1. Introduction
ESG messaging has emerged as the dominant trend in modern marketing, as companies increasingly emerge as ethically responsible stakeholders to appeal to consumers who are concerned about sustainability (
De Freitas Netto et al. 2020;
Ktisti et al. 2022). Across all mainstream and digital media, ESG messaging has reached saturation point from green product branding to corporate social media efforts. But it has also created opportunities for greenwashing—the tendency to deceive or overstate claims of environmental or social responsibility for the sake of brand reputation, not any actual environmental or social benefit (
Lima et al. 2024;
Raghunandan and Rajgopal 2022).
Recent studies have shown that greenwashing not only occurs in the form of clearly deceptive claims but also, less obviously, using evasive terms, unverifiable claims regarding the environment and influencer sponsorship with inadequate disclosure (
Raghunandan and Rajgopal 2022;
Santos et al. 2024). More than half of all green statements within the European Union are deemed to be misleading or unsubstantiated. The problem is particularly acute in online advertising spaces, where influencer marketing, emotive storytelling, and platform-based nudges can potentially blur the distinction between substantive ESG action and performative signaling (
Santos et al. 2024;
Yang et al. 2020). The risks are heightened in areas like educational technology (EdTech) and e-commerce, where sustainability messaging typically reaches consumers via algorithms, pop-ups, and curated content—with neither open accountability nor verification.
Greenwashing is highly perilous, individually as well as cumulatively. For consumers, misleading ESG advertising dissolves trust, triggers confusion, and distorts decision-making. Empirical research has established perceived greenwashing with lower brand trust, lower product satisfaction, and lower purchase intentions. At the macro level, it refutes the overall credibility of green advertising and risks diluting the effectiveness of truly sustainable practice (
Akram et al. 2024;
Borah et al. 2024). Apart from reputational risk, greenwashing is now a material business risk, attracting the attention of investors, regulators, and activist groups. Regulatory frameworks like the European Green Claims Directive are indicative of an increasing regulatory imperative for substantiable ESG communications. Economically, greenwashing entities risk not just reputational harm but also decreased investor trust, higher cost of capital, and loss of brand value—particularly where ESG performance is touted as a differentiator (
Borah et al. 2024;
Causevic et al. 2022).
Young consumers, especially Generation Z and younger millennials, are the most significant audience for digital ESG communication and are increasingly cynical about its veracity (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Das et al. 2025). They care about sustainability and social justice and habitually call brands to account for ethical dissonance. But their frequent exposure to online content and emotive appeals with little ability to scrutinize advanced ESG claims puts them at risk of profoundly sophisticated greenwashing tactics. While Gen Z is more ad- and persuasion-savvy, studies have established that even extremely skeptical instances are vulnerable to source credibility, emotional framing, and implicit cues in persuasive online settings. Ironically, media-skeptical skepticism is not always a cure for manipulative influence—particularly when messages are from credentialed sources or sites, or environmental claims are presented in sweet-tasting packages but have hollow meaning (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Das et al. 2025).
Theoretically, how young consumers react to ESG-labeled web ads must be responded to by an integration of strategies like the Persuasion Knowledge Model, Theory of Planned Behavior, and source credibility models (
Fella and Bausa 2024;
Dangelico et al. 2024;
Chwialkowska et al. 2024). These conclude that green marketing effects do not solely lie with message content but also cognitive processing (e.g., detection of intent to persuade), emotional experience, perceived source trustworthiness, and context-specific digital design features. In addition, variables like perceived greenwashing, ad skepticism, ESG trust, and digital ad literacy not only act as antecedents but also as mediators of behavioral intentions, influencing how consumers process, judge, and respond to communications of sustainability (
De Freitas Netto et al. 2020;
Dangelico et al. 2024;
Chwialkowska et al. 2024). As the literature continues to grow, it gravitates towards complex, multi-path models that reflect the psychological richness of digital persuasion—particularly that of younger consumers who must negotiate a dense and morally complex ad environment (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Díaz et al. 2024;
Fang 2024).
In spite of an increasing body of research on greenwashing and electronic persuasion, not much empirical research exists that synthesizes these fields into one area of study—in the case of ESG-labeled marketing to children and young people (
Ktisti et al. 2022;
Fehr 2023;
Huang et al. 2024;
Le et al. 2024). Most of the existing research studies investigate greenwashing and persuasion literacy as distinct constructs with little effort towards creating holistic behavioral theories that explain youth cognition and emotion about marketing targeting sustainability. Although previous research has proven greenwashing to undermine trust and lower purchase intentions, no overarching models have been developed that are focused on the interaction among perceived greenwashing, persuasion knowledge, advertisement skepticism, source credibility, and behavior intentions in web contexts (
Le et al. 2024;
Meet et al. 2024;
Nguyen-Viet and Thanh Tran 2024).
In addition, the majority of recent studies have focused on business segments like fast fashion, food, and hospitality and the practice of green marketing in these fields with minimal attention to other areas of business like educational technology (EdTech), where ESG disclosure is becoming deeply integrated into learning infrastructures and influencer marketing-driven content. This overlooks a growing business landscape where brand communication is colliding with learning interaction (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Gregory 2024). Furthermore, there has been limited academic study of Generation Z—a generation that not only suffers from disproportionate exposure to digital ESG messaging but one also that exhibits distinctive modes of idealism, cynicism, and media critical thinking. The failure to deeply study the complex reactions of this generation is a systematic flaw, especially as they become leading decision-makers and formulators of emerging consumer standards (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Gregory 2024). This research fills a crucial blind spot in this nascent debate through empirical modeling of the psychological processes by which young Greek consumers assess and react to ESG-branded web ads. Centering on industries like EdTech and retail—where ESG content goes increasingly mainstream but sometimes poorly regulated—research examines how perceived greenwashing, credibility of the source, and literacy with the ad interact to affect skepticism, knowledge of persuasion, and buying intention. By focusing on Gen Z in a culturally homogenous sample of a country, the study strives to generalize important cognitive and affective determinants of ESG ad evaluation, thus achieving theoretical and practical implications to ethical marketing, regulation, and consumer protection (
Ktisti et al. 2022;
Huang et al. 2024).
The Greek context is a first-rate setting where one can see how younger consumers react towards ESG-labeled ads, particularly in online areas such as EdTech and online shopping (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Díaz et al. 2024;
Fang 2024). Greece has a unique set of sociopolitical, economic, and cultural circumstances that determine reception, evaluation, and reaction to ESG messages. While on the one hand, young consumers’ online activity is significant and Gen Z is defined by strong media literacy, pervasive exposure to social network influencers, and familiarity with target areas of advertising, on the other hand, at the European level, Greece is lower in institutional trust, particularly as it concerns corporate communications and the media (
Fella and Bausa 2024;
Dangelico et al. 2024;
Chwialkowska et al. 2024). This deficit of trust, added to increased economic exposure and recent political unrest, renders a population susceptible to appeals of social justice but wary of insincere messages. Greek consumers, particularly young people, are thus well placed to identify contradiction in ESG messages and are thus perfect subjects with which to test how skepticism and awareness of persuasion respond to sustainability claims, whereas in highly regulated and well-established ESG systems of markets, Greece’s fragmented sustainability story can fuel greater skepticism—most notably on the part of digitally literate youth filtering through ambiguous or inflated ESG messaging (
Le et al. 2024;
Meet et al. 2024;
Nguyen-Viet and Thanh Tran 2024). Meanwhile, amidst the economically struggling but socially aware Gen Z of Greece lies an idealism that is combined with critical thinking to make them all the more receptive to the gap between brand messaging and perceived authenticity (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Gregory 2024). This duality renders the Greek sample extremely relevant in testing the psychological interplay of greenwashing, persuasion knowledge, and advertising skepticism in ESG communication. However, cultural specificity may constrain generalizability. Ad credibility norms, nature-based norms, and emotive framing norms vary significantly by country (
Le et al. 2024;
Meet et al. 2024;
Nguyen-Viet and Thanh Tran 2024). As great a context as Greece is for a study of the psychological impact of vague ESG messages, subsequent studies will have to cross-validate these findings in more regulatory or digitally amplified contexts to evaluate the strength of the model (
Le et al. 2024;
Meet et al. 2024;
Nguyen-Viet and Thanh Tran 2024). In this instance, the Greek case serves as a stress test of ESG influence models and as a signal of the necessity to consider contextual dynamics in digital sustainability communication research.
The results revealed that all direct effects hypothesized, for example, the impact of perceived greenwashing, digital advertisement literacy, and source credibility on purchase intention, were statistically confirmed. Persuasion knowledge and advertisement skepticism also proved to be effective mediators, in favor of a dual-process theory of digital persuasion. Multi-group comparisons also revealed the presence of differences between demographic and psychographic subgroups, such as age, gender, ESG familiarity, ad-skipping tendency, and education level. These findings highlight the multifaceted and contextual nature of consumer reactions to ESG-labeled online ads, providing both theoretical insights and practical recommendations for improved sustainability communication.
The rest of the paper is organized as follows:
Section 2 presents the major literature regarding ESG advertising, knowledge of persuasion, and digital competency.
Section 3 presents the conceptual model and hypotheses.
Section 4 describes the methodology, which includes data collection and SEM procedures.
Section 5 presents the results, including direct, mediated, and moderated effects.
Section 6 presents practical implications, and
Section 7 concludes with limitations and future research directions.
4. Data Analysis and Results
The structural analysis in the current research was obtained via structural equation modeling (SEM) in the SmartPLS 4 software package (Version 4.1.1.4). Proceeded as per the recommendations of
Nitzl et al. (
2016), SEM—more variance-based—is widely regarded as a good analytical tool for management and social science research. Partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) was used because it has the ability to estimate sophisticated causal relationships by maximizing endogenous latent variables’ explained variance (
Cheah et al. 2023;
Hair et al. 2006). Multi-group analysis (MGA) was also employed for subgroup differences analysis, which allows contextual differences, at times unknowable by using typical regression techniques, to be identified (
Cheah et al. 2023;
Hair et al. 2006). The estimation process followed methodological guidelines of
Wong (
2013), for precise calculation of path coefficients, standard errors, and reliability estimates. Indicator reliability for the reflective measurement model was defined by outer loadings greater than a 0.70 threshold in order to be in reasonable correlation with their own latent constructs.
4.1. Common Method Bias (CMB)
In order to provide evidence of the reliability and validity of the findings, common method bias (CMB) was examined according to the methodological procedure suggested by (
Podsakoff et al. 2003). Harman’s single-factor test was utilized in order to determine if a single latent factor explained most of the variance in the data. The unrotated principal component analysis found that the largest factor explained 33.863% of the overall variance, which was far from the suggested 50% value. Although CMB was not a significant threat in this study, its assessment contributes to the robustness of the analysis by avoiding potential biases and enhancing the validity of observed relationships among constructs (
Podsakoff et al. 2003,
2012).
4.2. Measurement Model
The first step of the PLS–SEM procedure is measurement model evaluation, where all the constructs are measured in terms of reflective indicators. Based on the recommendations of
Hair et al. (
2021), this is performed by ensuring composite reliability, indicator reliability, convergent validity, and discriminant validity. Indicator reliability, as defined by
Vinzi et al. (
2010), is the degree to which the variation in observed variables is captured by the latent construct it represents, usually quantifiable through outer loadings. Loadings of 0.70 or greater are commonly regarded as adequate, according to the criteria of
Wong (
2013), and
Chin (
2009). Yet,
Vinzi et al. (
2010), also admit that in social science research there may be indicators with less than this threshold. Under such conditions, low-loading indicators must be assessed for their contribution to composite reliability and convergent validity prior to their elimination.
Hair et al. (
2011), state that indicators with loadings of 0.40 to 0.70 can be eliminated only if their removal would significantly enhance composite reliability or the average variance extracted (AVE). According to these guidelines, one of the indicators (ADL5) with a loading of below 0.500 was removed from the model, as shown in
Table 2, according to the optimization criteria of
Gefen and Straub (
2005).
Reliability in the current study was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha, rho_A, and composite reliability. As suggested by Wasko et al. (
Wasko and Faraj 2005), scores above the cut-off point of 0.70 were achieved with constructs like PG, ADL, SC, PK, AS, and PI, while the other constructs also showed moderate-to-high internal consistency, in line with evidence from the previous literature (
Hair et al. 2021,
2011,
2016). The rho_A coefficient, theoretically placed between Cronbach’s alpha and composite reliability, also exceeded the 0.70 cut-off for most constructs, thus supporting the reliability findings presented by
Sarstedt et al. (
2021), and
Henseler et al. (
2015).
Convergent validity was established, with the average variance extracted (AVE) for all but a few constructs above the cut-off threshold of 0.50 recommended by
Fornell and Larcker (
1981). Composite reliability was accepted where the AVE was marginally less than 0.50, given that composite reliability was above 0.60, which is also acceptable according to
Fornell and Larcker (
1981). Discriminant validity was initially checked using the Fornell–Larcker criterion, where the square root of AVE of every construct exceeded its correlations with the other constructs. Additional confirmation was also achieved using the heterotrait–monotrait ratio (HTMT), as proposed by
Henseler et al. (
2015), where all HTMT values were below the conservative threshold value of 0.85. The findings are shown in
Table 3 and
Table 4.
4.3. Structural Model
Structural model was verified based on R
2 and Q
2 and the significance of the path coefficients, as suggested by
Hair et al. (
2021). The R
2 values reported were good with respect to the variance explained: 0.465 for purchase intention, 0.461 for advertising skepticism, and 0.447 for persuasion knowledge. Further, predictive relevance (Q
2) values also contained moderate-to-high predictability, with scores of 0.455 for advertising skepticism, 0.409 for purchase intention, and 0.440 for persuasion knowledge.
To further examine the structural model, hypothesis testing was performed to evaluate the statistical significance of the associations between constructs. Path coefficients were estimated using a bootstrapping technique, as suggested by
Hair et al. (
2016). Mediation effects were tested using a bias-corrected one-tailed bootstrapping approach with 10,000 resamples, as proposed by
Preacher and Hayes (
2008), and
Streukens and Leroi-Werelds (
2016). These are summarized in
Table 5.
All five direct hypotheses (H1–H4b) were confirmed, showing statistically significant relationships between the independent and mediating variables and the dependent variable (purchase intention). Perceived greenwashing (PG), in particular, was shown to be significantly positively related to purchase intention (β = 0.151, t = 4.302, p < 0.001), confirming H1. This implies that even where greenwashing has been identified, it is not necessarily going to eliminate behavioral intention, perhaps because it impacts other variables such as ad literacy or relevance perception. Digital advertising literacy (DAL) was significantly correlated with purchase intention (β = 0.261, t = 5.637, p < 0.001), which confirms H2. This would mean that people who are more competent in critical reading of online advertisements possess a higher purchase intention, possibly because there are higher scrutiny levels involved in judging ESG claims. Source credibility (SC) also played an important role in purchase intention (β = 0.189, t = 5.521, p < 0.001), in support of H3. This highlights the significant role of perceived trustworthiness of the ad source—i.e., platforms, brands, or influencers—in affecting persuasive effectiveness. Both cognitive–affective mediators had persuasion knowledge (PK) significantly related to purchase intention (β = 0.109, t = 2.806, p = 0.003), validating H4a. This shows that awareness of the persuasive intention will not always discourage consumers but can assist them to read and assess messages more critically, sometimes increasing behavioral intention when messages are perceived as credible. Lastly, advertising skepticism (AS) had the greatest direct impact on purchase intention (β = 0.285, t = 7.208, p < 0.001), affirming H4b. Such an outcome means that greater skepticism, far from being seen as undesirable, may even create stronger cognitive processing of ad copy, leading to more stable attitudinal responses—either negative or, paradoxically, reinforcing intention if skepticism is overcome.
These direct impacts validate the theoretical applicability of persuasion knowledge and skepticism as cognitive filters that young consumers use to make sense of ESG advertising. All pathways were significant in the predicted direction and illustrate the predictive ability of the model’s key constructs in predicting purchase intention.
4.4. Mediation Analysis
Mediation effects were examined to identify if persuasion knowledge (PK) and advertising skepticism (AS) mediate the indirect relationships among the independent variables (perceived greenwashing, digital advertising literacy, and source credibility) and the dependent variable (purchase intention). The indirect effects were measured via bias-corrected bootstrapping with 10,000 resamples. The standardized coefficients (β), standard errors (SD), t-statistics, and significance levels (
p-values) for all mediation paths are shown in
Table 6.
Beginning with perceived greenwashing (PG), testing indicated a strong indirect effect on PI through PK (H5a: β = 0.013, SE = 0.005, t = 2.618, p = 0.004), and indirectly via AS (H5b: β = 0.057, SE = 0.012, t = 4.606, p < 0.001). Both indirect effects were positive, as was PG’s direct effect on PI (β = 0.151, p < 0.001), which indicates complementary mediation. This implies that greenwashing perception among participants partly mediates purchase intention as well as recognition of persuasion tactic and skepticism about advertising—without obviating PG’s direct effect.
For digital advertising literacy (DAL), there was significant mediation through PK (H6a: β = 0.052, SE = 0.018, t = 2.801, p = 0.003) and through AS (H6b: β = 0.167, SE = 0.023, t = 7.134, p < 0.001). The direct association between DAL and PI was also positive and significant (β = 0.261, p < 0.001), thus establishing complementary partial mediation. This would mean that higher digital advertising literacy increases both cognitive resistance (through PK) and affective resistance (through AS), which in turn shape stronger consumer purchase intention—or even cause prudent discrimination when receiving ESG messages.
With regard to source credibility (SC), the model also facilitated strong indirect effects to PI through both PK (H7a: β = 0.027, SE = 0.011, t = 2.451, p = 0.007) and AS (H7b: β = 0.023, SE = 0.012, t = 1.951, p = 0.026). Since the direct effect of SC on PI was still significant and positive (β = 0.189, p < 0.001), the above findings also indicate complementary mediation. This implies that the source of a digital ESG message is not just positively affecting purchase intention directly but also indirectly by affecting the extent to which the message appears to be effective and how critically it is considered.
In summary, all six indirect effects (H5a–H7b) were significant in a directional pattern consistent with their respective direct effects. Therefore, the mediating functions of persuasion knowledge and advertising skepticism are established to be complementary rather than substitutive to the direct effects of greenwashing perceptions, digital literacy, and source credibility on behavioral intentions. The evidence is supportive of a dual-process model of digital persuasion: central (cognitive) as well as peripheral (skeptical or affective) processes are engaged when people react to ESG-related digital communication.
4.5. Muti-Group Analysis (MGA)
To analyze possible moderation effects across the structural model, a sequence of multi-group analyses (MGAs) was executed with SmartPLS. Subgroup differences were analyzed by gender, age, familiarity with ESG, trust towards ESG influencers, frequency of ad-skipping, and education level. Statistically significant differences only (
p < 0.05) are listed below (
Table 7).
Gender differences were revealed particularly: men were more positively affected by digital ad literacy (DAL) and source credibility (SC) on building purchase intention (PI), whereas females experienced greater negative impacts of perceived greenwashing (PG) and persuasion knowledge (PK) on PI, as well as greater skepticism (AS). The findings indicate gender differentiation in digital persuasion processing. Age variation indicated younger subjects (18–20) to be more influenced by DAL, SC, and PK in building AS and PI, with older respondents (31–35) having higher SC → PK relationships. The trend verifies developmental differences in source evaluation processing and persuasion. Subjects with lower ESG familiarity were inclined to have a higher negative influence on PI through AS and PK, suggesting greater resistance. By contrast, strongly ESG-aware respondents responded more favorably to SC and DAL, thus pointing towards more thoughtful patterns of choice-making. The main routes were also moderated by trust in ESG influencers. Participants with high trust were more attracted to SC and DAL, whereas low-trust participants were more responsive toward PG, accompanied by greater skepticism and reactance. Ad-skipping behavior was related to more negative effects of AS and PK on PI, particularly for high skippers, while SC was a robust positive predictor in all such subgroups. These findings indicate persuasion avoidance and ad literacy variation. Level of education also moderated effects attributed to persuasion awareness and credibility. More highly educated respondents (Bachelor’s or Master’s) exhibited superior DAL and SC effects, which indicate more skilled processing of advertisement cues.
These results substantiate the hypothesis that ESG advertising’s impact is mediated by salient demographic, cognitive, and behavioral variables, influencing how various subgroups process, reject, or accept persuasive messages.
5. Discussion
The outcome of the structural equation model provides insightful information with regard to young consumers’ processing and reaction to ESG-labeled internet ads. All direct hypotheses (H1–H4b) were confirmed, demonstrating suitable and theoretically sound relationships between cognitive–affective processes and purchasing intention (PI) in an environment of online sustainability messages. The findings provide richness to the pool of existing research on greenwashing, persuasion knowledge, and Gen Z digital activity, as well as some surprising nuances.
5.1. Direct Relationships: Interpreting the Path Effects
Perceived greenwashing (PG) positively influenced purchase intention (β = 0.151,
p < 0.001), confirming H1 but contradicting some early expectations. Greenwashing is typically supposed to be monotonically negative for brand evaluation and behavioral outcomes, with the current literature demonstrating negative impacts on trust, satisfaction, and purchase willingness (
Raghunandan and Rajgopal 2022;
Rotman and David-Pennington 2024;
Stromberg and Bali Swain 2024). Yet the findings of this study reveal that even when consumers suspect ESG content to be potentially misleading, this does not inevitably annihilate behavioral intention. The reason may be that greenwashing detection may trigger other cognitive processes—critical analysis, relevance assessment, or moral engagement—that reconceptualize the message without cancelling its persuasive appeal. This finding is consistent with the paradox demonstrated in (
Mladenovic et al. 2024), in which low-credibility ESG signals remained persuasive under certain contextual cues.
Digital advertising literacy (DAL) was positively related to purchase intention (β = 0.261,
p < 0.001), confirming H2. This suggests that increased critical sensitivity toward digital persuasive tactics does not discourage, but rather reinforces, behavioral intent. This finding concurs with Persuasion Knowledge Model and newer evidence by Fella and Bausa, 2024, that consumers who are media-literate, particularly Gen Z, utilize literacy as a means to cope with persuasive communication more effectively, as opposed to avoiding it altogether. DAL can thus be a self-confidence device, allowing users to critically but constructively analyze ESG statements (
Díaz et al. 2024;
Cinceoglu and Strauß 2025). This is also connected to the theoretical value of incorporating DAL into models of online persuasion, particularly in the context of environmental communication.
The effect of source credibility (SC) on purchase intention (β = 0.189,
p < 0.001) also aligns with the Elaboration Likelihood Model, which places credibility at the center as a key peripheral cue in persuasion. This also aligns with more contemporary research by
Higueras-Castillo et al. (
2024), and
Crapa et al. (
2024), where those micro-communicators and influencers who were perceived as authentic were found to be more engaging. With skepticism and saturation dominating the world of advertising online, communicator–recipient trust is an important success determinant of persuasion. With Gen Z, for instance, source attributes including expertise, transparency, and moral congruence can make up for otherwise failing content or context (
Raghunandan and Rajgopal 2022;
Palmieri et al. 2025).
Persuasion knowledge (PK) was also positively correlated with purchase intention (β = 0.109,
p = 0.003), supporting H4a. This provides a more nuanced understanding of PK’s contribution to advertising effectiveness. While previous versions of the PKM implied greater persuasion knowledge as a vehicle to counteract susceptibility to marketing (e.g., by triggering resistance), newer evidence indicates that PK facilitates more sophisticated responses—particularly among skeptical young consumers. For instance, Bertucci Lima’s model is focused on the extent to which PK, in conjunction with emotionally congruent framing and credible sources, can enable rather than hinder persuasion. In this regard, the capacity to discern persuasive intent may have enabled participants to form better judgments regarding ESG ads, effectively cementing rather than dismissing behavioral intention (
Díaz et al. 2024;
Rotman and David-Pennington 2024;
Stromberg and Bali Swain 2024).
Notably, advertising skepticism (AS) most strongly positively directly affected purchase intention (β = 0.285,
p < 0.001), hence empirically confirming H4b and posing a theoretical paradox. Skepticism is often thought of as a barrier to persuasion, yet here seems to be positively linked with purchasing intention. One way of interpreting this is that skepticism, instead of being a signal of rejection, is elevated cognitive performance. This aligns with research by
Huang et al. (
2024), and
Chwialkowska et al. (
2024), whereby skepticism was found to drive closer questioning of message authenticity, especially when consumers are faced with trite or cliched ESG appeals. For Gen Z consumers, who share the collective moniker of “critical believers,” skepticism can be less of a barrier and more of a filter—through which they might test the message before adopting it. This route also represents a “suspicion–resolve” process: when suspicious consumers are confident that a message meets their authenticity standards, they can reward it with stronger behavioral intent (
Akram et al. 2024;
Chwialkowska et al. 2024).
Collectively, these direct effects corroborate the theoretical hypothesis that online persuasion in ESG advertising is not a content of message, but an interpretive dynamic shaped by knowledge, trust, emotional skepticism, and media literacy. The model confirms the validity of frameworks such as PKM and ELM, but conjectures that with communications to Gen Z through ESG, linear assumptions such as “more skepticism equals less persuasion” are no longer necessary. Rather, the interplay of psychological resilience, digital literacy, and emotional engagement creates a more dynamic environment of persuasion—where greenwashing does not necessarily rebound, and skepticism can be a strength rather than a liability.
5.2. Mediation Analysis: The Role of Persuasion Knowledge and Advertising Skepticism
Mediation analysis provides us with interesting insights into the role of perceived greenwashing (PG), digital advertising literacy (DAL), and source credibility (SC) influencing consumer purchase intentions (PI) in ESG-labeled digital advertisement. More precisely, the current study tests the mediating functions of persuasion knowledge (PK) and advertising skepticism (AS)—two of the most basic cognitive and affective processing constructs of persuasive messages. All six of the indirect effects hypothesized (H5a–H7b) were significant, indicating complementary partial mediation in each instance. This is consistent with the idea that both central and peripheral processing pathways are operating at the same time, as predicted by dual-process theories of persuasion like the Elaboration Likelihood Model and extensions of the Persuasion Knowledge Model.
Beginning with perceived greenwashing, both pathways, PG → PK → PI (H5a) and PG → AS → PI (H5b), were positive and significant. This indicates that even though greenwashing has a tendency to induce skepticism and critical thinking, these beliefs perhaps may not always deter behavioral intention. Instead, critical sensitivity (through PK) and affective sensitivity (through AS) can turn suspicion into discriminative consideration, particularly with the addition of other items of coherent evidence like credible source information or emotional appeal. This is consistent with new research that shows skepticism about ESG communication does not necessarily equal rejection (
Akram et al. 2024;
Borah et al. 2024;
Chwialkowska et al. 2024). Instead, consumers—particularly Gen Z—will retain intention to interact with sustainable products if they are empowered to be able to critically examine messages.
Digital advertising literacy showed even more powerful indirect effects via both PK (H6a: β = 0.052) and AS (H6b: β = 0.167), indicating that literate consumers think deeply about persuasive message content, identifying persuasive tactics and emotionally regulating their reactions. DAL seems to be a dormant asset that triggers both cognitive defenses (e.g., decoding persuasion attempts) and affective filters (e.g., regulating trust and relevance). This double mediation is important theoretically: it guarantees the position of DAL not merely as a protective shield, but as an enabler of interaction and efficient decision-making in complicated digital contexts. It also develops on previous research demanding an active concept of ad literacy—one that is enabling rather than inoculating (
Fella and Bausa 2024;
Nguyen-Viet and Thanh Tran 2024;
Nguyen-Viet et al. 2024).
Source credibility also had an influence indirectly through PK (H7a) and AS (H7b), complementing its direct positive influence on PI. The finding suggests that credible sources do not only persuade directly (by increasing trust) but that they influence the interpretive environment in which the consumer is evaluating message intention and genuineness. Credibility essentially appears to dampen resistance during processing of potentially skeptical ESG appeals, but modulates the efficacy of PK and AS rather than entirely preventing them from being effective.
Together, the mediation findings are in line with the simultaneous co-activation of affective and cognitive resistance processes to digital ESG messages. In contradistinction to serving as inhibitory blocks to persuasion, PK and AS operate as evaluative filters capable of enhancing message processing and generating more resistant or goal-directed behavioral intentions. This challenges the conventional assumption that resistance indicators—such as skepticism or persuasion knowledge—are inherently detrimental to persuasive outcomes.
5.3. Multi-Group Analysis (MGA): Moderation by Demographics and Contextual Factors
The multi-group analysis (MGA) yielded important findings on the contribution of demographic and contextual factors towards shaping the structural relationships in ESG-labeled online ads. Variance was present across gender, age, familiarity with ESG, trust in ESG endorsers, attitudes on skipping ads, and educational level, highlighting the importance of differentiated digital persuasion methods depending on the segmentation of the audience.
Gender differences manifested in differential processing mechanisms. In men, digital advertising literacy (DAL) exerted a significantly greater impact on purchase intention (PI), indicating advertising ability as an enabling factor of behavioral intention for male consumers. For females, however, perceived greenwashing (PG) and persuasion knowledge (PK) had an impact to a greater negative magnitude, resonating with greater critical resistance and affective scrutiny. Women also revealed more intense indirect effects via DAL → AS and SC → AS, indicating that affective skepticism plays a more salient function in their ESG content processing. These results are consistent with earlier findings on gendered cognitive–emotional reactions in persuasive situations and indicate the necessities for gender-aware digital sustainability communication (
De Freitas Netto et al. 2020;
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Dangelico et al. 2024).
Age comparisons indicated developmental differences in persuasion processing. Young adults (18–20) were more susceptible to DAL → AS and SC → AS, reflecting both greater reactivity to advertising appeals and less stability in decoding persuasive intent. Older age groups (31–35) showed greater SC → PK, reflecting more advanced integration of source credibility with critical evaluation. Declining influence of DAL → PI across older age groups could imply a shift towards experiential or reputational heuristics and away from literacy-dependent trust in buying decision-making. The results reflect digital maturity patterns and media skepticism based on age (
Das et al. 2025;
Le et al. 2024;
Lopes et al. 2024).
ESG familiarity further moderated persuasion effects. Participants with lower ESG awareness were significantly more affected by AS → PI and PK → PI, indicating a reliance on defensive cognitive–affective mechanisms when encountering ESG messaging. In contrast, high-familiarity individuals responded more favorably to SC → PI and DAL → PI, likely due to a combination of trust and selective engagement. These patterns underscore the importance of awareness-building strategies as a precursor to effective persuasion in sustainability contexts (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Das et al. 2025;
Theocharis and Tsekouropoulos 2025;
Lopes et al. 2024).
Trust in the ESG influencer facilitated several pathways. Low-trust respondents were more responsive to PG and AS signals with higher resistance and critical literacy. High-trust respondents were more dependent on SC → PI and DAL → PK, which were related to higher openness and credibility-based persuasion pathway. This lends support for the necessity of customized influencer-based campaigns as per target audience trust profiles.
Heavy ESG ad-skippers were found to yield larger negative impacts of PG, PK, and AS on PI, indicating an avoidance pattern as well as defensive processing. Source credibility, however, was still a compelling force among this group, indicating that credible presentation would be effective in alleviating ad fatigue to some extent.
Lastly, education had an effect on processing depth. Continuing or more education had more significant DAL → PK and SC → PK effects, i.e., more ability in persuasional strategy decoding. This fits with research substantiating education as a predictor of improved critical media literacy (
De Freitas Netto et al. 2020;
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Le et al. 2024).
Cumulatively, the MGA results speak to the context-dependent nature of online persuasion in ESG advertising. They suggest that individual differences—sex, age, awareness, and trust—moderate not just receptivity to the message but the very psychological process of influence. Researchers should continue examining these moderators with longitudinal or experimental designs in the future to advance targeted intervention and prevent ethical criticism.
6. Practical Implications
This research presents a set of actionable findings for stakeholders engaged in the production, regulation, and dissemination of ESG-labeled advertising. Through an exploration of interdependencies between perceived greenwashing, digital advertising literacy, source credibility, persuasion knowledge, skepticism, and purchase intention, the findings provide a competent understanding of how audiences interpret and react to ESG-themed promotional communication. Specifically, the research accentuates the mediating role of demographic and psychographic heterogeneity, calling for more targeted and audience-aware communication initiatives.
6.1. Implications for Policymakers and Regulators
The finding that perceived greenwashing has a positive direct relationship with purchase intention—even when it triggers persuasion knowledge and ad skepticism—poses a regulatory dilemma: misleading ESG communication can still be effective with some consumers. That would mean recent policy initiatives, such as the EU Green Claims Directive and FTC guidelines, need to move beyond penalizing false claims and towards demanding greater disclosure for ESG communication (
Akram et al. 2024;
Borah et al. 2024;
Chwialkowska et al. 2024).
Regulators can promote standardization of presentation of ESG claims, where corporation incentives can provide the foundation for their environmental claims in quantitative terms or well-recognized frameworks (e.g., GRI, CDP). Third-party-verified ESG-labeling messages’ policies can minimize the convincing power of likely misleading messages and build public confidence.
In addition, the literacy and suspicion of advertising as drivers of behavioral intention imply the merit of education campaigns to enhance citizens’ ability to identify persuasive purpose and scrutinize assertions of sustainability (
Causevic et al. 2022;
Cinceoglu and Strauß 2025;
Crapa et al. 2024). Consumer protection online would include interactive awareness sites (e.g., internet browser add-ons or sites on media literacy) that highlight greenwashing risk and prompt critical examination of advertisement messages.
6.2. Implications for Business and Marketing Practitioners
For business marketers and managers, the results are challenging as they provide opportunities. Source credibility, first of all, was a robust and straightforward, as well as indirect, predictor of buying intention. This indicates that advertising spend on authentic brand voices—e.g., vetted ESG influencers, institutional alliances, or evidence-based product claims—can yield high returns on investment for ESG messages (
De Freitas Netto et al. 2020;
Das et al. 2025;
Dangelico et al. 2024).
But the double function of greenwashing—both inducing awareness of persuasion and distrust, but still with an impact on behavior—poses that part of the consumers respond not because they are convinced but because they are confused or have no other option. The message speaks for itself: long-term trust is worth more than short-term persuasion. Additionally, digital ad literacy also proved to be even more potent in driving purchasing intent when mediated through critical thinking (persuasion knowledge) and also skepticism. This implies the major contribution of active consumer engagement in open, dialogic ESG communication, as opposed to tapping into passive formulaic slogan-based approaches (
Dangelico et al. 2024;
Crapa et al. 2024). Mechanisms like real-time ESG performance dashboards, open disclosures, and user feedback channels can make messages credible as well as consumers active agents.
Audience segmentation is necessary too. The multi-group analysis of the study also uncovered significant differences between various demographic groups in processing ESG content. For example, men were more positively responsive to high source credibility and advertisement literacy, but women were more responsive to greenwashing and attempts at persuasion. Young (18–20) users drew more heavily on affective skepticism, while older users employed more cognitively elaborated source judgments. This means that message customization—in tone, style, and medium—is to be given top priority in ESG messaging campaigns (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
Das et al. 2025).
For influencer-based initiatives, effects are particularly applicable. Engagers who have high trust in ESG influencers exhibited greater source credibility and digital literacy but lower resistance via skepticism. This indicates that influencer marketing must be tightly regulated not to take advantage of credibility and hold influencers to transparency and disclosure standards. Co-creation of content with authenticated experts or engaging influencers in sustainability education programs may enhance credibility while minimizing reputational risk.
6.3. Implications for Educators and Media Literacy Advocates
Educationalists, especially instructors of digital literacy, media studies, and environmental education, can make crucial inferences based on the mediating functions of advertisement skepticism and persuasion knowledge. The inferences point to the twofold significance of cognitive and affective abilities in managing digital advertising.
Advertising literacy programs must move beyond simplistic media analysis and instead incorporate scenario-based exercises, interactive ESG claim reviews, and role-playing assignments in which students critically examine brand messaging. Educating about source cues (influencers, government seals, NGO endorsements) that affect persuasion processes can enable students to differentiate between credibility-based versus manipulation-based ESG content (
De Freitas Netto et al. 2020;
Das et al. 2025).
The education- and age-based discrepancies revealed in this research are consistent with early and ongoing education in internet-based resistance to persuasion. University studies can be complemented with professional modules on cyber ethics, greenwashing identification, and practices of green marketing. Gamified learning resources among younger students with a focus on environmental literacy can be influential entry points for resilience development.
Lastly, the study recommends integrating established theories of persuasive communication—like the Elaboration Likelihood Model and the Persuasion Knowledge Model—into media and marketing education. This will prepare students better to examine how intentions to behave are formed by both message content, as well as perceived intent, framing, and delivery strategy.
7. Conclusions, Limitations, and Future Directions
This study examined how young adults process ESG-labeled online advertisements and how perceived greenwashing (PG), digital ad literacy (DAL), and source credibility (SC) influence purchase intention (PI), both directly and through the mediating roles of persuasion knowledge (PK) and advertising skepticism (AS). The findings confirmed all hypothesized relationships, revealing that both rational and affective mechanisms shape consumer reactions to ESG-themed advertising. Multi-group comparisons further highlighted important subgroup differences based on gender, age, ESG familiarity, trust in ESG influencers, ad-skipping behavior, and education level—each moderating the strength or direction of specific persuasion pathways.
While this research is significant theoretically and practically, there are some significant limitations that should be noted and provide avenues for future research into digital ESG persuasion. This research was conducted only with 18–35-year-old Greek consumers, which may restrict the external validity of the results in other institutional or cultural environments. Considering the unique sociopolitical environment of Greece—marked by intermediate levels of digital trust, evolving awareness of EU-aligned ESG regulation, and a historically elevated media skepticism among parts of the population—consumer reaction to ESG-branded advertising might differ from that in more digitally affluent or institutionally trusted environments (
Borah et al. 2024;
Causevic et al. 2022). Future studies will have to reproduce and generalize the existing model to other diverse national settings to determine potential cultural or media system-level differences in ESG ad effects. The second limitation of the cross-sectional design is that it cannot provide causal inferences about how persuasion processes change over time. Future longitudinal or experimental approaches may better establish how multiple exposures to ESG communications, especially with greenwashing elements, affect persuasion knowledge, skepticism, and behavioral intention at multiple time horizons (
Dangelico et al. 2024;
Chwialkowska et al. 2024). Second, the specificity of young participants, although justified in terms of their digital literacy and exposure to ESG agendas, limits generalizability. The inclusion of older age cohorts or lower levels of digitally active users might provide valuable data on whether and under what conditions dual-process persuasion processes differ among generational cohorts and levels of digital literacy (
Duffett and Mxunyelwa 2025;
He et al. 2023).
Third, although the research employed validated self-report measures, upcoming studies can improve analysis by incorporating behavioral or physiological measures—e.g., eye-tracking, click behavior, or response times—that provide more fine-grained insight into how consumers process ESG ads in the moment. Such measures might help to span the intention–behavior gap, a long-standing problem in sustainability research. Even though self-report perceptual measures tap into consumers’ likely ESG advertising processing in naturalistic conditions, experimental exposure to standard ad stimuli was not employed in this study. Future studies can potentially capitalize on that by including controlled advertisement manipulations—e.g., differential use of environmental imagery, third-party certification, or transparency of ESG claims—to systematically study message features’ impact on greenwashing perceptions and credibility judgments. This would enable more robust causal inference and finer-grained comprehension of the threshold at which sustainability communications become persuasive or suspicious.
Furthermore, the current model focused on persuasion knowledge and skepticism as mediators, yet emotional aspects—like guilt, pride, eco-anxiety, or moral outrage—are still untouched in the realm of ESG advertising. One potential follow-up study would be an extension of the dual-path framework with these emotional aspects, showing whether and how particular emotions facilitate or impede the rational and skeptical processing pathways found here (
Fella and Bausa 2024;
Higueras-Castillo et al. 2024). The research reveals greenwashing, as well, at the participants’ subjective level of understanding. While this clearly does capture real consumer experience, additional work could mix this with objective content analysis or experimentally created message attributes (e.g., nature imagery, trust badges, source disclaimers) to learn under what conditions ESG claims degenerate into perceived deception. Considering the substantial moderating roles established (e.g., education, ESG awareness, trust), future research could employ person-centered or segmentation approaches to determine underlying consumer segments—such as critical skeptics, passive accepters, or sustainability champions—and examine each segment’s unique interpretation and response to ESG communications (
Díaz et al. 2024;
Gregory 2024). Finally, institutional and platform-specific communication processes of ESG can be assessed in the future. As social media influencers and branded content websites increasingly play a critical role in influencing credibility and trust perception, researchers can test how platform design, influencer attribute, or targeting via algorithms affects acceptance and resistance to messages across groups.
In all, while current scholarship builds a strong dual-process model of ESG persuasion, it also reveals a landscape of still-pending questions. The shifting ground of digital communication—where ethics and influence, trust and doubt, are in uneasy tension with one another—requires ongoing scholarly focus. Upcoming research, by engaging in more varied methods, broader audiences, and affectively responsive models, can navigate new routes through this complicated psychological terrain. Because green messages seek to move the heart as they persuade the mind, so too should questions bring rigor and empathy together in wedded bliss. Only by such balanced questioning will we stand a chance of understanding how persuasion works not merely in amounts and decisions, but in the inner spaces where values, emotion, and will intersect.