variance).
Foods 09 00336 g003
Figure 4. Significant explanatory sensations for (a) grapes choice and (b) potato chips choice depicting standardized coefficient values including 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 4. Significant explanatory sensations for (a) grapes choice and (b) potato chips choice depicting standardized coefficient values including 95% confidence intervals.
Foods 09 00336 g004
Figure 5. Correlation loadings from L-PLS regression analysis showing consumers’ real choice () for the six snacks (), explained by both subjective sensations () and background variables (). The plot displays Factor 1 (X explained variance: 88%, Y: 0.6%, Z: 75%) vs. Factor 2 (X explained variance: 8%, Y: 0.4%, Z: 25%). Variables not explanatory for choice were omitted through preliminary full cross-validated two-block PLS regression analyses.
Figure 5. Correlation loadings from L-PLS regression analysis showing consumers’ real choice () for the six snacks (), explained by both subjective sensations () and background variables (). The plot displays Factor 1 (X explained variance: 88%, Y: 0.6%, Z: 75%) vs. Factor 2 (X explained variance: 8%, Y: 0.4%, Z: 25%). Variables not explanatory for choice were omitted through preliminary full cross-validated two-block PLS regression analyses.
Foods 09 00336 g005
Table 1. Participant characteristics.
Table 1. Participant characteristics.
Characteristics
ntotal
Male/female
Age (years)
Weight (kg)
Height (cm)
BMI 1 (kg/m2)
253
98/155
20.5 ± 2.8 (18−30) *
71.0 ± 15.2 (45−125) *
172.0 ± 9.6 (150−195) *
23.9 ± 4.3 (15.9−45.9) *
* Mean ± standard deviation (range); 1 BMI = body mass index.
Table 2. Questionnaire variables.
Table 2. Questionnaire variables.
Background InformationSensation Variables
Gender
Age
Height
Weight
Time since last intake
Health consciousness
Energy
Concentration
Sleepiness
Fullness
Hunger
Overall wellbeing
Physical wellbeing
Mental wellbeing
Desire-to-eat
Desire-to-snack
Sweet desire
Salty desire
Fatty desire
Table 3. The six snacks used in the study divided into categories.
Table 3. The six snacks used in the study divided into categories.
-SweetSaltyFatty
HealthyGrapesNutsDark chocolate
UnhealthyJelly beansPotato chipsCookies
Table 4. Least squares means ± standard deviations (n = 253) from analysis of variance (ANOVA).
Table 4. Least squares means ± standard deviations (n = 253) from analysis of variance (ANOVA).
p-ValueFCookiesDark ChocolatePotato ChipsNutsJelly BeansGrapes
Energized0.790 (ns)0.484.69 ± 2.25.46 ± 2.25.11 ± 2.25.22 ± 1.94.85 ± 1.95.25 ± 2.2
Concentration0.748 (ns)0.545.44 ± 2.15.71 ± 2.05.30 ± 2.25.35 ± 1.96.05 ± 2.35.27 ± 2.2
Sleepy0.674 (ns)0.635.73 ± 2.64.90 ± 2.74.63 ± 2.64.53 ± 2.74.89 ± 2.94.92 ± 2.9
Fullness0.062 (ns)2.134.69 ± 2.34.89 ± 2.63.87 ± 2.73.99 ± 2.66.37 ± 2.94.56 ± 2.9
Hunger0.0093.144.66 ± 2.3 ab4.49 ± 2.6 ab5.96 ± 2.7 b5.14 ± 2.9 ab2.98 ± 2.6 a4.35 ± 3.2 a
Overall Wellbeing0.265 (ns)1.306.15 ± 2.16.44 ± 1.86.33 ± 2.16.34 ± 1.95.27 ± 2.05.81 ± 2.1
Physical wellbeing0.0302.515.96 ± 2.0 ab6.18 ± 2.0 ab5.65 ± 2.3 ab6.86 ± 2.0 b5.22 ± 1.7 a5.59 ± 2.2 ab
Mental wellbeing0.163 (ns)1.596.42 ± 1.56.25 ± 2.05.95 ± 2.16.26 ± 2.35.05 ± 2.45.52 ± 2.5
Desire to eat0.0232.656.10 ± 2.5 ab5.57 ± 2.4 ab6.58 ± 2.6b5.66 ± 2.8 ab4.92 ± 3.2 ab4.80 ± 3.2 a
Desire to snack0.0014.086.56 ± 2.2 b5.33 ± 2.5 ab6.31 ± 2.6 b5.17 ± 2.7 ab5.58 ± 3.1 ab4.44 ± 2.9 a
Sweet desire<0.00016.035.50 ± 2.4 bc5.72 ± 2.7 c3.16 ± 2.1 a3.64 ± 2.3 ab4.96 ± 3.4 abc3.94 ± 2.9 ab
Fatty desire<0.00014.864.41 ± 2.1 ab3.57 ± 2.7 ab6.02 ± 2.8 b4.62 ± 2.6 a3.07 ± 2.6 ab3.21 ± 2.5 a
Salty desire<0.00018.023.89 ± 2.5 ab3.39 ± 2.3 a4.98 ± 2.5 b3.04 ± 2.5 ab3.67 ± 3.2 a2.75 ± 2.7 a
Means with different superscript (a,b,c) within a row differ significantly (Tukey p < 0.05); Degrees of freedom (5, 247); ns = no significant difference; Data was collected on a continuous visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from 0 to 10.
Table 5. The effect of subjective sensations on each snack choice including significant p-values and positive/negative coefficient values (COEF) from binomial logistic regression models.
Table 5. The effect of subjective sensations on each snack choice including significant p-values and positive/negative coefficient values (COEF) from binomial logistic regression models.
-GrapesJelly BeansNutsPotato ChipsDark ChocolateCookies
-Pr > X2COEFPr > X2COEFPr > X2COEFPr > X2COEFPr > X2COEFPr > X2COEF
Energized------------
Concentration--0.038 *0.371--------
Sleepy------------
Fullness------------
Hunger--0.005 **-0.604--------
Overall Wellbeing--0.012 *−0.459--0.037 *0.312----
Physical wellbeing----0.003 **0.3120.016 *−0.358----
Mental wellbeing0.050 *−0.162----------
Desire to eat------------
Desire to snack0.017 *−0.2100.019 *−0.440------0.022 *0.282
Sweet desire------0.000 ***−0.4600.000 ***0.347--
Salty desire0.005 **−0.261----0.000 ***0.389----
Fatty desire------0.001 ***0.385----
The ‘−’ denotes non-significant variables removed from the model to produce a more stable model; COEF = standardized coefficient value: negative or positive effect on snack choice; * p ≤ 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.0001; X2 = Chi-square.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Duerlund, M.; Andersen, B.V.; Alexi, N.; Peng, M.; Byrne, D.V. Subjective Sensations related to Food as Determinants of Snack Choice. Foods 2020, 9, 336. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9030336

AMA Style

Duerlund M, Andersen BV, Alexi N, Peng M, Byrne DV. Subjective Sensations related to Food as Determinants of Snack Choice. Foods. 2020; 9(3):336. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9030336

Chicago/Turabian Style

Duerlund, Mette, Barbara Vad Andersen, Niki Alexi, Mei Peng, and Derek Victor Byrne. 2020. "Subjective Sensations related to Food as Determinants of Snack Choice" Foods 9, no. 3: 336. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9030336

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop