Next Article in Journal
Surgical Advances in Paediatric Neuroblastoma
Previous Article in Journal
Association between Serum Total and Specific Immunoglobulin E Levels and Body Height: A Cross-Sectional Study of Children and Adolescents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Factors Associated with the Suspected Delay in the Language Development of Early Childhood in Southern Thailand

by
Namfon Rithipukdee
and
Kiatkamjorn Kusol
*
Department of Children and Adolescent Nursing, School of Nursing, Walailak University, Thasala, Nakhon Si Thammarat 80160, Thailand
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2022, 9(5), 662; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050662
Submission received: 6 March 2022 / Revised: 26 April 2022 / Accepted: 28 April 2022 / Published: 4 May 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Nursing)

Abstract

:
Many children have a suspected delay in language development and need extensive support from parents and the health care team. This study aimed to investigate the suspected delay in language development and the factors associated with the suspected delay in language development among early childhood in Southern Thailand. Children aged 24 to 60 months were recruited as study samples using stratified random sampling conducted in 23 districts and simple random sampling in 7 sections (425 children). The research instruments comprised demographic data on the children and families, the preschool temperament questionnaire, and the Language Development Screening questionnaire using the Developmental Surveillance and Promotion Manual and data collected from July 2020 to January 2021. Data analysis used descriptive statistics and logistic regression. The results showed that the suspected delay in development was 40.9 percent. Daily screen time exceeding 2 h per day (AOR = 17.30, 95% CI: 7.35–40.72), and regarding a child’s temperament, moderate-to-difficult temperament (AOR = 9.56, 95% CI: 5.12–17.85) was significantly associated with the suspected delay in language development. Genders, gestational age of early childhood, and the age of the caregivers were not associated with the suspected delay in language development. The study suggested that a decrease in daily screen time and appropriate responses will help develop language in children.

1. Introduction

Language capacity is an integral part of the life of children. Language development is a hierarchical change beginning with hearing and learning by hearing through the sound of words [1]. According to Piaget’s theory, before a child can speak, the child communicates through action and behaviors. After that, children begin to perform concrete thinking operations and develop expressive and receptive language [2]. After six months, children’s ability to distinguish sounds will have improved, followed by development concerning lexical resources and fast word interconnection from 1.5 to 3 years [3]. These skills significantly contribute to the development in early childhood in the long run. The assessment of early childhood development screening tools includes the Modified Denver, Denver Developmental Screening Test (DDST), Diagnostic Inventory for Screening Children (DISC), Developmental Skills Instrument (DSI), and Modified Denver II [4,5]. The screening of early childhood development in Thailand is mainly performed using the Developmental Surveillance and Promotion Manual (DSPM). DSPM in language development screening can be categorized into two aspects: receptive language comprehension through sensory nerves obtained through input from hearing and input from sight. With this, a child can differentiate differences in sounds, interpret them and grasp their meanings. Turning to expressive language is a verbal communication of intentions. The World Health Organization (WHO) disclosed that 8 percent of children under 5 years of age exhibit non-age-appropriate development [6]. In Thailand, data on the overall development of children in early childhood in 2017 suggested that 23 percent of the children in early childhood exhibited a suspected delay in development. More specifically, 39.87 percent of the children were potentially had postponed development, and 26.63 were imagined to have a delay in linguistic development in Health Area 11, Southern Thailand [7], which was higher than the overall national proportion.
Language development reflects how a child’s brain and nervous system function continuously through each inherent developmental stage [8,9]. Studies on a child’s linguistic progress have demonstrated that preterm infants undergo relatively more delayed development during the early years, especially in the domain of prescriptive language use than those born in full pregnancy term [10,11]. Preterm children aged 18 to 36 months were found to have more linguistic limitations than those born at full-term. The preterm children, for example, acquired vocabularies of less than 50 and had difficulty piecing together their vocabularies. The preterm children had a delay in linguistic development, some of whom experienced a delay in both using and understanding the language [12]. Additionally, studies regarding the relationship between gender and linguistic abilities suggested that female children demonstrated faster linguistic development than male children [13,14].
Temperament is an innate, intrinsic model unique to each child with outstanding resistance to change [15]. Children with easy temperaments can be understood as possessing effective emotional control leading to positive social interactions [16,17]. Delayed linguistic ability undermines a child’s temperament and personality [18]. On the other hand, those with difficult temperaments lessen others’ interest in talking to them, consequently compromising the child’s expressive language development and susceptibility to anxiety when exposed to unacquainted surroundings. Hence, changing from a familiar home to a school can become a catalyst for mutism in children [19,20].
Caregivers or parents play a significant role in enhancing a child’s linguistic ability in early childhood because they provide understanding, care, connection, and interaction with the child. Previous studies have shown that the predictive factors of age-appropriate development in early childhood include the mother’s age: chances for mothers aged 20–35 years and above 35 years to give birth to age-appropriate development in children decreased by 18 and 33 percent, respectively, compared to mothers less than 20 years. This is because such a group of mothers is a working-age population, meaning they have less time to care for their children. As a result, their children are prone to a suspected delay in development [21]. The studies conducted with preterm infants in low-income families have found that mothers’ age attributed to a delay in development [22].
On the contrary, Phongphetdit and Authawee have found that mothers aged 20 to 35 could raise children to the age-appropriate development stages because, as part of the working-age population, they were able to seek knowledge and access a wide variety of media easily and instantly [23]. Nevertheless, under some circumstances, parents’ or caregivers’ limited abilities and skills complicated the orientation of the home environment to cater to a child’s development, not to mention today’s technological dynamics evolving by leaps and bounds. Children in early childhood have become more attached to screen gadgets such as smartphones, tablets, computers, or even televisions [24]. The American Academy of Pediatrics pointed out that children aged 2 to 5 years could spend one hour a day watching quality media [25]. The careers of parents usually contribute to children watching television or surfing the internet alone for entertainment, or sometimes screen time is used to appease a child’s tantrums. These all result in a child’s delayed linguistic abilities [26]. This also contributes to sleep problems, impaired executive function and general cognition, and the relationship between parent and child in early childhood [27,28,29,30]. In addition, children with language delays or communication difficulties may be at an increased risk of learning disabilities and illiteracy, including reading and writing problems in adulthood [31,32]. The issues and importance of the factors mentioned above concerning a child’s development (including caregivers, children, environment, or changing social conditions) influence the child’s development; however, there is a lack of studies on language development in early childhood in Southern Thailand. Researchers have acknowledged the importance of investigating the situations related to language development and the factors associated with a delay in language development in early childhood to provide empirical data that can be applied for language development so that children in early childhood can reach language developmental milestones.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

This study is a descriptive study. The researchers collected data from the well-baby clinics in the Tambon Health Promoting Hospitals from July 2020 to January 2021. The research population comprised children aged 24 to 60 months and parents or caregivers residing in Southern Thailand. The researchers conducted randomized sampling in Nakhon Si Thammarat Province, which has a total children population of 92,973 persons. The researchers calculated the sample size using the Krejcie and Morgan formula, generating 382 research samples [33]. The researchers added about 10% to prevent the collection of incomprehensive data. Stratified random sampling was employed in 23 districts, followed by simple random sampling in 7 selected districts. Then, two of the Tambon Health Promoting hospitals were selected, yielding a group of 425 research samples. The inclusion criteria specified conditions for the caregivers and the children as follows: Caregivers may be parents or primary caregivers, anyone living with the child, and those providing regular care for longer than six months. The caregivers must also be 18 years of age or above, be of Thai ethnicity and Thai nationality, and communicate and understand Thai. The children must be within 24–60 months, visit the well-baby clinic for vaccination, and be free from genetic disorders and any other disease that impacts child development such as down syndrome, mental retardation, autistic disorder, etc. The children must exhibit normal visual, and learning abilities.

2.2. Measures

The research instruments are as follows:
  • The demographic data questionnaire for the children, parents or caregivers, and families (14 items):
    1.1
    Demographic data of the children, including genders, religions, age, gestational age, delivery methods, birth weight, the average number of hours spent watching television, videos, smartphones, tablets, telephone, and computer games per day, and child temperament;
    1.2
    Demographic data of the parents or caregivers and families, including age, relationship with the children, family characteristics, levels of education, occupations, and monthly household income.
  • The preschool temperament questionnaire: The researchers employed the research instrument developed by Nattaya Sangsai et al. (2011), with a reliability value of 0.80 and content validity of 0.83 [34]. The questionnaire consists of 36 items featuring characteristics of 6 aspects of temperament: Activity level (8 items), Rhythmicity or Regularity (5 items), Approach/ withdrawal (5 items), Adaptability (5 items), Intensity of Reaction (7 items), and Mood (6 items). Questions were answered using a 5-point rating scale from 36 to 180 points. Interpretations of the children’s temperament were clustered into two groups: those who scored 36–132 were categorized as children with moderate-to-difficult temperament, and those who scored 133–180 were classified as children with an easy temperament.
  • The language development screening questionnaire: The researcher screened a child’s development with the Developmental Surveillance and Promotion Manual (DSPM), divided into Receptive language and Expressive language developed by Siriporn Kanchana et al. [35]. The questionnaire yielded a sensitivity value of 96.04 and a specificity value of 64.67. The interpretation was categorized as either age-appropriate development (1 score) or suspected delay (0 scores). As demonstrated by Cronbach’s alpha, the questionnaire’s reliability was 0.81.

2.3. Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed using the SPSS® Version 24.0 for Windows™ (IBM Corporation, New York, NY, USA). The number of suspected delays in language development and demographic data was analyzed using frequency distribution, percentage, mean, and standard deviation (S.D.) The factors’ correlation with suspected delay in language development was assessed using binary logistic regression.

3. Results

Regarding the children’s language development in early childhood, 59.1 percent exhibited age-appropriate development, while 40.9 percent showed a suspected delay in development, as shown in Table 1.

3.1. Demographic Data of Children

Most of the children were Buddhists (72%), and their average age was 40 months. In addition, 397 of the children (93.41%) were full-term infants, and 76% were born through vaginal delivery. Forty-eight percent of children’s birth weights fell into a range of 2500 to 3000 g. More than half of the children in early childhood (73.6%) were reported to have less than 2 h of average daily screen time (i.e., television, videos, smartphones, tablets, or computer games). The children demonstrated easy temperament, as the children’s average temperament score was 105.99, as displayed in Table 2.

3.2. Demographic Data of Parents or Caregivers

Most caregivers were from 25 to 35 years of age (58.60%), and more than half of the caregivers were mothers (80.24%). In addition, 62.82% were single-family, 53.41% obtained a secondary school degree, and almost one-third (28%) were unemployed. Finally, more than half (53.64%) earned 10,000–20,000 Thai baht per month, as displayed in Table 3.
The binary logistic regression analysis suggested that the children with more than 2 h of daily screen time were significantly associated with a suspected delay in language development (AOR = 17.30 (95% CI: 7.35–40.72). In terms of temperament, the children exhibiting moderate-to-difficult temperament were significantly associated with a suspected delay in language development (AOR = 9.56; 95% CI: 5.12–17.85). Finally, gender, gestational age, and caregivers’ age groups were not associated with a suspected delay in language development, as displayed in Table 4.

4. Discussion

The analysis results yielded in the research sample group of average 40 months-old children ( x ¯ = 40.90, S.D. = 9.04) indicated that they were mainly delivered at full-term and had an age-appropriate birth weight. For a mother assuming the role of the primary caregiver in a single-family home and having obtained a secondary school degree, the data suggested that in terms of the language development situation of Southern Thai children in early childhood, nearly half exhibited a suspected delay in language development (40.90%). The analysis further illuminated that the children that spent more than 2 h on daily screen time were significantly associated with a suspected delay in language development. Furthermore, the children categorized as having moderate-to-difficult temperament were significantly associated with a suspected delay in language development compared to those with easy temperament. At the same time, gender, gestational age, and caregivers’ age groups were not associated with a suspected language development delay.
This study suggests that children with more than 2 h of daily screen time were associated with a suspected delay in language development. This was consistent with the American Academy of Pediatrics and World Health Organization that advised against screen time for children below two years of age or a daily maximum 1 h limit; in addition, quality programs should be allowed [26,36,37]. This is due to the fast-paced technological evolvement, which has made technology more accessible for children, resulting in the children’s passive interaction in a non-virtual domain. In addition, a prolific increase in young children’s screen time may derive from multiple causes, one of which is the essential caregivers and the home environment. If, for instance, a mother permits a child screen time even in the mother’s presence during meals as a common stratagem to stop the child’s naughty behavior or to manipulate the child’s behavior rather than doing other activities, an increase in screen time will subsequently follow [38,39]. In the context of a traditional Thai family, some children live in an extended family together with their grandparents, who perceive the use of screen time as entertainment for their grandchildren and, as a result, allow them to indulge their cravings for screen time without experience in operating these gadgets themselves [40]. This study, in most cases, found that caregivers or parents filled their children’s time with the use of gadgets, the simplest case of which was all-day television watching while they were away for a full-time job schedule or tending to house chores The reason was rooted in the parents’ need to assume other responsibilities besides caring for their child. Unaccompanied children might be drawn to programs featuring age-inappropriate content [41]. Exposure to excessive screen time (>6.5 h/day) via television, mobile phones, iPads/tablets, or computers is likely to inhibit verbal interaction or communication between the child and the caregiver, resulting in even greater susceptibility to a delay in language development [42,43]. Therefore, over the age period from 0 to5 years, a child’s learning should originate from outdoor activities, physical contact, or storytelling because the input received through the sensory channels involved relatively accelerates a child’s learning progress in the acquisition, problem-solving, and enthusiasm to learn more than the screen can deliver [26,40].
In addition, this study suggested that the children with moderate-to-difficult temperaments were associated with a suspected delay in language development compared to those with easy temperaments. This may be related to the fact that character in early childhood is mainly governed by genetic and environmental predispositions even though it becomes gradually nurtured with older age. A child’s character is indicative of how they respond to the environment; for example, those with moderate-to-difficult temperament may cause caregivers to provide inappropriate care, followed by an even worse temperament exhibited by the child [44]. Difficult temperament impedes parenting and caregiving in almost every sphere, including eating, sleeping, excreting, and temper control. There is a possibility that this may be detrimental to a child’s ability to learn and control emotions [45], which decreases people’s desire to converse with the child, potentially hindering their linguistic capacity. Over the age period from 2 to 5 years, children naturally begin to make evident linguistic progress in both receptive and progressive language to convey meanings, express opinions, and live with others through reception, interpretation, decision-making, and expression or gestures. Equipped with an easy temperament, a child can maintain a good mood, easily get along with others, acquire novel information, and effectively communicate with peers, solidifying the child’s linguistic ability to a greater extent. In addition, caregivers can offer stimuli, assistance, and caregiving to promote children with problematic temperaments in linguistic development to communicate better [46]. Caregivers’ strategies that emphasize a family’s relationship, prompt attention to a child’s needs, closeness, and attachment also positively affect the child’s language development [47].
On the contrary, other factors, including gender, gestational age, and caregivers’ age groups, were not associated with a suspected language development delay. Of the 174 children in this study that had a suspected language development delay, 37.80% were females and 45.10% were males. Research reports have suggested that male children were more susceptible to a suspected delay in language development than their female counterparts. This is consistent with the study on the correlation between anthropometric indices at birth and a developmental delay in children aged 4–60 months in Isfahan, where there was an increased rate of suspected delay in language development among male children [48]. Despite the lack of tangible evidence, the climbing tendency of the suspected delay in language development in male children is greater than that in female children, since abnormalities of the X-link are common among males. These are involved in a child’s suspected delay in language development [49,50]. In this study, 90 percent of the research samples were born at full-term. Therefore, gestational age did not have a dominant effect on the suspected delay in language development. However, some studies have shown that infants born prematurely, weighing between 1500 to 1999 g and delivered using a cesarean delivery, were prone to delayed development [51,52,53]. Most caregivers were within a 25–35-year age range and belonged to the working population, so they were more prepared for proper child-raising. Chances for mothers above 35 years to have a child with a suspected delay in development are higher than for those 25–35 years old. However, children born to young mothers have an increased rate of exhibiting a suspected delay in development in all aspects at 60 months. Such a risk can be reduced when children are born to older-aged mothers, and the correlation between health conditions and better development of children in early childhood is examined [54,55].

5. Conclusions

Delay in language development may include difficulties in learning and short attention spans, depriving the child of learning abilities at successive advanced levels. The study results revealed that the factors associated with a suspected delay in language development in early childhood include daily screen time exceeding 2 h and a moderate-to-difficult temperament. Therefore, nurses and health care providers should be aware of these factors and give information to caregivers to reduce the factors contributing to daily screen time and to provide appropriate responses that will help in a child’s language development.

6. Limitations and Scope of Future Research

Even though this study was exhaustive, some limitations still exist as follows. There were limitations regarding the location and sample selection. This study involved 425 children in early childhood who resided in Nakhon Si Thammarat Province. Nakhon Si Thammarat is one of the provinces in Southern Thailand and is considered an urban area, but various fields of employment for parents and caregivers were not taken into consideration in the rural areas. Thus, future studies should include children in early childhood and caregivers from all provinces in Thailand, including urban areas, rural areas, and parents or caregivers from various fields of employment. Therefore, the findings can be generalized to the early childhood population in Thailand.

Author Contributions

N.R. and K.K. designed and executed this study in this research, assisted with the data colletion, analyses, and wrote the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by an award to the first author through a grant from the Research Institute for Health Sciences and the Center of Excellence School of Nursing, Walailak University.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants followed the ethical standards of the Ethical Institutional Consideration. Ethical approval was obtained from the Office of the Human Research Ethics Committee of Walailak University, dated 16 March 2020, No. WUEC-20-060-01.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all parents or caregivers of the subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting this study’s findings are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The researchers greatly appreciate the Research Institute for Health Sciences of Walailak University for their support in research funding. Moreover, we would like to thank the Center of Excellence School of Nursing, Walailak University, for their help in driving this research forward. Lastly, we would like to sincerely thank the health care providers, presidents of the Health Promoting Hospitals, and the study participants for their kind and active cooperation.

Conflicts of Interest

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author.

References

  1. Polan, E.U. Life span development. In Foundation of Nursing, 3rd ed.; Christensen, L.B., Kockrow, O.E., Eds.; Mosby: St. Louis, MO, USA, 1999; pp. 86–117. [Google Scholar]
  2. Piaget, J. The Theory of Stages in Cognitive Development. In Measurement and Piaget; Green, D., Ford, M.P., Flamer, G.B., Eds.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1971; pp. 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  3. Roberts, M.Y.; Curtis, P.R.; Sone, B.J.; Hampton, L.H. Association of Parent Training With Child Language Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2019, 173, 671–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Kotchabhakdi, N.J.; Lertawasadatrakul, O. Manual for Training to Test Developmental Early Childhood: Denver II (Thai Version), 3rd ed.; The National Institute for Child and Family, Mahidol University: Nakhon Pathom, Thailand, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  5. Sirithongthaworn, S. Development of Manual for Promotion Developmental Child Aged Birth to 5 Years for Parents (TDSI 70 Items); Rajanagarindra Institute of Child Development: Chiang Mai, Thailand, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  6. Global Research on Developmental Disabilities Collaborators. Developmental disabilities among children younger than 5 years in 195 countries and territories, 1990–2016: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet Glob. Health 2018, 6, e1100–e1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. The National Institute of Child Development; Department of Health Ministry of Health. Annual Report 2017, 1st ed.; The National Institute of Child Development; Samcharoen Panich: Bangkok, Thailand, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  8. Zhou, S.; Zhao, C.; Huang, X.; Li, Z.; Ye, R.; Shi, H.; Zhao, Q.; Zhou, Y.; Chen, X.; O’Sullivan, M.; et al. The effect of a community-based, integrated and nurturing care intervention on early childhood development in rural China. Public Health 2019, 167, 125–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Feldman, R. Sensitive periods in human social development: New insights from research on oxytocin, synchrony, and high-risk parenting. Dev. Psychopathol. 2015, 27, 369–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hosseinpour, M.; Esmaeilpour, A.M.; Maleki, F. The prevalence and socioeconomic determinants of undetected developmental delay in preschool-aged children in the northwest of Iran: A population-based study. Early Child Dev. Care 2019, 14, 1624–1635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Pérez-Pereira, M. Prevalence of language delay among healthy preterm children, language outcomes and predictive factors. Children 2021, 8, 282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Suttora, C.; Guarini, A.; Zuccarini, M.; Aceti, A.; Corvaglia, L.; Sansavini, A. Speech and language skills of low-risk preterm and full-term late talkers: The role of child factors and parent input. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Adani, S.; Cepanec, M. Sex differences in early communication development: Behavioral and neurobiological indicators of more vulnerable communication system development in boys. Croat. Med. J. 2019, 60, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Collisson, B.A.; Graham, S.A.; Preston, J.L.; Rose, M.S.; Mc Donald, S.; Tough, S. Risk and protective factors for late talking: An epidemiologic investigation. J. Pediatr. 2016, 172, 168–174.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Tripathi, S. The digital youth’s personality and conscience versus their vision adjustment. J. Early Child. Educ. Manag. 2019, 1, 90–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ekathikhomkit, N. Child temperament and parental stress. JPNMH 2017, 31, 17–29. Available online: https://he02.tci-thaijo.org/index.php/JPNMH/article/view/110024 (accessed on 21 April 2022).
  17. Acar, I.H.; Torquati, J.C.; Encinger, A.; Colgrove, A. The role of child temperament on low-income preschool. Children’s relationships with their parents and teachers. Inf. Child Dev. 2018, 27, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Marrus, N.; Hall, L. Intellectual disability and language disorder. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2017, 26, 539–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Jang, Y.; Hong, Y.-J. The relationship between children’s temperament and fundamental movement skills mediated by autonomy and self-regulation. Early Child Dev. Care. 2020, 190, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Andreadakis, E.; Laurin, J.C.; Joussemet, M.; Mageau, G.A. Toddler temperament, parent stress, and autonomy support. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2020, 29, 3029–3043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Pattanapongthorn, J.; Boonsuwan, C.; Thanajaroenwatchara, N. The Study of Thai Early Child Development; The National Institute of Child Development; Department of Health Ministry of Health: Bangkok, Thailand, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  22. Potijk, M.R.; Kerstjens, J.M.; Bos, A.F.; Reijneveld, S.A.; de Winter, A.F. Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children with low socioeconomic status: Risks multiply. J. Pediatr. 2013, 163, 1289–1295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Phongphetdit, B.; Authawee, B. Factors affecting the development of early childhood Thailand: Public health region 5. KJN 2020, 27, 59–70. Available online: https://he01.tci-thaijo.org/index.php/kcn/article/view/167937 (accessed on 23 April 2022).
  24. Nobre, J.N.P.; Vinolas Prat, B.; Santos, J.N.; Santos, L.R.; Pereira, L.; Guedes, S.D.C.; Ribeiro, R.F.; Morais, R.L.D.S. Quality of interactive media use in early childhood and child development: A multicriteria analysis. J. Pediatr. (Rio J.) 2020, 96, 310–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kabali, H.K.; Irigoyen, M.M.; Nunez-Davis, R.; Budacki, J.G.; Mohanty, S.H.; Leister, K.P.; Bonner, R.L., Jr. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics 2015, 136, 1044–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Reid Chassiakos, Y.L.; Radesky, J.; Christakis, D.; Moreno, M.A.; Cross, C.; Council on Communications and Media; Hill, D.; Ameenuddin, N.; Hutchinson, J.; Levine, A.; et al. Children and adolescents and digital media. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20162593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Carter, B.; Rees, P.; Hale, L.; Bhattacharjee, D.; Paradkar, M.S. Association between portable screen-based media device access or use and sleep outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2016, 170, 1202–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Garrison, M.M.; Liekweg, K.; Christakis, D.A. Media use and child sleep: The impact of content, timing, and environment. Pediatrics 2011, 128, 29–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Lillard, A.S.; Li, H.; Boguszewski, K. Television and children’s executive function. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 2015, 48, 219–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Walsh, J.J.; Barnes, J.D.; Cameron, J.D.; Goldfield, G.S.; Chaput, J.-P.; Gunnell, K.E.; Ledoux, A.-A.; Zemek, R.L.; Tremblay, M.S. Associations between 24-hour movement behaviors and global cognition in US children: A cross-sectional observational study. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2018, 2, 783–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Siu, A.L. Screening for speech and language delay and disorders in children aged 5 years or younger: US preventive services task force recommendation statement. Pediatrics 2015, 136, e474–e481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Jullien, S. Screening for language and speech delay in children under five years. BMC Pediatr. 2021, 21, 362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Krejcie, R.V.; Morgan, D.W. Determining sample size for research activities. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1970, 30, 607–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sangsai, N. Predictors of Adjustment Behavior of Preschoolers Receiving Service in Daycare Centersm. Master’s Thesis, Burapha University, Saen Suk, Thailand, 2011. (In Thailand). [Google Scholar]
  35. Kanchana, S.; Kotchabhakdi, N.; Sirithongthaworn, S.; Isaranarug, S. Developmental Surveillance and Promotion Manual (DSPM); Chindasan Printing House: Ratchaburi, Thailand, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  36. World Health Organization. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behavior, and Sleep for Children under 5 Years of Age; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2019; Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664 (accessed on 23 April 2022).
  37. Byrne, R.; Terranova, C.O.; Trost, S.G. Measurement of screen time among young children aged 0–6 years: A systematic review. Obes. Rev. 2021, 22, e13260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Tang, L.; Darlington, G.; Ma, D.W.L.; Haines, J. Mothers’ and fathers’ media parenting practices associated with young children’s screen-time: A cross-sectional study. BMC Obes. 2018, 5, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Waisman, I.; Hidalgo, E.; Rossi, M.L. Screen use among young children in a city of Argentina. Uso de pantallas en niños pequeños en una ciudad de Argentina. Arch. Argent Pediatr. 2018, 116, e186–e195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Elias, N.; Nimrod, G.; Lemish, D. The ultimate treat? Young Israeli children’s media use under their grandparents’ care. J. Child. Media 2019, 13, 472–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Supanitayanon, S.; Trairatvorakul, P.; Chonchaiya, W. Screen media exposure in the first 2 years of life and preschool cognitive development: A longitudinal study. Pediatr. Res. 2020, 88, 894–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Lin, L.Y.; Cherng, R.J.; Chen, Y.J.; Chen, Y.J.; Yang, H.M. Effects of television exposure on developmental skills among young children. Infant Behav. Dev. 2015, 38, 20–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Krueger, R.F.; South, S.; Johnson, W.; Iacono, W. The heritability of personality is not always 50%: Gene-environment interactions and correlations between personality and parenting. J. Pers. 2008, 76, 1485–1522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Glynn, L.M.; Howland, M.A.; Sandman, C.A.; Davis, E.P.; Phelan, M.; Baram, T.Z.; Stern, H.S. Prenatal maternal mood patterns predict child temperament and adolescent mental health. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 228, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Klein, M.R.; Lengua, L.J.; Thompson, S.F.; Moran, L.; Ruberry, E.J.; Kiff, C.; Zalewski, M. Bidirectional Relations Between Temperament and Parenting Predicting Preschool-Age Children’s Adjustment. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2018, 47, S113–S126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Davison, L.; Warwick, H.; Campbell, K.; Gartstein, M. Infant temperament affects toddler language development. J. Educ. e-learn. Res. 2019, 6, 122–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Madigan, S.; Prime, H.; Graham, S.A.; Rodrigues, M.; Anderson, N.; Khoury, J.; Jenkins, J.M. Parenting Behavior and Child Language: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2019, 144, e20183556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Amir Ali Akbari, S.; Montazeri, S.; Torabi, F.; Amiri, S.; Soleimani, F.; Alavi Majd, H. Correlation between anthropometric indices at birth and developmental delay in children aged 4–60 months in Isfahan, Iran. Int. J. Gen. Med. 2012, 5, 683–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Srour, M.; Mazer, B.; Shevell, M.I. Analysis of clinical features predicting etiologic yield in the assessment of global developmental delay. Pediatrics 2006, 118, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Moeschler, J.B.; Shevell, M. Committee on Genetics. Comprehensive evaluation of the child with an intellectual disability or global developmental delays. Pediatrics 2014, 134, e903–e918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Donald, K.A.; Wedderburn, C.J.; Barnett, W.; Nhapi, R.T.; Rehman, A.M.; Stadler, J.A.M.; Hoffman, N.; Koen, N.; Zar, H.J.; Stein, D. Risk and protective factors for child development: An observational South African birth cohort. PLoS Med. 2019, 16, e1002920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Drozd-Dąbrowska, M.; Trusewicz, R.; Ganczak, M. Selected Risk factors of developmental delay in polish infants: A case-control study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Sharma, N.; Masood, J.; Singh, S.N.; Ahmad, N.; Mishra, P.; Bhattacharya, S. Assessment of risk factors for developmental delays among children in a rural community of North India: A cross-sectional study. J. Educ. Health Promot. 2019, 8, 112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Falster, K.; Hanly, M.; Banks, E.; Lynch, J.; Chambers, G.; Brownell, M.; Eades, S.; Jorm, L. Maternal age and offspring developmental vulnerability at age five: A population-based cohort study of Australian children. PLoS Med. 2018, 15, e1002558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Sutcliffe, A.G.; Barnes, J.; Belsky, J.; Gardiner, J.; Melhuish, E. The health and development of children born to older mothers in the United Kingdom: An observational study using longitudinal cohort data. BMJ 2012, 345, e5116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Table 1. The children’s language development status (n = 425).
Table 1. The children’s language development status (n = 425).
Language DevelopmentNumber%
Children with normal language development25159.10
Children with a suspected delay17440.90
Table 2. The demographic data of children (n = 425).
Table 2. The demographic data of children (n = 425).
Demographic Data of ChildrenNumber %
Gender
 Male18443.29
 Female24156.71
Religion
 Buddhism30672.00
 Islam11928.00
Age groups (Range 24–60, x ¯ = 40.90, S.D. = 9.04)
 24–36 months15235.76
 37–48 months18343.06
 49–60 months9021.18
Gestational age (weeks)
 <37286.59
 ≥3739793.41
Type of delivery
 Normal32376.00
 Cesarean 10224.00
Birth weight (g)
 <25007116.71
 2500–300020448.00
 >300015035.29
Daily screen time (hours)
 <231373.60
 >2 11226.40
Child’s temperament (Score range 59–169, x ¯ = 105.99, S.D. = 29.84)
 Easy temperament20247.50
 Moderate to difficult temperament22352.50
Table 3. The demographic data of parents or caregivers (n = 425).
Table 3. The demographic data of parents or caregivers (n = 425).
Demographic Data of Parents or CaregiversNumber %
Age (years)
 <257116.70
 25–35 24958.60
 >35 10524.70
Family characteristic
 Single-family26762.82
 Extended family15837.18
Relationship with the children
 Father348.00
 Mother34180.24
 Grandmother/Grandfather4410.35
 Other 61.41
Levels of education
 Primary education 5813.65
 Secondary education22753.41
 Diploma education5613.18
 Undergraduate/Graduate degree8419.76
Occupation
 Agriculturist8219.29
 Government employee/State Enterprises286.59
 Employment235.41
 Private business6615.53
 Trader10725.18
 Unemployed11928.00
Household income/ month (Thai baht)
 <10,000 13832.47
 10,000–20,000 22853.64
 20,001–30,000 5212.24
 >30,000 71.65
Table 4. Binary logistic regression analysis for exploring factors associated with a suspected delay in language development (n = 425).
Table 4. Binary logistic regression analysis for exploring factors associated with a suspected delay in language development (n = 425).
VariableLanguage
Development
Status
BSEWaldp-ValueCrude OR
(95% CI)
Adjusted OR
(95% CI)
Normal
N (%)
Suspected
N (%)
GenderFemale150 (62.20)91 (37.80) 11
Male101 (54.90)83 (45.10)0.280.280.960.3221.35
(0.91–2.00)
1.32
(0.75–2.33)
Gestational age (weeks)≥37237 (59.70)160 (40.30) 11
<3714 (50.00)14 (50.00)0.280.560.250.611.48
(0.68–3.19)
1.33
(0.44–4.02)
Daily screen time (hours)<2244 (78.00)69 (22.00) 11
>27(6.25)105 (93.75)2.850.4342.600.0053.04
(23.58–119.28)
17.30
(7.35–40.72)
Age of caregivers (years)<2538 (53.52)33 (46.48) 11
25–35152 (61.04)97 (38.96)−0.220.390.320.571.20
(0.65–2.20)
0.79
(0.36–1.73)
>3561 (58.09)44 (41.91)0.100.440.050.810.88
(0.55–1.40)
1.10
(0.46–2.62)
Child’s temperamentEasy temperament185 (91.58)17
(8.42)
11
Moderate-to-difficult temperament66 (29.60)157 (70.40)2.250.3150.280.0025.88
(14.58–45.95)
9.56
(5.12–17.85)
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rithipukdee, N.; Kusol, K. Factors Associated with the Suspected Delay in the Language Development of Early Childhood in Southern Thailand. Children 2022, 9, 662. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050662

AMA Style

Rithipukdee N, Kusol K. Factors Associated with the Suspected Delay in the Language Development of Early Childhood in Southern Thailand. Children. 2022; 9(5):662. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050662

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rithipukdee, Namfon, and Kiatkamjorn Kusol. 2022. "Factors Associated with the Suspected Delay in the Language Development of Early Childhood in Southern Thailand" Children 9, no. 5: 662. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050662

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop