How Do Children Play with Toy Trains and for What Benefits? A Scoping Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Aims of the Paper
2. Materials and Methods
3. Results
3.1. General Description of Studies
3.2. How Do Children Play with Toy Trains?
3.3. Children’s Social Play with Toy Trains
3.4. How Is Children’s Play with Toy Trains Associated with Their Development?
3.5. Children’s Preferences for Toy Trains
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions and Implications
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Complete Search Strategy
Search | Key Word (KW) or Subject Heading (SH) | Terms | Results |
#1 | KW | Play OR preten * OR game* OR toy OR recreation (all fields) | 230,463 |
#2 | SH | Recreation OR doll play OR toys OR childhood play behaviour OR games OR childhood play development OR playfulness | 27,222 |
#3 | KW | Train OR trains OR train set OR train track OR train play OR Thomas the tank engine OR Thomas and friends OR Hornby (all fields) | 18,855 |
#5 | SH | Railroad trains | 659 |
#6 | 1 OR 2 | 230,744 | |
#7 | 3 OR 5 | 18,855 | |
#8 | 6 AND 7 | 1215 | |
#9 | Limit 8 to childhood (birth to 12 years) or adolescence (13 to 17 years) | 254 | |
Note. For key words related to ‘Train’, the term ‘train *’ was not used as it resulted in many irrel-evant articles, largely related to the term ‘training’. * reflects truncation of the search term. |
Search | Key Word (KW) or Subject Heading (SH) | Terms | Results |
#1 | KW | “play” [all fields] OR “preten*” [all fields] OR “game*” [all fields] OR “toy” [all fields] OR “recreation” [all fields] | 902,704 |
#2 | SH/MeSH | “recreation” [MeSH terms] OR “play and playthings” [MeSH terms] | 239,869 |
#3 | KW | “train” [all fields] OR “trains” [all fields] OR “train set” [all fields] OR “train track” [all fields] OR “train play” [all fields] OR “Hornby” [all fields] | 59,758 |
#4 | 1 OR 2 | 1,103,474 | |
#5 | 4 AND 3 | 1215 | |
#6 | 5 Filters: child birth–18 years | 794 | |
Note. For key words related to ‘Train’, the term ‘train*’ was not used as it resulted in many irrele-vant articles, largely related to the term ‘training’. Subject headings/MeSH terms in PubMed were different to those used in PsycInfo due to database differences, and an analogous term for ‘railroad trains’ did not exist. * reflects truncation of the search term. |
Search | Key Word (KW) or Subject Heading (SH) | Terms | Results |
#1 | KW | Play OR preten* OR game* OR toy OR recreation (all fields) | 97,451 |
#2 | SH | Recreation OR toys OR dramatic play OR games | 17,256 |
#3 | KW | Train OR trains OR train set OR train track OR train play OR Thomas the tank engine OR Thomas and friends OR Hornby (all fields) | 10,099 |
#4 | 1 OR 2 | 97,451 | |
#5 | 4 AND 3 | 658 | |
Note. For key words related to ‘Train’, the term ‘train*’ was not used as it resulted in many irrele-vant articles, largely related to the term ‘training’. Subject headings in ERIC were different to those used in PsycInfo due to database differences, and an analogous term for ‘railroad trains’ did not exist. Additionally, there is no functionality to set a limit to childhood given the focus of the database on education. * reflects truncation of the search term. |
Search | Key Word (KW) or Subject Heading (SH) | Terms | Results |
#1 | KW | Play OR preten* OR game* OR toy OR recreation (all fields) | 13,928 |
#2 | SH | Play OR imaginative play OR symbolic play OR games | 1925 |
#3 | KW | Train OR trains OR train set OR train track OR train play OR Thomas the tank engine OR Thomas and friends OR Hornby (all fields) | 974 |
#4 | 1 OR 2 | 13,928 | |
#5 | 4 AND 3 | 62 | |
Note. For key words related to ‘Train’, the term ‘train*’ was not used as it resulted in many irrele-vant articles, largely related to the term ‘training’. Subject headings in the British Education In-dex were different to those used in PsycInfo due to database differences, and an analogous term for ‘railroad trains’ did not exist. Additionally, though there was functionality to set a limit to childhood; this removed many relevant papers and so was not used. |
Appendix B. Summary of Studies Included in the Review
Author(s) | Sample Information | Play Task, Measure, and Other Variables | Results in Relation to Train Play |
---|---|---|---|
Abuhatoum et al. (2020) [73] | n = 46 Age range: T1 focal child = 56.4 months (4.7 years); T2 focal child = 94.58 months (7.9 years) Gender: Mixed Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends Country: US | Children freely played with either a train, farm, or village set for 15 min at 2 time points. Conflict, power resources, and power effectiveness coded. | No significant differences in measures/variables according to play set, focal child gender, or gender composition. |
Alhaddad et al. (2019) [56] | n = 10 Age range: 7–10 years old Gender: All boys Sample with ASD diagnosis Design: Country: Qatar | Four 6 min long experiments video-recorded using different stimuli from the list below. Experiment 1 was a free play session to explore children’s preference for the first 5 toys below. Experiment 2 and 3 were to explore preferences for social humanoid robots and Thomas the Tank Engine trains, respectively. Experiment 4 was to explore if bubbles generated from a toy train increase its appeal. Play materials:
| In experiment 1, the plastic train was the 4th most preferred toy (out of 5). “Three [children] liked the features of the train, such as the colours and wheels”. In experiment 3, just over 46% of children showed interest to the Thomas the Tank Engine trains with 20% showing interest in the other train. “Children were more excited in this experiment”. Familiarity with the train played a role in making interactions fluid and spontaneous, with children re-enacting crashing scenes and producing sound effects of trains. In experiment 4, nearly all children (93.3%) preferred the train with bubbles over their previously most favoured train. The duration of interaction increased in terms of interacting with the bubbles. |
Besler & Kurt (2016) [47] | n = 3 Age range: 5–6 years old Gender: All boys Sample with ASD diagnosis Country: Turkey | Mothers trained and guided to produce a video modelling how to build a Lego train as a play skill. Children were then shown the video, and the children were assessed on whether they learned the play skill and maintained this. | Children were better able to build the Lego train after successive sessions of watching the training videos and one week after the last session. |
Chu et al. (2006) [41] | n = 140 Age range: 13–59 months Gender: 82 boys, 58 girls; 72 children with developmental delay Country: China | Symbolic play test (SPT; validated in Western cultures) was modified to be appropriate to a Chinese sample. In the SPT, children are presented with 4 different ‘situations’ of increasing difficulty in which an experimenter places toys in front of the child who is encouraged to play with them. Children’s behaviour is then scored according to how they play with the toy and whether it demonstrates some form of pretend play. In the modified version used in the present study, a tractor and trailer were replaced with a train and train track and cutlery was replaced with chopsticks to be more appropriate for Chinese children. | Children performed better (evidenced symbolic/pretend play) more when the ‘situation’ was modified to include a train instead of a tractor. |
Cordoni et al. (2016) [74] | n = 129 Age range: 3–5 years old (mean ages 38.75–63.07 months) Gender: 73 boys, 56 girls Country: Italy | Recordings made for 6 h a day, for 69 days in a Kindergarten which had toys available, including a train set. From the recordings, children’s behaviour (aggression, affiliation, play) was coded. | No distinct analyses by toy set or of train set. No gender differences in children’s aggression or who the target of aggression was; in groups of boys, there was more of a reliance on physical contacts than girls. Gender segregation in play was seen only in boys, regardless of age. |
Dalgin-Eyiip & Ulke-Kurkcuogluu (2021) [45] | n = 4 Age range: 5–8 years old Gender: 3 boys, 1 girl Sample with ASD diagnosis Country: Turkey | Videos were made whereby ‘peers’ modelled three pretend play skills (tea time play, hairdressing play, and railway train play). These videos were shown to the children who were reinforced with treats for watching it, and whether they engaged in the pretend play modelled was assessed. | Children’s pretend play skills/replication of the modelled actions increased following sessions where the videos were shown. |
DeLoache et al. (2007) [75] | n = 177 Age range: 11 months–6 years old (mean 35.1 months) Gender: 84 boys, 93 girls Country: US | Parents completed questionnaires and interviews in relation to their children’s ‘extremely intense interests’. | In total, 116 children were identified as having or having had one intense interest. These interests were more likely to be present in boys. Five of these children (all boys) had an extreme interest in trains. |
Desha et al. (2003) [42] | n = 24 Age range: 3.42–7.17 years old (M = 5.21 years old) Gender: 17 boys, 7 girls Sample with ASD diagnosis Country: Australia | Children videotaped for 15 min of unstructured play and 15 min of structured play.
| Of the play objects, the Thomas the Tank Engine train set, gross motor, and construction play objects were played with the most, more than expected by chance. No gender differences found in how often children played with the train set (or any other toys other than the dolls). |
Dominguez et al. (2006) [35] | n = 59 Children with diagnosis of ASD: n = 24; mean age = 5.42 years old; 17 boys, 7 girls Neurotypical children: n = 34, mean age = 4.58 years old; 16 boys, 18 girls. Country: Australia | The study formed part of a larger study that included Desha et al. [42] above. Methods were identical to Desha et al. [42] above, with the focus being a comparison between the ASD and neurotypical groups. | Children in the ASD group showed more exploratory (moving or turning it over in hands), sensorimotor (behaviours which do not take into account function of toy [e.g., banging, swinging]), and relational play (playing with two or more objects which does not take into account function of toy [e.g., piling objects, putting objects in a box]) than the neurotypical group. The difference in relation to sensorimotor play was present when looking specifically at the Thomas the Tank Engine train set. Children in the ASD group showed more of a preference for the Thomas the Tank Engine train set, gross motor toys, infant toys, dress-up accessories, actional figures, and plastic animals compared to the neurotypical group. |
Hobson et al. (2005) [76] | n = 32 10 mothers with borderline personality disorder (BPD) and 22 mothers with no history of psychiatric conditions Infant age: 47–58 weeks (mean = 54 weeks Infant gender: 16 boys, 16 girls Country: Unstated, likely UK | Mothers and their infants played together with a plastic toy train for 2 min. Maternal relatedness/sensitivity coded based on this interaction. | Mothers with BPD were more ‘intrusive insensitive’ (how much the mother’s actions cut across, took over or disrupted the infant’s activities) when playing with the train than mothers without BPD. |
Howe et al. (2022) [43] | n = 44 Age: 7 years old (M = 7.88; SD = 0.94 years old) Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 10 min. Play materials: Children were given a wooden village and train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Conversational turns and shared meaning strategies were coded from videos of the free play. Shared meaning strategies included introductions to play; simple maintenance strategies; semantic tying strategies; clarifications; responses to negotiation; prosocial behaviour; and disruptive behaviour. | When playing with siblings, children used more simple strategies (descriptions and imitations) and clarifications (agreement of ideas and sharing references) with the village set and more prosocial strategies (teaching/helping, social statements, shared affect) with the train set. When playing with friends, children used more simple strategies with the village set and used more introductions (suggesting play themes or calling attention), prosocial behaviour, and disruptive strategies (directives/control statements, negative behaviours, irrelevant behaviours to play) with the train set. No effects of gender composition or birth order were present in relation to playing with the train set. |
Howe et al. (1993) [46] | n = 100 Age range: 2.5–5 years old Gender: 60 boys, 40 girls Country: Unstated, likely Canada | All children played in one room organised into activity centres which were designed by early education students. These included: (1) a hospital office; (2) a bakery; (3) a pharmacy; (4) a pirate ship; (5) a pizzeria; (6) an airplane; (7) an animal hospital; (8) a train station; (9) a store; (10) a farm. Children’s play in these centres was categorised according to cognitive play (functional, constructive, dramatic, rule-based, exploratory); its social context (solitary, parallel, group); and other non-play behaviour. | Train themed ‘center’ was less familiar and less likely to elicit dramatic play. |
Howe et al. (2022b) [18] | n = 52 Age: 7 years old (M = 7.82; SD = 0.89 years old) Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 15 min. Play materials: Children were given a wooden village and train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Conversational turns were coded from videos as well as play scenarios, object use, and internal state language. Play scenarios were coded as set-up/organisation; expected; or creative. Object use was coded as set-up/organisation; expected use/transformation; creative use/transformation; or no objects used. Internal state language coded as references to cognitions; goals; emotions; and preferences. | In general, children engaged in set-up/organisation scenarios more than expected scenarios, and expected scenarios more than creative ones. In relation to the toy sets, children engaged in expected scenarios more with the train set than with the village set. In general, children used objects to set-up/organise the most and transformed them in creative ways the least. In relation to the toy sets, children engaged in expected object use more with the train set than with the village set. In general, children referred to goals more so than cognitions, followed by emotions and preferences which were not significantly different. In relation to the toy sets, children referred to goals more when playing with the train set than when playing with the village set. No effects of gender composition or birth order were present in relation to playing with the train set. |
Lamminmäki et al. (2012) [59] | n = 47 Age: 14 months Gender: 21 boys, 26 girls Country: Finland | Toy preference test from 9 toys that were considered as female-preferred (a tea set, a soft doll, a baby doll with a bathtub), male-preferred (a truck, a train, and a parking toy with motorbikes), or neutral (a teddy bear, a soft picture book, and a set of keys). Children played freely with the toys for 8–10 min. Time during which child played with each toy was calculated. | Boys played more with the train compared to the girls; girls played more with the baby doll than the boys. No other gender difference was present for the time spent with other toys. Testosterone levels positively associated with playing with the train in the girls, but not the boys. |
Le Maner-Idrissi (1996; Experiment 1) [60] | n = 24 Age: 24 months Gender: 12 boys, 12 girls All oldest child or only child Country: Unstated, likely France | Two children of the same gender were brought into one room for 20 min and presented with stereotypically male toys (a train, a pistol, and a workbench); female toys (a baby doll, a vanity, and a tea set); and neutral toys (a phone, a ball, and a farm). Children’s choice of objects and imitative behaviour was recorded. | Children’s toy preferences were sex-stereotyped. |
Le Maner-Idrissi (1996; Experiment 2) [60] | n = 24 Age: 24 months Gender: 12 boys, 12 girls All oldest child or only child Country: Unstated, likely France | Two children of mixed gender were brought into one room for 20 min and presented with stereotypically male toys (a train, a pistol, and a workbench); female toys (a baby doll, a vanity, and a tea set); and neutral toys (a phone, a ball, and a farm). Children’s choice of objects and imitative behaviour was recorded. | Girls in the presence of a boy preferred the female toys to male and neutral toys, but boys’ preferences depended on the composition of the dyad–when with boys they preferred the male toys, but in the presence of a girl, they no longer exhibited their preferences. Boys also chose significantly more female toys when with another girl than another boy (based on experiments 1 & 2). |
Le Maner-Idrissi (1996; Experiment 3) [60] | n = 24 Age: 24 months Gender: 12 boys, 12 girls All oldest child or only child Country: Unstated, likely France | Two children of the same gender were brought into the room and presented with male toys (a robot, a pistol, a garage, a helicopter, a jeep and its trailer, 4 small cars, a workbench, a construction game, and a train) first and then a week later only the female toys (a doll, a baby basket, a stroller, a baby doll with a bottle, 4 pieces of jewellery, a vanity, a tea set, and a market stand). Children’s choice of objects and imitative behaviour was recorded. | Girls chose significantly more female toys than male toys when they were presented separately, but the boys did not show a preference. |
Le Maner-Idrissi & Renault (2006) [61] | n = 48 Age range: 34–52 months Gender: 24 boys, 24 girls All children knew each other Country: Unstated, likely France | Two children of mixed gender were brought into one room for 20 min and presented with stereotypically male toys (a train, a pistol, and a workbench); female toys (a baby doll, a vanity, and a tea set); and neutral toys (a phone, a ball, and a farm). Children’s choice of objects and interactions (solitary, parallel, or interactive play) was recorded. | Three-year-old boys prefer male toys to either the female or neutral ones, but three-year-old girls did not prefer the male toys over female ones. Four-year-old boys preferred male and neutral toys over the female ones, and four-year-old girls preferred male toys over female toys. |
Leach et al. (2015) [58] | n = 65 Age: 56.4 months Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 15 min. Play materials: At time 1, children were given either a wooden village, farm, or train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. At time 2, children were given either the village set or farm set but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Play sessions were coded for the number of conversational turns and internal state language. | No analyses by play set. Children referred to cognitions more at time 2 than time 1, specifically with their siblings, and were more likely to refer to shared internal states at time 2. Children talked about shared goals and their own cognitions more at time 2 than time 1, and children talked about emotions about the toys more at time 1 than time 2. At time 1, children with an older sibling talked more about goals and cognitions than children playing with a younger sibling. |
Leach et al. (2019) [50] | Time 1 n = 65 Time 2 n = 46 Time 1 age = 56 months Time 2 age = 94.58 months Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 15 min. Play materials: At time 1, children were given either a wooden village, farm, or train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. At time 2, children were given either the village set or farm set but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Play sessions were coded for the number of conversational turns and the ways in which children constructed shared meanings in their play | No analyses by play set. Children used more positive shared-meaning strategies with friends compared to siblings and more introductions with siblings than friends. Children used more simple strategies, building of ideas, and prosocial strategies at time 1 compared to time 2. Specifically, when children were using a play voice, children used more simple strategies at time 2 and more clarifications at time 1. |
Leach et al. (2019) [51] | Time 1 n = 44 Time 2 n = 46 Time 1 age = 56.4 months Time 2 age = 96.77 months Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 15 min. Play materials: At time 1, children were given either a wooden village, farm, or train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. At time 2, children were given either the village set or farm set but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Play sessions were coded for the number of conversational turns, the ‘connectedness’ of children’s conversations, the quality of interactions, and the emotional tone of the children’s conversations. | No differences found in outcome measures in relation to the play set, therefore this was not analysed separately. Children were more cooperative at time 2, and children were more cooperative with friends than with siblings. Children engaged in long sequences of connectedness with friends than with siblings and were more likely to engage in short sequences with siblings than with friends. Children showed a more positive tone with friends compared to siblings, and a more negative tone with siblings than with friends. |
Leach et al. (2022) [52] | Time 1 n = 65 Time 2 n = 46 Time 1 age = 56 months Time 2 age = 94.58 months Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 15 min. Play materials: At time 1, children were given either a wooden village, farm, or train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. At time 2, children were given either the village set or farm set but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Play sessions were coded for the number of conversational turns and the ‘connectedness’ of children’s conversations. | No differences found in outcome measures in relation to the play set, therefore this was not analysed separately. Children made more failed attempts at establishing connectedness and engaged in more self-talk when playing with siblings than friends, and they maintained connectedness more with friends than siblings. At time 1, children ended connected interactions more often than their siblings, and siblings engaged in more self-talk and unclear statements than at time 2 only. The balance of participation did not differ between children and friends at either time 1 or time 2. |
Leach et al. (2022) [53] | Time 1 n = 65 Time 2 n = 46 Time 1 age = 56 months Time 2 age = 94.58 months Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 15 min. Play materials: At time 1, children were given either a wooden village, farm, or train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. At time 2, children were given either the village set or farm set but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Play sessions were coded for the number of conversational turns, the strategies used to create shared meanings, and the ‘connectedness’ of children’s conversations. | No analyses by play set. Children used simple strategies most often to initiate and sustain connectedness. Children engaged in prosocial behaviour and used the play voice when initiating connectedness with their friends more than with siblings. Children used clarifications when sustaining connectedness more often with siblings than friends. |
Murphy et al. (1986) [77] | n = 20 Age: 14.5 years old Gender: 13 boys, 7 girls. All children with developmental delays Country: Unstated, likely UK | Three 5 min observations of children playing with a panda, a car, and a train separately. Children then experienced control and experimental sessions (randomised and counterbalanced). Both conditions consisted of 5 sessions with each toy and children were shown how the toy works, and in the experimental condition, children were encouraged to interact with it. Children’s contact with the toys measured. | Children showed more contact with the car and the train in the experimental condition, but not for the panda. Modelling of play behaviour facilitated children’s play. |
Neisworth et al. (2002) [78] | n = 4 Age: 3–6 years old Gender: All boys Sample with diagnosis of ASD Country: Unstated, likely US | Videos were created of the children demonstrating ‘spontaneous requesting’ (asking for an object, action, or help) after being trained to do so, from a 30 min play session in the home. Children chose what toys to play with (which included a Thomas the Tank Engine toy set). Children then watched these videos once a day for 5 days, and were observed in the school setting for the target behaviour of spontaneous helping. | Children’s spontaneous requesting increased from baseline after the intervention, and was maintained afterwards. |
O’Bleness (2016) [57] | n = 155 Time 1 age = 30.68 months Time 2 age = 33.16 months Gender: 75 girls, 80 boys 74 mother–child dyads were in the play-as-usual group and 81 mother–child dyads were in the experimental group. Country: US | Children and their mothers played together for 10 min at time 1 and time 2 with an assortment of toys, including a train set. In between these sessions, all dyads completed eight play sessions (4 at home, 4 in the lab), once a week for 20 min. The train set was included during some of these. In addition, the two groups completed a training session: for the control group, they were asked to play with their child as they usually would on a daily basis; for the experimental group, mothers were instructed to learn and engage in the child’s game (child-led play that was rewarded). Children’s language production coded from the play sessions at time 1 and time 2. | No analyses by play set. Children and mothers’ language associated from time 1 to time 2. Mothers’ language production decreased from time 1 to time 2, but children’s language production increased from time 1 to time 2 (and was particularly marked for children with the lowest language production at time 1). |
Paine et al. (2021) [48] | Time 1 n = 65 Time 2 n = 46 Time 1 age = 56 months Time 2 age = 94.58 months Children played with older and younger siblings; mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free-played for up to 15 min. Play materials: At time 1, children were given either a wooden village, farm, or train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. At time 2, children were given either the village set or farm set but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Play sessions were coded for the presence of humour in children’s play. | No analyses by play set. Humour did not differ according to relationship (friends vs. siblings) or from time 1 to time 2. Children’s production of humour with sibling at time 1 was associated with humour production with a friend, both at time 1 and time 2. Children playing with an older sibling produced more humour with their older sibling than children playing with their younger sibling. |
Paine et al. (2019) [49] | n = 86 Age: 7.82 years old Children played with older or younger siblings; 31 mixed gender compositions and 55 same gender compositions. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home with sibling or friend free-playing for 15 min. Play materials: Children either played with village set (n = 42) or train set (n = 44). Play sessions were coded for conversational turns and the presence of humour in children’s play. | More humour produced when playing with the village toys compared to the train set. The production of humour was dependent on the other sibling/partner in the play session. Humour differed according to the age and gender composition of the children.) In general, the boys produced more humour than the girls. |
Parten (1933) [54] | n = 34 Age: Pre-school age Country: US | Children observed daily for one minute each during morning free play in pre-school. Records made of the play activity and the number and characteristics of children in each group. | Of the 11 most popular activities reported, playing with train toys was the 3rd most popular. There were no clear age-related trends for train play, but it was more popular with boys. Younger children tended to play with the trains on their own, whereas for older children, train play was a social activity involving building the tracks or stations with blocks. |
Petrakos & Howe (1996) [44] | n = 31 Age range: 43–64 months Gender: 18 boys, 13 girls Country: Unstated, likely Canada | Children were semi-randomly assigned to groups of four but matched on dramatic play abilities and peer familiarity. Each group entered the dramatic play centre in groups of four and played for 10 min in each dramatic play centre. For the intervention, four centres were provided: (1) extended housekeeping; (2) train station which were set up to promote either solitary or group interactions. Children’s play categorised according to type of play (functional, constructive, dramatic) and degree of sociality (solitary, parallel, group, onlooker, unoccupied). | The social/solitary designs of the centres promoted the social/solitary type of play, respectively. Children engaged in role play that was theme-related and consistent with the theme of the centre. |
Ritter-Brinton & Beattie (1994) [79] | n = 1 Age: 5 years old Gender: Not stated Deaf child of hearing parents Country: Unstated, likely Canada | Eight 15 min play sessions, alternating between a train set and a doctor role play kit. Focal child played with two same-aged male peers who were also deaf. Different types of play behaviour were recorded: initiation, maintenance, shift, and termination. | No explicit analyses of differences between toys, but data are presented separately. However, children showed more play behaviours for the doctor role play kit than the train set. Further, the child showed similar play behaviours for all categories other than termination (ending a play sequence), which were less for the train set as compared to the doctor role play kit. |
Roggman (1989) [55] | n = 108 Age: 36 10-month-olds; 36 15-month-olds; 36 29-month-olds Gender: 10-month-old group = 13 boys, 23 girls; 15-month-old group = 17 boys and 19 girls; 29-month-old group = 13 boys and 23 girls Country: Unstated, likely US | Children played with two toy trains—one that was non-social in that it could be played with alone, and the other was social in that it required adult help (an object needed to move the train was out of reach). Both trains were presented to each child, but the position (on the left or right) was counterbalanced. Toy preference was measured according to the time spent looking at each train. | The youngest and oldest group did not show a preference for either toy, but the 15-month-olds showed a preference for the ‘social’ train compared to the ‘non-social’ train. |
Spektor-Levy et al. (2017) [80] | n = 106 Age range: 5–10 years old Gender: 52 girls, 54 boys. Country: UK | Children completed problem-solving tasks that involved building a train track to match a pre-set shape or building a LEGO model according to a pre-set plan. Children’s private (self-directed) speech and private (self-directed) gestures coded. | Children’s self-directed speech positively associated with age for both tasks. Aspects of children’s self-directed speech were correlated across tasks, but only for the boys. |
Tavassoli et al. (2020) [81] | Time 1 n = 63 Time 2 n = 44 Time 1 age = 4.56 years old; Time 2 age = 8.06 years old Children played with older and younger siblings, mixed gender compositions as well as with friends of the same age and gender. Country: US | Children videotaped in the home either with sibling or friend (counterbalanced) 1 week apart. Children free played for up to 15 min. Play materials: At time 1, children were given either a wooden village, farm, or train set (including tracks, trains, bridge, crane, people, boats), but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. At time 2, children were given either the village set or farm set but whether this was with the sibling or friend was counterbalanced, as was the order of presentation. Play sessions were coded for how children responded to their play partner’s request for help. | No analyses by play set. Children were more likely to refuse to be prosocial with siblings compared to friends. The ways in which children refused to be prosocial differed according to age (i.e., from time 1 to time 2) and according to the request being made. |
Trawick-Smith et al. (2015) [9] | n = 60 Age: 48.8 months Gender: 32 girls, 28 boys Country: US | Nine toys were placed in the classroom for a 20 min video recorded free play. The play of children who chose to play with the toy was recorded. Toys included: wooden train set; Bristle blocks; Duplo bricks; Lincoln logs; Measure up! cups; Rainbow people; Castle bucket set; Shape, model and mould; tree blocks. Play assessed in terms of ‘quality’—whether the play evidenced: (1) thinking and learning; (2) problem solving; (3) curiosity and inquiry; (4) sustained interest; (5) creative expression; (6) symbolic transformation; (7) interactions with peers; (8) autonomous play. | Train set not amongst the highest for play quality, but also not the lowest. No differences according to gender, children from low SES backgrounds showed higher ‘play quality’ with trains compared to other toys, but then no difference in quality between those from higher or lower SES backgrounds. |
References
- Lillard, A.S. The development of play. In Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science; Lerner, R.M., Ed.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ofcom. Children and Parents: Media Use and Attitudes. 2019. Available online: https://www.ofcom.org.uk/data/assets/pdf_file/0024/234609/childrens-media-use-and-attitudes-report-2022.pdf (accessed on 1 March 2023).
- Downey, S.; Hayes, N.; O’Neill, B. Play and Technology for Children Aged 4–12; Centre for Social and Educational Research, Dublin Institute of Technology, Office of the Minister for Children: Dublin, Ireland, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Case-Smith, J.; Kuhaneck, H.M. Play preferences of typically developing children and children with developmental delays between ages 3 and 7 years. OTJR Occup. Particip. Health 2008, 28, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hashmi, S. Seven-Year-Olds’ Imaginative Engagement with Play in Non-Virtual and Virtual Contexts. Doctoral Dissertation, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, D.; MacKeith, S.A. The Development of Imagination: The Private Worlds of Childhood; Routledge: London, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Fein, G.G. Pretend play in childhood: An integrative review. Child Dev. 1981, 52, 1095–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singer, D.G.; Singer, J.L. The House of Make-Believe: Children’s Play and Developing Imagination; Harvard University Press: London, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Trawick-Smith, J.; Wolff, J.; Koschel, M.; Vallarelli, J. Effects of toys on the play quality of preschool children: Influence of gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Early Child. Educ. J. 2015, 43, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giffin, H. The coordination of meaning in the creation of a shared make-believe reality. In Symbolic Play: The Development of Social Understanding; Bretherton, I., Ed.; Academic Press, Inc.: Orlando, FL, USA, 1984; pp. 73–100. [Google Scholar]
- Engel, S. The narrative worlds of what is and what if. Cogn. Dev. 2005, 20, 514–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howe, N.; Petrakos, H.; Rinaldi, C.M. “All the sheeps are dead. He murdered them”: Sibling pretense, negotiation, internal state language, and relationship quality. Child Dev. 1998, 69, 182–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howe, N.; Abuhatoum, S.; Chang-Kredl, S. “Everything’s upside down. We’ll call it upside down valley!”: Siblings’ creative play themes, object use, and language during pretend play. Early Educ. Dev. 2014, 25, 381–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gibson, J.J. The theory of affordances. In The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception; Gibson, J.J., Ed.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 1979; pp. 119–136. [Google Scholar]
- Hutt, C. Specific and diversive exploration. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 1970, 5, 119–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singer, D.G.; Singer, J.L. Imagination and Play in the Electronic Age; Harvard University Press: London, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Møller, S.J. Imagination, playfulness, and creativity in children’s play with different toys. Am. J. Play. 2015, 7, 322. [Google Scholar]
- Howe, N.; Tavassoli, N.; Leach, J.; Farhat, F.; Dehart, G. “This is a pit of fire”: Associations of play materials with children’s creativity during play and internal state language. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2022, 36, 64–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paine, A.L.; Hashmi, S.; Howe, N.; Johnson, N.; Scott, M.; Hay, D.F. “A pirate goes nee-nor-nee-nor!” humor with siblings in middle childhood: A window to social understanding? Dev. Psychol. 2022, 58, 1986–1998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McLoyd, V.C. The effects of the structure of play objects on the pretend play of low-income preschool children. Child Dev. 1983, 54, 626–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trawick-Smith, J. The effects of realistic versus non-realistic play materials on young children’s symbolic transformation of objects. J. Res. Child. Educ. 1990, 5, 27–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lillard, A.S.; Lerner, M.D.; Hopkins, E.J.; Dore, R.A.; Smith, E.D.; Palmquist, C.M. The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hashmi, S.; Vanderwert, R.E.; Price, H.A.; Gerson, S.A. Exploring the benefits of doll play through neuroscience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 560176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hashmi, S.; Paine, A.L.; Hay, D.F. Seven-year-olds’ references to internal states when playing with toy figures and a video game. Infant Child Dev. 2021, 30, e2223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hashmi, S.; Vanderwert, R.E.; Paine, A.L.; Gerson, S.A. Doll play prompts social thinking and social talking: Representations of internal state language in the brain. Dev. Sci. 2022, 25, e13163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubin, K.H.; Howe, N. Toys and play behaviors: An overview. Top. Early Child. Spec. Educ. 1985, 5, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferrara, K.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Newcombe, N.S.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Lam, W.S. Block talk: Spatial language during block play. Mind Brain Educ. 2011, 5, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanline, M.F.; Milton, S.; Phelps, P.C. The relationship between preschool block play and reading and maths abilities in early elementary school: A longitudinal study of children with and without disabilities. Early Child Dev. Care 2010, 180, 1005–1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parsons, A.; Howe, N. Superhero toys and boys’ physically active and imaginative play. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2006, 20, 287–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parsons, A.; Howe, N. “This Is Spiderman’s Mask”. “No, It’s Green Goblin’s”: Shared Meanings During Boys’ Pretend Play With Superhero and Generic Toys. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2013, 27, 190–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruble, D.N.; Martin, C.L.; Berenbaum, S.A. Gender development. In Handbook of Child Psychology, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development; Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eisenberg, N., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006; pp. 858–932. [Google Scholar]
- Matthews, W.S. Modes of transformation in the initiation of fantasy play. Dev. Psychol. 1977, 13, 212–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, D.; Grollman, S.H. Ways of playing: Individual differences in imaginative style. In The Play of Children: Current Theory and Research; Damon, J.A., Ed.; S. Karger AG: Buffalo, NY, USA, 1982; pp. 46–63. [Google Scholar]
- Leaf, J.B.; Oppenheim-Leaf, M.L.; Leaf, R.; Courtemanche, A.B.; Taubman, M.; McEachin, J.; Sheldon, J.B.; Sherman, J.A. Observational effects on the preferences of children with autism. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2012, 45, 473–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dominguez, A.; Ziviani, J.; Rodger, S. Play behaviours and play object preferences of young children with autistic disorder in a clinical play environment. Autism 2006, 10, 53–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- González-Sala, F.; Gómez-Marí, I.; Tárraga-Mínguez, R.; Vicente-Carvajal, A.; Pastor-Cerezuela, G. Symbolic play among children with autism spectrum disorder: A scoping review. Children 2021, 8, 801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Libby, S.; Powell, S.; Messer, D.; Jordan, R. Spontaneous play in children with autism: A reappraisal. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1998, 28, 487–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munn, Z.; Peters, M.D.; Stern, C.; Tufanaru, C.; McArthur, A.; Aromataris, E. Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2018, 18, 143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; Moher, D.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amodia-Bidakowska, A.; Laverty, C.; Ramchandani, P.G. Father-child play: A systematic review of its frequency, characteristics and potential impact on children’s development. Dev. Rev. 2020, 57, 100924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chu, M.M.Y.; Lee, W.C.; Leung, J.L.S.; Wong, V. Modified symbolic play test for Oriental children. Pediatr. Int. 2006, 48, 519–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Desha, L.; Ziviani, J.; Rodger, S. Play preferences and behavior of preschool children with autistic spectrum disorder in the clinical environment. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2003, 23, 21–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Howe, N.; Leach, J.; DeHart, G. “This is a mailbox, right?”: Associations of play materials with siblings’ and friends’ shared meanings during pretend play. J. Early Child. Res. 2022, 20, 80–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petrakos, H.; Howe, N. The influence of the physical design of the dramatic play center on children’s play. Early Child. Res. Q. 1996, 11, 63–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalgin-Eyiip, O.; Ulke-Kurkcuoglu, B. Video enhanced activity schedules: Teaching schedule following and pretend play skills to children with ASD. Educ. Train. Autism Dev. Disabil. 2021, 56, 205–224. [Google Scholar]
- Howe, N.; Moller, L.; Chambers, B.; Petrakos, H. The ecology of dramatic play centers and children’s social and cognitive play. Early Child. Res. Q. 1993, 8, 235–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Besler, F.; Kurt, O. Effectiveness of video modeling provided by mothers in teaching play skills to children with autism. Kuram Uygulamada Egit. Bilim. Educ. Sci. Theory Pract. 2016, 16, 209–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paine, A.L.; Howe, N.; Gilmore, V.; Karajian, G.; DeHart, G. “Goosebump man. That’s funny!”: Humor with siblings and friends from early to middle childhood. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2021, 77, 101321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paine, A.L.; Howe, N.; Karajian, G.; Hay, D.F.; DeHart, G. ‘H, I, J, K, L, M, N, O, PEE! Get it? Pee!’: Siblings’ shared humour in childhood. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2019, 37, 336–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, J.; Howe, N.; DeHart, G. “Let’s make a place where giants live!”: Children’s communication during play with siblings and friends from early to middle childhood. Infant Child Dev. 2019, 28, e2156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, J.; Howe, N.; DeHart, G. A Longitudinal Investigation of Siblings’ and Friends’ Features of Connectedness and Interaction Quality During Play. Early Educ. Dev. 2019, 30, 709–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, J.; Howe, N.; DeHart, G. Children’s connectedness with siblings and friends from early to middle childhood during play. Early Educ. Dev. 2022, 33, 1289–1303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, J.; Howe, N.; DeHart, G. Children’s connectedness and shared meanings strategies during play with siblings and friends. Infant Child Dev. 2022, 31, e2365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parten, M.B. Social play among preschool children. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1933, 28, 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roggman, L.A. Age Differences in the Goals of Toddler Play. In Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, MO, USA, 27–30 April 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Alhaddad, A.Y.; Javed, H.; Connor, O.; Banire, B.; Al Thani, D.; Cabibihan, J.J. Robotic trains as an educational and therapeutic tool for autism spectrum disorder intervention. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Robotics and Education RiE 2017, Sofia, Bulgaria, 26–28 April 2017; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; pp. 249–262. [Google Scholar]
- O’Bleness, J. Play-based intervention effects on language production in toddlers from a high-risk background. Diss. Abstr. Int. Sect. B Sci. Eng. 2016, 77, 3726921. [Google Scholar]
- Leach, J.; Howe, N.; Dehart, G. ‘An Earthquake Shocked Up the Land!’ Children’s Communication During Play With Siblings and Friends. Soc. Dev. 2015, 24, 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamminmäki, A.; Hines, M.; Kuiri-Hänninen, T.; Kilpeläinen, L.; Dunkel, L.; Sankilampi, U. Testosterone measured in infancy predicts subsequent sex-typed behavior in boys and in girls. Horm. Behav. 2012, 61, 611–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Le Maner-Idrissi, G. An internal gender system at 24 months. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 1996, 11, 301–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Le Maner-Idrissi, G.; Renault, L. Developpement du “schema de genre”: Une asymetrie entre filles et garcons? Enfance 2006, 58, 251–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albertson, K.; Shore, C. Holding in mind conflicting information: Pretending, working memory, and executive control. J. Cogn. Dev. 2009, 9, 390–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelly, R.; Dissanayake, C.; Ihsen, E.; Hammond, S. The relationship between symbolic play and executive function in young children. Australas. J. Early Child. 2011, 36, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russ, S.W.; Wallace, C.E. Pretend play and creative processes. Am. J. Play. 2013, 6, 136–148. [Google Scholar]
- Sachet, A.B.; Mottweiler, C.M. The distinction between role-play and object substitution in pretend play. In The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Imagination; Taylor, M., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Sansanwal, S. Pretend play enhances creativity and imagination. J. Arts Humanit. 2014, 3, 70–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thibodeau, R.B.; Gilpin, A.T.; Brown, M.M.; Meyer, B.A. The effects of fantastical pretend-play on the development of executive functions: An intervention study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2016, 145, 120–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunn, J.; Cutting, A.L. Understanding others, and individual differences in friendship interactions in young children. Soc. Dev. 1999, 8, 201–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, P.L. The Work of the Imagination; Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, M.M.; Thibodeau, R.B.; Pierucci, J.M.; Gilpin, A.T. Supporting the development of empathy: The role of theory of mind and fantasy orientation. Soc. Dev. 2017, 26, 951–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianco, F.; Lecce, S.; Banerjee, R. Conversations about mental states and theory of mind development during middle childhood: A training study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2016, 149, 41–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nath, S.; Szücs, D. Construction play and cognitive skills associated with the development of mathematical abilities in 7-year-old children. Learn. Instr. 2014, 32, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abuhatoum, S.; Della Porta, S.; Howe, N.; DeHart, G. A longitudinal examination of power in sibling and friend conflict. Soc. Dev. 2020, 29, 903–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cordoni, G.; Demuru, E.; Ceccarelli, E.; Palagi, E. Play, aggressive conflict and reconciliation in pre-school children: What matters? Behaviour 2016, 153, 1075–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeLoache, J.S.; Simcock, G.; Macari, S. Planes, trains, automobiles—And tea sets: Extremely intense interests in very young children. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 1579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hobson, R.P.; Patrick, M.; Crandell, L.; García-Pérez, R.O.S.A.; Lee, A. Personal relatedness and attachment in infants of mothers with borderline personality disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 2005, 17, 329–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Murphy, G.; Carr, J.; Callias, M. Increasing simple toy play in profoundly mentally handicapped children: II. Designing special toys. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1986, 16, 45–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Neisworth, J.T.; Wert, B.Y. Videotaped Self-Modeling as a Technique for Training Preschoolers with Autism in Social-Communicative Functioning; The Pennsylvania State University: State College, PA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Ritter-Brinton, K.; Beattie, R. Play stategies of a signing deaf preschooler. Rev. Can. Readapt. 1994, 7, 209–216. [Google Scholar]
- Spektor-Levy, O.; Basilio, M.; Zachariou, A.; Whitebread, D. Young Children’s Spontaneous Manifestation of Self-Regulation and Metacognition during Constructional Play Tasks. Teach. Coll. Rec. 2017, 119, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tavassoli, N.; Howe, N.; DeHart, G. Investigating the development of prosociality through the lens of refusals: Children’s prosocial refusals with siblings and friends. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2020, 66, 421–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Hashmi, S. How Do Children Play with Toy Trains and for What Benefits? A Scoping Review. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2023, 13, 2112-2134. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13100149
Hashmi S. How Do Children Play with Toy Trains and for What Benefits? A Scoping Review. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2023; 13(10):2112-2134. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13100149
Chicago/Turabian StyleHashmi, Salim. 2023. "How Do Children Play with Toy Trains and for What Benefits? A Scoping Review" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 13, no. 10: 2112-2134. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13100149
APA StyleHashmi, S. (2023). How Do Children Play with Toy Trains and for What Benefits? A Scoping Review. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 13(10), 2112-2134. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13100149