Next Article in Journal
A Technology-Driven Assistive Learning Tool and Framework for Personalized Dyscalculia Interventions
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Social Capital on Depression in University Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Arithmetic Proficiency Across Adulthood: Cognitive and Subjective Influences

1
Department of Neurology, Medical University Innsbruck, 6020 Innsbruck, Austria
2
Cognitive and Educational Sciences Lab (CESLab), Faculty of Education, Free University of Bolzano-Bozen, 39042 Bressanone-Brixen, Italy
3
Centro de Investigación en Neuropsicologia y Neurociencias Cognitivas (CINPSI Neurocog), Universidad Católica del Maule, Talca 3605, Chile
4
Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, 38068 Rovereto, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15(5), 84; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050084
Submission received: 31 March 2025 / Revised: 3 May 2025 / Accepted: 5 May 2025 / Published: 15 May 2025

Abstract

:
Arithmetic competence is crucial for navigating modern society and maintaining independence. It relies on domain-general and domain-specific cognitive skills, as well as subjective factors. Given its importance, understanding how these factors shape adult arithmetic proficiency is essential. This study investigated demographic, cognitive, and subjective influences on various arithmetic skills throughout adulthood, including both younger and older individuals. In total, 134 adults aged 20–68 completed computerized tasks assessing simple calculations, exact and approximate complex calculations, and arithmetic principles, alongside neuropsychological testing and self-ratings on math anxiety, math self-concept, attitudes toward mathematics, and the frequency of engagement with numbers. The results indicate that accuracy varied by task, with approximate calculations being the most challenging. Self-ratings showed low math anxiety but moderate-to-high math self-concept, positive attitudes, and moderate engagement with numbers. Age correlated only with arithmetic principles; however, interference inhibition and engagement with numbers, not age, best predicted performance. Executive functions correlated solely with approximate calculations and arithmetic principles, while subjective measures were related to all arithmetic tasks. The regression analyses indicate strong interrelationships, particularly among calculation tasks. The findings highlight the multifaceted nature of arithmetic competence and suggest it remains stable in adulthood, with age-related declines only evident in arithmetic principles, likely due to declining executive functions.

1. Introduction

The ability to understand and work with numbers is fundamental at all ages and in many aspects of our daily lives, such as adjusting recipes or paying at the grocery store. Arithmetic performance is closely related to a range of cognitive abilities that include both domain-general functions and domain-specific skills (Knops et al., 2017; LeFevre et al., 2010). Three basic types of knowledge have been identified as crucial for arithmetic (Delazer, 2003). Arithmetic fact knowledge allows for the quick recall of solutions to simple calculations directly from memory (e.g., “9 × 4 = 36”). Procedural knowledge involves applying computational procedures and algorithms to solve problems, primarily for familiar tasks (e.g., knowing how to multiply “36 × 58”). Conceptual knowledge fosters a deeper understanding of mathematical principles and numerical relationships, such as grasping the inverse relationship between addition and subtraction (Delazer, 2003).
Arithmetic competence relies heavily on various domain-general cognitive functions, including attention (LeFevre et al., 2010), language (Chow & Ekholm, 2019), spatial skills (Hawes et al., 2022), and executive functions (Bull & Lee, 2014; Cragg et al., 2017). Among these executive functions, working memory has been found to be strongly related to arithmetic performance, whether in single-digit or multi-digit arithmetic (Chen & Bailey, 2021; Raghubar et al., 2010). Set-shifting is thought to be essential for enabling individuals to switch between different operations, such as solving addition and multiplication problems alternately (Zamarian et al., 2007b), or between different problem-solving strategies (Bull & Lee, 2014; Cragg et al., 2017). Inhibition is considered to play a significant role in mental arithmetic, although the evidence regarding its effect is mixed (Bull & Lee, 2014; Gilmore et al., 2015). Inhibition appears to influence conceptual knowledge in adults but procedural competence in children, likely due to the different mechanisms involved in mental arithmetic at varying ages (Gilmore et al., 2015). It helps individuals shift their attention from procedural solutions to identifying underlying numerical relationships (Gilmore et al., 2015).
Some domain-general functions, such as executive functions, are found to decline over the lifespan (Spreng & Turner, 2019). Consequently, it may be assumed that certain arithmetic skills—particularly complex mental calculation that requires quick processing, inhibition, shifting, or the updating of information in working memory—also decline with advanced age. Research demonstrates that, compared to young adults, older adults employ fewer and less efficient strategies, compute more slowly, and are less accurate, especially as task complexity increases (Duverne & Lemaire, 2005; Hinault & Lemaire, 2016; Salthouse & Coon, 1994). This decline is thought to be associated with reduced executive control, which makes it more difficult for older adults, for example, to switch between problem-solving strategies or to inhibit interfering information (Hinault & Lemaire, 2016; Hodzik & Lemaire, 2011). In contrast, older adults appear to retain highly automated processes, such as arithmetic fact retrieval, at levels comparable to or even better than those of young adults (Zamarian et al., 2018). However, slower peripheral processing may hinder the response process in older adults, contributing to the age-related differences observed in fact retrieval speed (Zamarian et al., 2007c).
Mathematical performance may be influenced not only by cognitive factors but also by subjective factors (Dowker et al., 2016; Hernández De La Hera et al., 2023; Rossi et al., 2022; Rossi et al., 2023). Math anxiety is a psychological condition characterized by feelings of tension, apprehension, or even fear when dealing with numbers and math-related situations (Ashcraft, 2002). Symptoms can manifest physically (e.g., nervousness and physiological reactions) and cognitively (e.g., worry and self-critical thoughts; Liebert & Morris, 1967). Math anxiety can affect people of all ages (Dowker et al., 2016; Dowker & Sheridan, 2022; Hart & Ganley, 2019). Meta-analyses, primarily focusing on studies involving children, indicate a small-to-moderate negative association between math anxiety and mathematical achievement, suggesting that higher math anxiety levels are generally linked to lower mathematical performance (Barroso et al., 2021). Significant gender effects have been observed, with women typically reporting higher math anxiety levels and rating their abilities lower compared to men, although actual performance differences in gender-equitable educational settings tend to be minimal or absent (Devine et al., 2012; Dowker et al., 2016; Spelke, 2005). Mathematical performance is associated not only with math anxiety but also with attitudes toward mathematics—encompassing a range of components such as an individual’s feelings, beliefs, and opinions regarding mathematics that influence engagement with the subject—and math self-concept, which refers to one’s perception of their own mathematical competence (Di Martino & Zan, 2010; Lipnevich et al., 2016; Rossi et al., 2022; Hernández De La Hera et al., 2023). Various factors may influence math anxiety, attitudes toward mathematics, and math self-concept, including an individual’s past experiences, social factors (such as parental attitudes or peer influences), or cultural stereotypes (Rossi et al., 2022). To our knowledge, few studies (e.g., Hart & Ganley, 2019) have examined the relationship between mathematics and these subjective factors across adulthood, encompassing both younger and older individuals, rather than focusing exclusively on children or university students.
Our study aimed to fill this gap by systematically investigating basic arithmetic competence throughout adulthood, including both younger and older adults, while examining the relative contributions of different domain-general and domain-specific cognitive abilities, as well as subjective factors. Specifically, we focused on exact simple and complex computations, approximate complex computations, and the understanding of arithmetic principles as proxies of arithmetic fact knowledge, procedural knowledge, and conceptual knowledge. We were interested in (1) the effects of demographic variables (age, education, and sex); (2) the effects of domain-general cognitive functions (particularly, processing speed and executive functions); (3) the impact of subjective factors (like math anxiety, math self-concept, attitudes toward mathematics, and the frequency of engagement with numbers in daily life, educational settings, and professional contexts in the last 10 years); and (4) the predictors of arithmetic processing, which include different objective and subjective arithmetic measures, alongside demographic variables and domain-general cognitive factors. For objective arithmetic measures, we used accuracy rates from computerized tasks assessing various arithmetic skills. For subjective arithmetic measures, we employed self-rated scales adapted for adults, assessing math anxiety, math self-concept, attitudes toward mathematics, and the frequency of engagement with numbers.
We predicted that (1) older age and lower education levels would be associated with poorer arithmetic performance, particularly in more complex tasks. Whether there are differences between women and men on any of the tasks remains an open question. Furthermore, we expected that (2) better domain-general cognitive functions would be associated with better arithmetic performance. In particular, executive functions should play a key role in complex mental calculation, whether exact or approximate, and in the conceptual task. Additionally, we predicted that (3) lower math anxiety levels, more positive attitudes toward mathematics, a stronger math self-concept, and a greater frequency of engagement with numbers would be associated with better arithmetic performance. Finally, we assumed (4) close interrelationships among the different components of arithmetic. Although the different components are, in principle, independent, a highly proficient individual is likely to excel across all arithmetic tasks. Since simple facts appear to underpin proficiency in more complex computations, we expected them to serve as a significant predictor for other tasks.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Between December 2022 and August 2024, 134 participants were recruited through personal acquaintances and public advertisements. The inclusion criteria were being older than 18 years old, having completed at least 8 years of education, and having achieved a score of at least 27 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for people older than 60 years (Folstein et al., 1975). The exclusion criteria were having a history of neurological diseases (e.g., dementia), psychiatric disorders (e.g., major depression), addiction, substance abuse, or repeated surgery with general anesthesia in the past two years.
Overall, the mean age of our sample was 43.1 years (SD 14.2, range 20–68) and the mean length of education was 13.4 years (SD 3.0, range 8–21). In total, 67 participants (50%) were female. All participants were German-speaking, living in Austria or Germany, and had an estimated verbal intelligence quotient of at least 85 (Lehrl, 2005). Detailed information on the participants’ demographic characteristics by decade, as well as their performance on neuropsychological tests, arithmetic tasks, and subjective measures, can be found in Table S1 in Supplementary Materials.
With a sample size of 134 participants, we aimed to ensure sufficient statistical power to detect effect sizes (f2) between 0.10 and 0.15 (Cohen, 1988) in multiple regression models that could include up to 13 predictors (two-sided, α = 0.05, power = 0.95; G*Power version 3.1.9.7; Faul et al., 2007).

2.2. Neuropsychological Background Assessment

Participants completed a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment to ensure the integrity of their cognitive functioning (for a description, see Supplementary Materials). This included standardized cognitive tests of verbal episodic memory, verbal attention span, executive functions (verbal working memory, verbal fluency, set-shifting, and interference inhibition), and information processing speed (Aschenbrenner et al., 2000; Bäumler, 1985; Härting et al., 2000; Petermann et al., 2016; Smith, 1973). Additionally, a mental health questionnaire was administered (Herrmann et al., 1995). For our primary analyses, we focused specifically on verbal working memory, verbal fluency, set-shifting, interference inhibition, and information processing speed.

2.3. Objective Arithmetic Measures

Arithmetic competence was assessed using selected items from the Number Processing and Calculation (NPC) battery (Delazer et al., 2003) in a computerized format with E-Prime 3.0 Software (for a detailed description, see Supplementary Materials). The tasks included simple facts (FACTS, 40 items), exact complex calculations (COMPL, 40 items), approximate complex calculations (APPROX, 16 items), and arithmetic principles (PRINC, 20 items). FACTS and COMPL involved production tasks, while APPROX was a multiple-choice task requiring participants to choose the closest alternative to the correct solution. PRINC required solving problems by inference without direct calculation. While FACTS, COMPL, and APPROX assessed all four basic operations, PRINC focused specifically on addition and multiplication. In each task, the items were displayed at the center of the computer screen until a response was given or the time limit expired. The participants were instructed to enter their responses as quickly and accurately as possible. The response times (RTs) and accuracy were recorded. Analyses were conducted using the overall scores for each task, without distinguishing between operations. To facilitate more accurate comparisons between tasks with differing demands (e.g., production vs. verification), our analyses focused on accuracy rates. In this study, we primarily used RTs for quality control purposes (e.g., to identify responses that were implausibly fast in the approximation task).

2.4. Subjective Arithmetic Measures

We assessed math anxiety, math self-concept, attitudes toward mathematics, and the frequency of engagement with numbers using Likert-scale questionnaires that were translated and adapted for adult participants (for a description, see Supplementary Materials). Math anxiety (affective and cognitive) and math self-concept were measured using selected items from Henschel and Roick’s (2017) scale, with higher scores indicating greater anxiety or higher perceived competence. Higher scores on the Attitudes Toward Mathematics Instrument (ATMI; Tapia, 1996) reflect more positive attitudes. Finally, the participants were asked to respond to two questions regarding their frequency of interactions with numbers in everyday life, educational settings, and professional situations. Analyses were conducted on the individual median scores for each (sub)scale.

2.5. Procedure

Testing was conducted either at the Medical University of Innsbruck or in a quiet environment of the participant’s choice (e.g., the participant’s home or individual study rooms). Neuropsychological tests were administered in a pseudo-randomized order, followed by questionnaires on subjective arithmetic measures. The computerized arithmetic tasks were administered last to prevent any influence on responses to the subjective scales. The entire testing session lasted 75–90 min, with breaks allowed as needed.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out with SPSS Version 29.0 (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA), employing parametric statistical methods. Separate Pearson correlation analyses were conducted to investigate the relationships between objective arithmetic performance and demographic variables, domain-general cognitive factors, and subjective arithmetic measures. The false discovery rate (FDR) method was applied to correct for multiple correlations. We conducted a series of hierarchical regression analyses with each objective arithmetic measure as the dependent variable in separate models, while including the remaining objective arithmetic measures as domain-specific regressors. Independent variables were entered into each model in the following order: first, demographic variables; second, domain-general cognitive factors; third, domain-specific variables; and finally, subjective arithmetic measures. Only variables that showed significant correlations (after FDR correction) with each arithmetic task were included in the regression analysis. The significance threshold was set at α = 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Outcomes in Neuropsychological Background Tests

In general, group scores in neuropsychological tests were in the average range of standardized norms (for detailed information, see Supplementary Materials).

3.2. Objective Arithmetic Outcomes

The results are reported in Table 1. Pairwise t-tests indicated that accuracy rates were significantly higher when solving FACTS compared to COMPL (t(133) = 12.45, p < 0.001), to APPROX (t(133) = −19.26, p < 0.001), and to PRINC (t(133) = 10.54, p < 0.001). Additionally, accuracy rates in COMPL were higher than those for PRINC (t(133) = 2.12, p = 0.036) and for APPROX (t(133) = −11.98, p < 0.001). The comparison between PRINC and APPROX was also significant (t(133) = −8.95, p < 0.001), with APPROX being the least accurate. Ceiling performance was achieved by 7.5% (n = 10) of the participants in FACTS, 2.2% (n = 3) in COMPL, and 3.7% (n = 5) in PRINC. None of the participants reached ceiling performance in APPROX. Only a few participants performed below 1 SD from the group mean on each task—FACTS 14.2% (n = 19), COMPL 14.9% (n = 20), APPROX 17.2% (n = 23), and PRINC 10.4% (n = 14).

3.3. Subjective Arithmetic Outcomes

In general, the group’s mean scores indicated low levels of math anxiety, along with moderate-to-high levels of math self-concept, positive attitudes toward mathematics, and moderate engagement with numbers. A pairwise t-test showed comparable levels of affective and cognitive math anxiety (t(133) = −1.00, p = 0.319). Only a few participants achieved an individual median score of at least 3, suggestive of moderate-to-high levels, on the affective (n = 5, 3.7%) and cognitive math anxiety (n = 8, 6.0%) subscales. The majority of participants scored at least 3 on the subscales assessing math self-concept (n = 103, 76.9%) and attitudes toward mathematics (n = 123, 91.8%).

3.4. Correlation Analysis

3.4.1. Correlations with Demographic Variables

There was a significant negative correlation between age and accuracy in PRINC (r = −0.295, p < 0.001). Education correlated positively and significantly with accuracy across all arithmetic tasks—FACTS (r = 0.312, p < 0.001), COMPL (r = 0.407, p < 0.001), APPROX (r = 0.321, p < 0.001), and PRINC (r = 0.201, p = 0.020). Sex correlated significantly with accuracy in COMPL (r = 0.228, p = 0.008), with women scoring lower than men. Other correlations were not significant (all p > 0.05).
For correlations between demographic variables and subjective arithmetic measures, please refer to Figure S1 in Supplementary Materials.

3.4.2. Correlations with Domain-General Cognitive Factors

The results are presented in Figure 1. We found no significant correlations for FACTS and COMPL. In contrast, higher accuracy in APPROX significantly correlated with higher scores in a verbal working memory test. Additionally, better scores on tests of interference inhibition and information processing speed were associated with higher accuracy in PRINC.
Figure 2 shows the correlation of performance on the PRINC task with age (in years) and interference inhibition (measured as the difference score between two Stroop sub-tests; higher difference scores mean lower performance).

3.4.3. Correlations with Subjective Arithmetic Outcomes

The results are presented in Figure 3. Higher accuracy rates in FACTS and COMPL were associated with a greater frequency of interactions with numbers, lower anxiety levels, a more positive attitude toward mathematics, and, specifically for COMPL, a stronger math self-concept. Higher accuracy rates in APPROX and PRINC were significantly correlated with a greater frequency of interactions with numbers and, specifically for PRINC, with a stronger math self-concept. Other correlations were not significant (all p > 0.05).

3.5. Hierarchical Regression Analysis

3.5.1. Simple Calculations

The results are reported in Table 2 (upper panel). The analysis conducted for FACTS showed that Model 1, which included education as a predictor, was significant (F(1, 132) = 14.22, p < 0.001), accounting for 9.0% of the variance. Model 2, which added objective performance in other arithmetic tasks to the predictor of Model 1, explained significantly more variance (ΔR2 = 0.372, F(3, 129) = 30.10, p < 0.001). This model accounted for 45.2% of the variance and was significant (F(4, 129) = 28.48, p < 0.001), with COMPL emerging as a significant predictor. Model 3, which added subjective arithmetic measures to the factors of Model 2, did not yield a significant increase in explained variance (ΔR2 = 0.018, F(5, 124) = 0.84, p = 0.520). This model accounted for 44.9% of the variance and was significant (F(9, 124) = 13.05, p < 0.001), with COMPL being the only significant predictor.

3.5.2. Exact Complex Calculations

The results are reported in Table 2 (lower panel). In the analysis computed for COMPL, Model 1 was significant (F(2, 131) = 20.18, p < 0.001), explaining 22.4% of the variance. Both education and sex were identified as significant predictors. In Model 2, objective performance in other arithmetic tasks was added to the predictors of Model 1, resulting in a significant increase in explained variance (ΔR2 = 0.328, F(3, 128) = 32.15, p < 0.001). Model 2 explained 54.7% of the variance and was significant (F(5, 128) = 33.12, p < 0.001). The significant predictors in Model 2 were education, sex, FACTS, and PRINC. Model 3, which finally added subjective arithmetic factors to the predictors of Model 2, did not explain significantly more variance (ΔR2 = 0.019, F(5, 123) = 1.15, p = 0.339). This model accounted for 55.0% of the variance and was significant (F(10, 123) = 17.23, p < 0.001), with education and FACTS identified as the significant predictors.

3.5.3. Approximate Complex Calculations

The results are reported in Table 3 (upper panel). The analysis of APPROX showed that Model 1, which included education as a predictor, was significant (F(1, 132) = 15.15, p < 0.001) and explained 9.6% of the variance. Model 2, which added verbal working memory as a predictor, significantly improved the model fit (ΔR2 = 0.036, F(1, 131) = 5.45, p = 0.021). Overall, Model 2 accounted for 12.6% of the variance and was significant (F(2, 131) = 10.56, p < 0.001), with both education and verbal working memory identified as significant predictors. In Model 3, objective performance in other arithmetic tasks was added to the predictors of Model 2, resulting in a significant increase in explained variance (ΔR2 = 0.138, F(3, 128) = 8.14, p < 0.001). This model was significant (F(5, 128) = 9.80, p < 0.001) and accounted for 24.9% of the variance, with COMPL and PRINC emerging as significant predictors. Model 4, which added a subjective arithmetic measure to the predictors of Model 3, did not account for a significant increase in variance (ΔR2 = 0.006, F(1, 127) = 1.05, p = 0.307). Overall, Model 4 explained 24.9% of the variance and was significant (F(6, 127) = 8.34, p < 0.001), with COMPL being the only significant predictor.

3.5.4. Arithmetic Principles

The results are reported in Table 3 (lower panel). In the analysis conducted for PRINC, Model 1 was significant (F(2, 131) = 9.08, p < 0.001), accounting for 10.8% of the variance, with both age and education as significant predictors. Model 2 improved upon Model 1, accounting for significantly more variance (ΔR2 = 0.167, F(2, 129) = 15.15, p < 0.001). Model 2 explained 26.7% of the variance and was significant (F(4, 129) = 13.10, p < 0.001), with interference inhibition emerging as the only significant predictor. Model 3, which added objective performance in other arithmetic tasks to the predictors of Model 2, accounted for a further significant increase in variance (ΔR2 = 0.100, F(3, 126) = 6.84, p < 0.001). Model 3 explained 35.4% of the variance and was significant (F(7, 126) = 11.43, p < 0.001), with interference inhibition and COMPL emerging as significant predictors. Finally, Model 4, which added subjective arithmetic measures to the predictors of Model 3, also accounted for a significant increase in variance (ΔR2 = 0.063, F(2, 124) = 7.06, p < 0.001). Overall, Model 4 explained 41.1% of the variance and was significant (F(9, 124) = 11.31, p < 0.001). The significant predictors identified in this model were interference inhibition and frequency of interactions with numbers.
In all analyses, the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) indicated that multicollinearity was not a concern. In summary, the hierarchical regression analyses indicated significant model improvements, particularly when adding competence in other arithmetic tasks as predictors.

4. Discussion

This study investigated basic arithmetic competence throughout adulthood, encompassing both younger and older adults, while evaluating how domain-general cognitive functions, domain-specific skills, and subjective arithmetic factors contribute to performance. Overall, we found that the accuracy rate varied across tasks, with simple computations yielding the most accurate responses (mean 89% correct), while approximate complex computations resulted in the least accurate answers (mean 57% correct). These findings align with previous studies on various aspects of arithmetic competence in healthy adults (Delazer et al., 2003). The results from the subjective arithmetic scales indicated that levels of math anxiety were low in our sample, with means of 1.3 on a Likert scale from 1 (“does not apply at all”) to 4 (“fully applies”), especially when compared to those reported for younger samples (e.g., elementary school children, with means of 2.0 and 2.2 on the cognitive and affective math anxiety subscales used in our study; Henschel & Roick, 2017). In general, math self-concept, attitudes toward mathematics, and the frequency of handling numbers in daily life, educational settings, and work contexts were rated as moderate-to-high. We will discuss our results in the same order as we made our aims and predictions.

4.1. Demographic Factors

An age effect was found only in the arithmetic principles task, which assessed conceptual knowledge. Other arithmetic tasks showed no decline in accuracy with increasing age. Contrary to expectations, performance on both exact and approximate complex calculations was not negatively associated with age. Notably, our study focused on accuracy rather than response time, which prior research has shown to decrease with age (Avcil & Artemenko, 2025; Hinault & Lemaire, 2016; Salthouse & Coon, 1994; Zamarian et al., 2018). Similarly, a recent cross-sectional study on number processing and calculation skills (Avcil & Artemenko, 2025) found comparable accuracy in complex addition and subtraction tasks among younger, middle-aged, and older adults. In our study, we also found that higher arithmetic competence was associated with higher education levels, aligning with expectations and prior studies (OECD, 2024; Ritchie & Bates, 2013). Our observation that men outperformed women in only one task—namely, exact complex calculations—is consistent with research suggesting minimal gender differences in arithmetic performance (e.g., Rossi et al., 2023). In countries promoting gender equality in education, differences in number processing and arithmetic skills are often negligible or absent (Guiso et al., 2008), supporting the view that such disparities mostly stem from social factors (e.g., educational opportunities or societal role stereotypes) rather than innate ability (Hyde & Mertz, 2009; Spelke, 2005).

4.2. Domain-General Cognitive Factors

Our findings reveal that domain-general cognitive factors are more strongly associated with “non-exact” arithmetic performance—namely, approximate complex calculations and understanding of arithmetic principles—than with “exact” arithmetic performance. Specifically, performance on simple computations, which serves as a proxy for arithmetic fact knowledge, and on exact complex calculations, was not linked to any domain-general cognitive functions. In contrast, better performance on approximate complex computations was related to higher verbal working memory capacity, while increased accuracy on the arithmetic principles task was associated with faster information processing and stronger inhibitory control. These results suggest that, in adults, domain-general factors play a more relevant role in supporting arithmetic performance on tasks involving approximation or conceptual understanding, rather than on those relying on the retrieval of arithmetic facts from memory or exact calculations. Existing research supports this, indicating that cognitive functions such as working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting are particularly important when arithmetic skills are less automatized, newly learned, or cognitively demanding (Desoete, 2015; Hinault & Lemaire, 2016; Zamarian et al., 2007b; Zamarian et al., 2018). Our study aligns with this research, showing no significant correlations with domain-general cognitive factors for exact (simple and complex) calculations, while the significant associations observed for approximate calculations and arithmetic principles underscore the greater relevance of domain-general cognitive factors for “non-exact” arithmetic performance.

4.3. Subjective Arithmetic Factors

Our study reveals several important findings regarding subjective arithmetic factors in adulthood. First, we observed that higher arithmetic competence was associated with lower math anxiety levels, more positive attitudes toward mathematics, a stronger math self-concept, and a greater frequency of using numbers in daily life, educational settings, and professional contexts. Second, our results indicate that math anxiety levels, particularly on the affective scale, tend to decrease with age (see Figure S1 in Supplementary Materials).
These findings build on prior research (Dowker et al., 2016; Hernández De La Hera et al., 2023; Rossi et al., 2022, 2023) by demonstrating that different dimensions of basic arithmetic competence—including simple computations, exact and approximate complex computations, and understanding of arithmetic principles—relate to various subjective arithmetic factors. Specifically, the observed associations between arithmetic skills and math anxiety, math self-concept, and attitudes toward mathematics highlight the interplay between objective and subjective arithmetic components across adulthood.
While much research has examined math anxiety in children, adolescents, and university students (Barroso et al., 2021), few studies have investigated it in the general adult population (Hart & Ganley, 2019) or older individuals (Donelle et al., 2007). Our findings contribute to filling this gap by showing decreasing math anxiety with age, which aligns with aging research suggesting changes in emotion regulation—particularly, a greater focus on positive information, goals, and experiences—in later life (Mather, 2016; Mather & Carstensen, 2005).

4.4. Predictors of Arithmetic Performance

The regression analyses that included demographic variables and domain-general cognitive factors, alongside objective and subjective arithmetic measures, explained a total of 45%, 55%, 25%, and 41% of the variance in simple computations, exact complex computations, approximate complex computations, and arithmetic principles. Several aspects of these analyses are particularly noteworthy. First, the inclusion of proficiency in other arithmetic tasks in the regression models accounted for an additional 37% of the variance in simple computations, 33% in exact complex computations, 14% in approximate complex computations, and 10% in the arithmetic principles task. These contributions, especially in computation tasks, were greater than those attributable to demographic variables, domain-general cognitive factors, and subjective arithmetic outcomes. Second, proficiency in other arithmetic tasks emerged as a significant predictor of performance on both simple and complex computations (whether exact or approximate), but not for the arithmetic principles task. Specifically, performance on exact complex computations was identified as a significant predictor of performance on both simple computations and approximate complex computations, while proficiency in simple computations emerged as a significant predictor of performance on exact complex computations. Proficiency in exact complex computations clearly provides an advantage when solving simple calculations or estimating solutions to complex arithmetic problems. Since exact complex calculations can be approached by breaking the problem down into smaller components (Zamarian et al., 2007a), proficiency in retrieving arithmetic facts aids in performing complex arithmetic problems.
Notably, while objective arithmetic measures predicted performance across calculation tasks, subjective measures showed predictive value only for arithmetic principles. After controlling for other factors, the strongest predictors of performance in the arithmetic principles task were interference inhibition and self-reported engagement with numbers in daily life, educational settings, and professional contexts. None of the other arithmetic tasks emerged as significant predictors, consistent with neuropsychological findings from single-case studies of brain-damaged patients, which demonstrates dissociations among factual, procedural, and conceptual knowledge (for reviews, Delazer, 2003; Zamarian et al., 2007a). These results suggest that, in healthy adults, competence with arithmetic principles (e.g., commutativity, associativity, and distributivity) relies on distinct mechanisms from proficiency in computation, regardless of whether calculations are exact, approximate, simple, or complex. Understanding and applying arithmetic principles reflect arithmetic conceptual knowledge (Delazer, 2003). Regular engagement with numbers, alongside inhibitory control (see also Gilmore et al., 2015), may help adults develop greater automaticity in recognizing numerical patterns and relationships while strengthening their conceptual understanding of arithmetic principles. General inductive reasoning abilities might also contribute to the comprehension and application of mathematical concepts (Christou & Papageorgiou, 2007). While our findings highlight relationships between arithmetic conceptual knowledge, executive functions, and real-world numerical experiences, future research should directly examine how inductive reasoning influences performance on arithmetic principles tasks across adulthood.

4.5. Limitations and Strengths

We ought to highlight some limitations. The results, particularly concerning the subjective arithmetic measures, may be somewhat biased, as it is possible that only individuals with positive attitudes toward mathematics chose to participate. Future investigations involving adults should aim to increase variability by including individuals with high levels of math anxiety, low math self-concept, and negative attitudes toward mathematics. This approach could help clarify the distinct contributions of various subjective factors—beyond the frequency of engagement with numbers in daily life—to the comprehension and application of arithmetic principles across adulthood. Additionally, it may lead to a deeper understanding of how subjective factors influence the relationship between domain-general cognitive functions and arithmetic performance in older adults. As suggested by Zhang et al. (2018), the contribution of executive functions may become particularly evident in individuals with high levels of math anxiety, which can deplete the cognitive resources necessary for excelling in arithmetic. Furthermore, we assessed subjective arithmetic factors using shortened versions of existing measures, which were adapted for a general adult population. Future studies should evaluate the reliability of these adapted scales. Additionally, our study design does not allow for a detailed investigation of the direction of causation between objective and subjective arithmetic outcomes. While research indicates that math anxiety may significantly influence an individual’s math self-concept in both children and university students (Justicia-Galiano et al., 2017; Rossi et al., 2022), the relationship between math anxiety and mathematical performance is likely bidirectional (Carey et al., 2016), complicating the analysis in cross-sectional studies such as ours. A key strength of our study lies in being the first to investigate the relative contributions of various domain-general cognitive functions, domain-specific skills, and subjective factors to basic arithmetic competence across adulthood. Additionally, our inclusion of a broad adult age range (20–68 years) enhances generalizability compared to the university student samples typically used in prior research. This approach yields more representative data on adult cognitive profiles by encompassing both younger and older individuals.

5. Conclusions

Competence in performing exact mental calculations (whether simple or complex), estimating approximate solutions to complex arithmetic problems, and applying arithmetic principles (e.g., the commutative property) constitutes a fundamental goal of basic education. This competence remains essential throughout adulthood for maintaining independent functioning. Research has consistently shown that individuals with low numeracy skills face significant disadvantages compared to those with high numeracy (Reyna et al., 2009). Our study demonstrates that most arithmetic abilities remain largely stable across adulthood and are associated with positive feelings, beliefs, and attitudes toward mathematics. Arithmetic encompasses several independent components that tend to remain stable with aging, although performance on arithmetic principles presents an important exception. While advanced age shows minimal effects on factual and procedural knowledge, it may negatively impact arithmetic conceptual knowledge. Our findings reveal a strong correlation between arithmetic principles and interference inhibition, which is known to decline with age (Spreng & Turner, 2019). Beyond interference inhibition, we found that the frequency of engagement with numbers in everyday situations significantly predicts performance on the arithmetic principles task. This finding reinforces the importance of including subjective measures when testing arithmetic proficiency in the general adult population. Notably, higher objective arithmetic competence is associated not only with more positive feelings toward mathematics but also with greater engagement with numbers in daily activities.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ejihpe15050084/s1, Figure S1: Coefficients of a Pearson correlation analysis between subjective arithmetic outcomes and demographical variables for the whole sample (N = 134); Table S1: Sociodemographic variables, neuropsychological test scores, objective arithmetic measures, and subjective arithmetic measures across decades; Table S2: Scores in the neuropsychological background tests that were entered in the correlation analysis for the whole sample (N = 134).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.B., M.P., and L.Z.; methodology, M.D., D.B., M.P., and L.Z.; formal analysis, E.G., K.T., and L.Z.; investigation, E.G. and K.T.; resources, M.D., M.K., and L.Z.; data curation, E.G., K.T., and L.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, E.G. and L.Z.; writing—review and editing, K.T., M.D., M.K., D.B., and M.P.; visualization, E.G. and K.T.; supervision, L.Z.; project administration, E.G., M.K., and L.Z.; funding acquisition, D.B., M.P., and L.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the Euregio Tyrol-South Tyrol-Trentino within the framework of the 4th Euregio Science Fund call and by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF; grant number: IPN 135-B). For open access purposes, the authors have applied a CC BY public copyright license to any author-accepted manuscript version arising from this submission.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study is part of a larger project on number cognition across the lifespan. It was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical University of Innsbruck with the approval number 1421/2021, dated 22 March 2022.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Acknowledgments

We thank V. Mayr, S. Molina Terron, J. Sinitski, C. Schaar, K. Berwanger, and M. Eichler for their help with data collection as well as the participants for their contribution.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Aschenbrenner, S., Tucha, O., & Lange, K. W. (2000). Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test (RWT). Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 181–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Avcil, M., & Artemenko, C. (2025). Development of arithmetic across the lifespan: A registered report. Developmental Psychology. in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Barroso, C., Ganley, C. M., McGraw, A. L., Geer, E. A., Hart, S. A., & Daucourt, M. C. (2021). A meta-analysis of the relation between math anxiety and math achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 147(2), 134–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bäumler, G. (1985). Farbe-Wort-Interferenztest (FWIT) nach J. R. Stroop—Handanweisung. Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 36–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., & Szücs, D. (2016). The chicken or the egg? The direction of the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Chen, E. H., & Bailey, D. H. (2021). Dual-task studies of working memory and arithmetic performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(2), 220–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Chow, J. C., & Ekholm, E. (2019). Language domains differentially predict mathematics performance in young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 179–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Christou, C., & Papageorgiou, E. (2007). A framework of mathematics inductive reasoning. Learning and Instruction, 17(1), 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cragg, L., Keeble, S., Richardson, S., Roome, H. E., & Gilmore, C. (2017). Direct and indirect influences of executive functions on mathematics achievement. Cognition, 162, 12–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Delazer, M. (2003). Neuropsychological findings on conceptual knowledge of arithmetic. In The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise (pp. 385–407). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  14. Delazer, M., Girelli, L., Granà, A., & Domahs, F. (2003). Number processing and calculation—Normative data from healthy adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17(3), 331–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Desoete, A. (2015). Cognitive predictors of mathematical abilities and disabilities. In R. C. Kadosh, & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 915–932). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Devine, A., Fawcett, K., Szűcs, D., & Dowker, A. (2012). Gender differences in mathematics anxiety and the relation to mathematics performance while controlling for test anxiety. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 8(1), 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Di Martino, P., & Zan, R. (2010). ‘“Me and maths”: Towards a definition of attitude grounded on students’ narratives’. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 13(1), 27–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Donelle, L., Hoffman-Goetz, L., & Arocha, J. F. (2007). Assessing health numeracy among community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Health Communication, 12(7), 651–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Dowker, A., & Sheridan, H. (2022). Relationships between mathematics performance and attitude to mathematics: Influences of gender, test anxiety, and working memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 814992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Duverne, S., & Lemaire, P. (2005). Aging and mental arithmetic. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 397–411). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Gilmore, C., Keeble, S., Richardson, S., & Cragg, L. (2015). The role of cognitive inhibition in different components of arithmetic. ZDM, 47(5), 771–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Diversity. Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320(5880), 1164–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hart, S. A., & Ganley, C. M. (2019). The nature of math anxiety in adults: Prevalence and correlates. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 5(2), 122–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hawes, Z. C. K., Gilligan-Lee, K. A., & Mix, K. S. (2022). Effects of spatial training on mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 58(1), 112–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Härting, C., Markowitsch, H. J., Neufeld, U., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K., & Kessler, J. (2000). Wechsler gedächtnis test—Revidierte Fassung: Deutsche adaption der revidierten fassung der wechsler-memory scale. Huber. [Google Scholar]
  29. Henschel, S., & Roick, T. (2017). Relationships of mathematics performance, control and value beliefs with cognitive and affective math anxiety. Learning and Individual Differences, 55, 97–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hernández De La Hera, J. M., Morales-Rodríguez, F. M., Rodríguez-Gobiet, J. P., & Martínez-Ramón, J. P. (2023). Attitudes toward mathematics/statistics, anxiety, self-efficacy and academic performance: An artificial neural network. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1214892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Herrmann, C., Buss, U., & Snaith, R. P. (1995). Hospital anxiety and depression scale-german version (HADS-D). Huber. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hinault, T., & Lemaire, P. (2016). Age-related changes in strategic variations during arithmetic problem solving. In Progress in brain research (Vol. 227, pp. 257–276). Elsevier. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hodzik, S., & Lemaire, P. (2011). Inhibition and shifting capacities mediate adults’ age-related differences in strategy selection and repertoire. Acta Psychologica, 137(3), 335–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hyde, J. S., & Mertz, J. E. (2009). Gender, culture, and mathematics performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(22), 8801–8807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Justicia-Galiano, M. J., Martín-Puga, M. E., Linares, R., & Pelegrina, S. (2017). Math anxiety and math performance in children: The mediating roles of working memory and math self-concept. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(4), 573–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Knops, A., Nuerk, H.-C., & Göbel, S. M. (2017). Domain-general factors influencing numerical and arithmetic processing. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 3(2), 112–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. LeFevre, J., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D., & Penner-Wilger, M. (2010). Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development, 81(6), 1753–1767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lehrl, S. (2005). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest: MWT-B (5th ed.). Spitta. [Google Scholar]
  39. Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20(3), 975–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lipnevich, A. A., Preckel, F., & Krumm, S. (2016). Mathematics attitudes and their unique contribution to achievement: Going over and above cognitive ability and personality. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 70–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Mather, M. (2016). The affective neuroscience of aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 213–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 496–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. OECD. (2024). Do adults have the skills they need to thrive in a changing world? Survey of adult skills 2023. OECD. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Petermann, F., Jäncke, L., & Waldmann, H.-C. (2016). Neuropsychological assessment battery deutschsprachige adaptation der Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB) von Robert A. Stern und Travis White. Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
  45. Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(2), 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Reyna, V. F., Nelson, W. L., Han, P. K., & Dieckmann, N. F. (2009). How numeracy influences risk comprehension and medical decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 943–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Ritchie, S. J., & Bates, T. C. (2013). Enduring links from childhood mathematics and reading achievement to adult socioeconomic status. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1301–1308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rossi, S., Xenidou-Dervou, I., & Cipora, K. (2023). Emotions and mathematics: Anxiety profiles and their influence on arithmetic performance in university students. Royal Society Open Science, 10(10), 230861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Rossi, S., Xenidou-Dervou, I., Simsek, E., Artemenko, C., Daroczy, G., Nuerk, H., & Cipora, K. (2022). Mathematics–gender stereotype endorsement influences mathematics anxiety, self-concept, and performance differently in men and women. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1513(1), 121–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Salthouse, T. A., & Coon, V. E. (1994). Interpretation of differential deficits: The case of aging and mental arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1172–1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Smith, A. (1973). Symbol digit modalities test [Dataset]. APA PsycTests. [Google Scholar]
  52. Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science? A critical review. American Psychologist, 60(9), 950–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Spreng, R. N., & Turner, G. R. (2019). The shifting architecture of cognition and brain function in older adulthood. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(4), 523–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Tapia, M. (1996, November 6). The attitudes toward mathematics instrument. Annual Meeting of the Mid-South Educational Research Association, Tuscaloosa, Alabama. [Google Scholar]
  55. Zamarian, L., López-Rolón, A., & Delazer, M. (2007a). Neuropsychological case studies on arithmetic processing. In D. B. Berch, & M. M. M. Mazzocco (Eds.), Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities (pp. 245–263). Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. [Google Scholar]
  56. Zamarian, L., Scherfler, C., Kremser, C., Pertl, M.-T., Gizewski, E., Benke, T., & Delazer, M. (2018). Arithmetic learning in advanced age. PLoS ONE, 13(2), e0193529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Zamarian, L., Semenza, C., Domahs, F., Benke, T., & Delazer, M. (2007b). Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment: Effects of shifting and interference in simple arithmetic. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 263(1–2), 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Zamarian, L., Stadelmann, E., Nürk, H.-C., Gamboz, N., Marksteiner, J., & Delazer, M. (2007c). Effects of age and mild cognitive impairment on direct and indirect access to arithmetic knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1511–1521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Zhang, J., Cheung, S. K., Wu, C., & Meng, Y. (2018). Cognitive and affective correlates of Chinese children’s mathematical word problem solving. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Coefficients of a Pearson correlation analysis between objective arithmetic measures and domain-general cognitive outcomes (N = 134). Notes: (*) indicates statistical significance after FDR correction; FACTS = simple arithmetic facts; COMPL = complex exact calculation; APPROX = complex approximate calculation; PRINC = arithmetic principles.
Figure 1. Coefficients of a Pearson correlation analysis between objective arithmetic measures and domain-general cognitive outcomes (N = 134). Notes: (*) indicates statistical significance after FDR correction; FACTS = simple arithmetic facts; COMPL = complex exact calculation; APPROX = complex approximate calculation; PRINC = arithmetic principles.
Ejihpe 15 00084 g001
Figure 2. Correlation of accuracy rates in PRINC with age (in years) and interference inhibition (measured as the difference score between two Stroop sub-tests). Notes: PRINC = arithmetic principles. The best-fit line for the correlation between PRINC accuracy and age is shown in blue, while the best-fit line for the correlation between PRINC accuracy and interference inhibition is shown in orange.
Figure 2. Correlation of accuracy rates in PRINC with age (in years) and interference inhibition (measured as the difference score between two Stroop sub-tests). Notes: PRINC = arithmetic principles. The best-fit line for the correlation between PRINC accuracy and age is shown in blue, while the best-fit line for the correlation between PRINC accuracy and interference inhibition is shown in orange.
Ejihpe 15 00084 g002
Figure 3. Coefficients of a Pearson correlation analysis between objective and subjective arithmetic measures (N = 134). Notes: (*) indicates statistical significance after FDR correction; FACTS = simple arithmetic facts; COMPL = complex exact calculation; APPROX = complex approximate calculation; PRINC = arithmetic principles; FIN = frequency of interactions with numbers.
Figure 3. Coefficients of a Pearson correlation analysis between objective and subjective arithmetic measures (N = 134). Notes: (*) indicates statistical significance after FDR correction; FACTS = simple arithmetic facts; COMPL = complex exact calculation; APPROX = complex approximate calculation; PRINC = arithmetic principles; FIN = frequency of interactions with numbers.
Ejihpe 15 00084 g003
Table 1. Scores in objective and subjective arithmetic measures (N = 134).
Table 1. Scores in objective and subjective arithmetic measures (N = 134).
Max. ScoreMSDMinMax
FACTS (% correct)10088.99.857.5100.0
COMPL (% correct)10076.715.122.5100.0
APPROX (% correct)10056.820.50.091.7
PRINC (% correct)10073.516.20.0100.0
Affective math anxiety (individual median)41.30.513
Cognitive math anxiety (individual median)41.30.513
Math self-concept (individual median)42.80.714
Attitudes Toward Mathematics (individual median)43.30.624
FIN (individual median)106.12.2110
Notes: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Min = minimum; Max = maximum; FACTS = simple calculation; COMPL = exact complex calculation; APPROX = approximate complex calculation; PRINC = arithmetic principles; FIN = frequency of interactions with numbers.
Table 2. Coefficients of hierarchical regression analyses where performance in objective arithmetic measures (FACTS/COMPL) is the dependent variable (N = 134).
Table 2. Coefficients of hierarchical regression analyses where performance in objective arithmetic measures (FACTS/COMPL) is the dependent variable (N = 134).
Unstandardized CoefficientsStandardized Coefficients Collinearity
Model BSEBeta (β)tPToleranceVIF
FACTS1(Intercept)0.7490.037 20.198<0.001
Education0.0100.0030.3123.772<0.0011.0001.000
2(Intercept)0.5340.041 13.170<0.001
Education0.0010.0020.0280.3940.6940.8091.235
COMPL0.4130.0510.6318.114<0.0010.6811.468
APPROX0.0400.0360.0841.1280.2610.7491.336
PRINC0.0000.0430.000−0.0030.9980.8331.200
3(Intercept)0.5340.064 8.312<0.001
Education0.0010.0020.0340.4690.6400.7931.262
COMPL0.3980.0540.6097.371<0.0010.6081.646
APPROX0.0330.0360.0680.9130.3630.7371.357
PRINC0.0010.0460.0010.0150.9880.7191.391
Affective math anxiety−0.0080.018−0.037−0.4540.6500.6301.588
Cognitive math anxiety−0.0010.017−0.005−0.0580.9540.6071.646
Math self-concept−0.0140.011−0.094−1.3040.1950.7951.259
Attitudes Toward Mathematics0.0140.0110.0861.2100.2290.8221.216
FIN0.0030.0030.0670.9270.3560.7841.276
COMPL1(Intercept)0.3600.065 5.507<0.001
Education0.0210.0040.4305.612<0.0010.9921.008
Sex (f = 1, m = 2)0.0790.0230.2653.455<0.0010.9921.008
2(Intercept)−0.2640.086 −3.0720.003
Education0.0090.0030.1892.9360.0040.8201.219
Sex (f = 1, m = 2)0.0380.0180.1272.0910.0380.9221.085
FACTS0.7960.1000.5217.943<0.0010.7931.262
APPROX0.0930.0490.1271.8850.0620.7521.330
PRINC0.1210.0590.1302.0580.0420.8581.166
3(Intercept)−0.2540.110 −2.3000.023
Education0.0080.0030.1512.2710.0250.7691.300
Sex (f = 1, m = 2)0.0250.0190.0851.3210.1890.8261.211
FACTS0.7590.1040.4967.319<0.0010.7371.357
APPROX0.0870.0500.1181.7430.0840.7421.348
PRINC0.1060.0630.1141.6750.0960.7351.361
Affective math anxiety−0.0050.025−0.017−0.2220.8240.6121.633
Cognitive math anxiety−0.0150.023−0.047−0.6290.5310.6061.651
Math self-concept0.0100.0160.0390.5980.5510.8091.236
Attitudes Toward Mathematics0.0240.0150.1051.5860.1150.7721.295
FIN0.0020.0050.0270.4080.6840.7771.287
Notes: FACTS = simple calculation; COMPL = exact complex calculation; APPROX = approximate complex calculation; PRINC = arithmetic principles; FIN = frequency of interactions with numbers. The significant statistical results (p < 0.050) are highlighted in bold.
Table 3. Coefficients of hierarchical regression analyses where performance in objective arithmetic measures (APPROX/PRINC) is the dependent variable (N = 134).
Table 3. Coefficients of hierarchical regression analyses where performance in objective arithmetic measures (APPROX/PRINC) is the dependent variable (N = 134).
Unstandardized CoefficientsStandardized Coefficients Collinearity
Model BSEBeta (β)tpToleranceVIF
APPROX1(Intercept)0.2760.077 3.584<0.001
Education0.0220.0060.3213.893<0.0011.0001.000
2(Intercept)0.1680.089 1.8930.061
Education0.0190.0060.2853.450<0.0010.9651.036
Verbal working memory0.0210.0090.1932.3350.0210.9651.036
3(Intercept)−0.2590.154 −1.6830.095
Education0.0090.0060.1321.5830.1160.8161.225
Verbal working memory0.0150.0080.1391.7900.0760.9431.061
FACTS0.2390.2140.1151.1200.2650.5361.865
COMPL0.3170.1500.2332.1140.0360.4662.144
PRINC0.2050.1030.1621.9970.0480.8551.170
4(Intercept)−0.2490.154 −1.6160.108
Education0.0090.0060.1311.5780.1170.8161.225
Verbal working memory0.0140.0080.1331.7180.0880.9391.065
FACTS0.2170.2150.1041.0110.3140.5311.884
COMPL0.3040.1500.2232.0220.0450.4632.159
PRINC0.1700.1080.1351.5760.1180.7711.297
FIN0.0080.0080.0871.0260.3070.7921.263
PRINC1(Intercept)0.7410.074 9.965<0.001
Age−0.0030.001−0.286−3.484<0.0010.9981.002
Education0.0100.0040.1872.2850.0240.9981.002
2(Intercept)0.8530.134 6.347<0.001
Age−0.0020.001−0.139−1.5940.1130.7231.383
Education0.0070.0040.1231.6120.1090.9481.055
Interference inhibition−0.0060.001−0.428−5.300<0.0010.8441.185
Information processing speed0.0010.0020.0310.3410.7340.6871.455
3(Intercept)0.6490.151 4.284<0.001
Age−0.0020.001−0.136−1.6180.1080.6871.455
Education−0.0010.004−0.025−0.3120.7550.7841.275
Interference inhibition−0.0060.001−0.415−5.455<0.0010.8401.191
Information processing speed0.0000.0010.0110.1250.9000.6831.465
FACTS0.0860.1610.0520.5300.5970.5041.982
COMPL0.2380.1110.2212.1470.0340.4572.186
APPROX0.1170.0630.1481.8450.0670.7581.320
4(Intercept)0.6110.146 4.197<0.001
Age−0.0020.001−0.131−1.6330.1050.6871.456
Education−0.0020.004−0.037−0.4830.6300.7721.296
Interference inhibition−0.0060.001−0.411−5.619<0.0010.8261.210
Information processing speed0.0000.001−0.023−0.2830.7780.6651.503
FACTS0.0530.1550.0330.3440.7310.4972.011
COMPL0.1610.1090.1501.4790.1420.4312.319
APPROX0.0840.0610.1071.3790.1700.7411.349
Math self-concept0.0270.0180.1111.5140.1330.8301.205
FIN0.0180.0050.2403.3200.0010.8501.177
Notes: FACTS = simple calculation; COMPL = exact complex calculation; APPROX = approximate complex calculation; PRINC = arithmetic principles; FIN = frequency of interactions with numbers. The significant statistical results (p < 0.050) are highlighted in bold.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Goettfried, E.; Thaler, K.; Delazer, M.; Basso, D.; Piazza, M.; Knoflach, M.; Zamarian, L. Arithmetic Proficiency Across Adulthood: Cognitive and Subjective Influences. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15, 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050084

AMA Style

Goettfried E, Thaler K, Delazer M, Basso D, Piazza M, Knoflach M, Zamarian L. Arithmetic Proficiency Across Adulthood: Cognitive and Subjective Influences. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2025; 15(5):84. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050084

Chicago/Turabian Style

Goettfried, Elisabeth, Katharina Thaler, Margarete Delazer, Demis Basso, Manuela Piazza, Michael Knoflach, and Laura Zamarian. 2025. "Arithmetic Proficiency Across Adulthood: Cognitive and Subjective Influences" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 15, no. 5: 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050084

APA Style

Goettfried, E., Thaler, K., Delazer, M., Basso, D., Piazza, M., Knoflach, M., & Zamarian, L. (2025). Arithmetic Proficiency Across Adulthood: Cognitive and Subjective Influences. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 15(5), 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050084

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop