Next Article in Journal
Recombinant Human Growth Hormone Therapy for Childhood Trichorhinophalangeal Syndrome Type I: A Case Report
Next Article in Special Issue
Active Video Games Performance and Heart Rate on the Wii or Kinect in Children with and without Developmental Coordination Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Lactoferrin as Possible Treatment for Chronic Gastrointestinal Symptoms in Children with Long COVID: Case Series and Literature Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Developmental Coordination Disorder and Most Prevalent Comorbidities: A Narrative Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

Sensory Processing Impairments in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder

1
Department of Physical Therapy, Graduate Institute of Rehabilitation Science, China Medical University, Taichung 40402, Taiwan
2
Department of Occupational Therapy, Asia University, Taichung 41354, Taiwan
3
Department of Physical Therapy, Asia University, Taichung 41354, Taiwan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Children 2022, 9(10), 1443; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9101443
Submission received: 1 September 2022 / Revised: 11 September 2022 / Accepted: 14 September 2022 / Published: 22 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Impact of Developmental Coordination Disorder on Children)

Abstract

:
The two objectives of this systematic review were to examine the following: (1) the difference in sensory processing areas (auditory, visual, vestibular, touch, proprioceptive, and multi-sensory) between children with and without developmental coordination disorder (DCD), and (2) the relationship between sensory processing and motor coordination in DCD. The following databases were comprehensively searched for relevant articles: PubMed, Science Direct, Web of Science, and Cochrane library. There were 1107 articles (published year = 2010 to 2021) found in the initial search. Full-text articles of all possibly relevant citations were obtained and inspected for suitability by two authors. The outcome measures were sensory processing impairments and their relationship with motor coordination. A total of 10 articles met the inclusion criteria. Children with DCD showed significant impairments in visual integration, tactile integration, proprioceptive integration, auditory integration, vestibular integration, and oral integration processes when compared with typically developing children. Evidence also supported that sensory processing impairments were associated with poor motor coordination in DCD. Preliminary support indicated that DCD have sensory processing impairments in visual, tactile, proprioceptive, auditory, and vestibular areas, which might contribute to participation restriction in motor activities. It is important to apply sensory integration therapy in rehabilitation programs for DCD in order to facilitate participation in daily activities.

1. Introduction

Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder, and its proportion is roughly 5–6% in children aged five to eleven years old [1,2]. According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-V), children with DCD are characterized by poor motor coordination when compared with age-matched children with typical development [3]. The deficiency in motor coordination occurs in the early developmental period, and children with DCD have shown participation restrictions in self-care, leisure, and physical activities.
Sensory processing is referred to as the capacity to manage the detection, modulation, interpretation, and organization of incoming sensory cues [4]. In Dunn’s model, sensory processing patterns are described as the interplay between people’s neurological thresholds and behavioral responses from self-regulation strategies [5]. From this view, there are four sensory processing patterns: low registration (high threshold and passive self-regulation), seeker (high threshold and active self-regulation), sensor (low threshold and passive self-regulation), and avoider (low threshold and active self-regulation). Furthermore, sensory processing patterns deeply influence a child’s behavior. A sizeable variation in the degree and pattern of perceptual–motor deficits has been shown in DCD children [3]. Some earlier literature demonstrated that deficiencies were found in visual–motor integration [6,7,8], impaired visual sensitivity [9], or visual–spatial processing in children with motor coordination problems. However, inconsistent findings regarding visual–perceptual ability have been found by previous studies. One study found that DCD children showed impaired visual sensitivity when differentiating children with typical development, whereas the other study showed no significant group difference [6,9]. In terms of tactile discrimination [10], two studies reported that children with DCD showed more deficits in proprioceptive processing function than children with typical development, such as the localization of single and double simultaneous stimuli, graphesthesia, fingers identification [11,12], and two-point discrimination in both moving and static conditions [10]. The above reviews indicated that children with poor motor coordination experienced predominately perceptual issues, but further study is required in order to examine the characteristics of visual or tactile processing function in children with DCD. Proprioceptive information not only affects the detection and correction of movement errors and the regulation of coordinated movement, but also influences individual participation in daily life [13,14]. Previous studies have reported that the considerable impairment in proprioceptive processing that leads to motor coordination problems has been found in DCD [15,16,17]. However, the empirical results with regard to proprioceptive abnormalities in children with DCD are inconsistent. Several studies have reported that DCD children have more impairments in proprioceptive processing function than typically developing children [16,17,18], while other studies reported that there was no association between proprioceptive dysfunction and motor coordination problems in DCD [19,20,21,22].
The below existing evidence indicates that coordination deficit and sensory processing difficulty are primary obstacles in DCD children. Misaki et al. (2018) found that abnormal sensory processing function might cause the pathophysiology of DCD [23]. Another study also reported that there were some difficulties in sensory processing and sensory integration in children with DCD, which may, in turn, hinder these children from participating in routine activities [24]. Poor sensorimotor integration is one of the most implicated causes of motor coordination challenges in DCD children. Wilson et al. (2017) indicated that diminished white matter organization in sensorimotor structures varied the transmittance of structural connectivity in the whole brain network in children with DCD; this could affect their motor skills [25]. Zwicker JG et al. (2012) demonstrated that decreased axial diffusivity in the motor and sensory tracts was the key cause of DCD [26]. Imaging studies reported that the left superior and inferior parietal lobules were less activated in children with DCD while doing continuous and visuomotor tracking tasks [27]. The above literature has shown that there is a possibility of a relationship between sensory processing function and motor coordination competence in children with DCD; this needs further study in order to explore and interpret the underlying mechanism and design efficient intervention approaches for DCD children.
Therefore, the two aims of this study were to systematically review the literature and to better understand (1) the difference in sensory processing areas (auditory, visual, vestibular, touch, proprioceptive, and multi-sensory) between children with and without DCD, and (2) the relationship between sensory processing function and motor coordination competence in DCD.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Search Strategy

The methodology used in the study was based on the guidelines from the Cochrane Handbook [28]. The protocol of this systematic review was previously published in the PROSPERO database, and the registration number is CRD42021249197. A comprehensive search for sensory processing functions that have been used to measure sensory processing was administrated within multiple computerized databases, including PubMed, Science Direct, Web of Science, and Cochrane library. The following Medical Subject Headings (MeSHs) were used: (“sensory” OR “sensorimotor” OR “sensory processing” OR “sensory deficit”) AND (“developmental coordination disorder” OR “DCD” OR “coordination disorder” OR “clumsy” OR “dyspraxia”). In addition, we also read references from SELECTED studies to find other eligible papers.

2.2. Inclusion Criteria

The following were the criteria for a study to be included: (1) The study population should be children with DCD. (2) The studies aimed to inspect both the sensory processing impairment and its relationship with motor coordination. That is, a standardized measure or scale for assessing the sensory processing function or motor coordination competence was used. (3) A control group of typically developing (TD) children was recruited. (4) The full text article was written in English and was published between 2010 to 2021.

2.3. Exclusion Criteria

Papers were excluded if participants with DCD had other comorbidity medical diagnoses, such as autism spectrum disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

2.4. Outcome Targets

The studies aimed to examine both the sensory processing impairment and its relationship with motor coordination. A standardized measure or scale for assessing the sensory processing function or motor coordination competence needed to be used by the studies.

2.5. Data Extraction and Analysis

The Newcastle–Ottawa Quality (NOQ) Assessment Scale [29] was utilized to access the methodological quality of each study based on the suggestions of the Cochrane Non- Randomized Studies Methods Working Group for case-control and cohort studies. The nine criteria of the NOQ were composed of three key domains: (1) selection of study groups (four criteria), (2) comparability of study groups (one criterion), and (3) ascertainment of the outcome of interest (three criteria). This scale utilized a star rating system for each study with a total score ranging from 0 to 9 stars. More stars indicated a higher study quality. Two authors (Hung-Yu Lin and Pei-Jung Wang) evaluated the quality assessment independently, with discrepancies resolved by the other author (Yao-Chuen Li) (Table 1).

3. Results

Figure 1 shows the entire process of searching and selecting the studies for inclusion in this systematic review. A total of 1107 studies were found in the databases, and 4 studies were found in the additional records identified through other sources from the initial search. After accounting for duplicates, we reviewed the titles and abstracts of 1055 articles; 840 articles were excluded based on our inclusion criteria. Most of the studies demonstrated topics not equivalent to the subject of our review. We reviewed the full text of the remaining 34 articles, and considered a total of 10 articles that fitted the inclusion criteria. The reasons for excluding the 24 full-text articles were no control group; the DCD group not excluding children with other disorders such as ADHD, dyspraxia, and intellectual disability; and no examination of the association between sensory impairment and motor dysfunction.
All 10 articles were cross-sectional studies. Table 2 outlines the study characteristics. The age range of participants were from 5 to 14 years. Different outcome measures were conducted to assess sensory processing dysfunction, including sensory profile (two studies), visual–tactile temporal order judgment task (one study), haptic detection/discrimination task (one study), the developmental test of visual–motor integration (one study), the test of visual perceptual skills (two studies), Semmes Weinstein Monofilaments: Registration (one study), single-point localization (one study), two-point discrimination (one study), Ayres Southern California Sensory Integration Test, and Doing study’s design task (two studies).
Regarding sensory processing, this systematic review categorized them into six processes: visual integration process, touch integration process, proprioceptive integration process, auditory integration process, vestibular integration process, and others. Four studies examined the difference in the impairment of the visual integration process between DCD children and children developing typically. All of the studies indicated a significantly lower visual integration process in DCD. Four studies demonstrated that DCD children had a significantly lower touch integration process score than their TD peers. In addition, two studies found that children with DCD had difficulty in the proprioceptive integration process when compared with the TD group. Two studies reported impairments of the auditory integration process and the vestibular integration process in children with DCD, separately. Figure 2 represents an overview of the sensory deficit in children with DCD.
Figure 2 shows that there is no evidence of a mechanoreceptor or peripheral nervous abnormal to explain proprioceptive impairment in DCD. Previous studies have found impairments in sensory integration in children with DCD. Regarding the sensory processing pattern, they have a lower registration (i.e., hypo-responsiveness to sensory stimuli) and more sensory sensitivity and sensation avoiding compared with TD children. Furthermore, dysfunctional processes of sensorimotor processing (motor planning or motor execution) also accounted as underlying for the sensory deficit in DCD. And reducing short-term sensory memory was found in children with DCD that also contribute to the sensory impairment in DCD. Moreover, compare with the other sensory area, DCD tend to reply on visual sense (having visual bias).
Table 3 presents the findings of the associations between sensory processing impairment and motor coordination. Two articles explored the relationship by using the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire—DCDQ. Six studies reported the association of sensory processing and motor coordination measured with the Movement Assessment Battery for Children test—MABC. One study used Jensen–Taylor Test of Hand Function [31] and the Evaluation Tool of Children’s Handwriting [32] and one used the Functional Independence Measure for Children. Figure 3 represents the overview of the correlation between sensory deficit and motor function in DCD children. The significant correlations between sensory processing and the subscale scores on the DCDQ were shown in this study, and two studies indicated a significant correlation between sensory processing area and the MABC-2 total and subscale scores.

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first systematic review to assess sensory impairment (auditory, vestibular, visual, touch, proprioceptive, and multi-sensory) in children with DCD when compared with children developing typically. According to previous reviews, it was conspicuous that the number of children with DCD was comparatively high, but the study findings for the sensory deficit were narrow. In a limited number of studies, minimal studies allocated in some parameters of sensory integration were associated with motor obstacles. Additionally, the studies we included aimed to find the connection between sensory integration deficits and motor coordination problems in DCD children who did not have comorbidity with ASD and ADHD.

4.1. Evidence Synthesis for Sensory Impairment in DCD Compared with Non-DCD

Regarding the touch integration process, children with DCD showed more deficiencies in somatosensory. Touch atypicalness might be a prevailing, but less recognized, feature in children with DCD. Registration and perception are two phases composed of tactile function [33]. A tactile stimulus is detected initially and essentially through the registration or sensitivity phase—the forerunner to perception. Subsequently, the sensory input, based on the spatial, temporal, and modality-specific components of a stimulus, is interpreted and gives meaning in the tactile perception or acuity phase, i.e., the smallest perceivable difference between two recognizable curves [34,35]. Therefore, it is crucial to assess both phases to determine the level and severity of the tactile dysfunction [33]. Major studies have found that the magnitude of the diminishing touch acuity was impaired in children with DCD, but no study investigated the haptic sensitivity impairment in DCD. The other quantitative study demonstrated that the difficulty in the sensory integration of touch information was one of the problems in DCD [36]. An alternative study indicated that children with DCD showed an elevated threshold for tactile-related sense, e.g., light touch [37].
Regarding our systematic review, two studies investigated the impairment of proprioceptive in DCD: one allocated the proprioceptive in the upper limbs, and the other in the lower limbs. Both studies found that joint position sense acuity was reduced, indicating proprioceptive deficits in children with DCD. This means that they had a decreased ability to identify joint position through proprioception. Moreover, there is extensive evidence that children with DCD showed greater proprioceptive impairment in distal joints than proximal joints; and this result corroborates previous studies reporting on patients with stroke and intracranial disorders [38,39]. An innovation study using a Sensory profile indicated that lower registration (i.e., hypo-responsiveness to sensory stimuli) was shown in DCD [23,40]; which regarding proprioceptive stimuli detection difficulty.
Regarding the visual integration process, DCD children revealed a substantially more inferior performance compared with TD. Before starting the examination, Nobusako et al. (2021) used a simple stimulus test to demonstrate that visual function was typical in DCD children. An earlier investigation measuring visual evoked potentials indicated that the performance of DCD was similar to TD [41]. The four studies related to visual abilities included in this review mainly estimated the diverse aspects of the visual–perceptual capabilities in DCD. The major findings were that there was a notable decrease in accuracy of the visual–perceptual performance in DCD children compared with those with TD. For visual sensitivity tasks, children with DCD showed a remarkably low sensitivity compared with typical development children. The above finding indicates that DCD had a significantly higher threshold than TD. In signal processing, DCD children tended to rely on visual bias more than the TD children.
Only one study indicated impairment in the auditory, vestibular, and oral processing in children with DCD. In fact, there are various previous experiments that corroborate this finding. The auditory and vestibular sensory integration difficulties, using parent-reporting questionnaires, were found in DCD children from 5 to 12 years old in Allen and Casey’s study [24]. Furthermore, DCD children in the early child period could have trouble with oral movement, for example, eating difficulties and speech/language challenges, because of the oral sensory processing problems [42,43,44].
Fundamentally, Figure 2 represents an overview of the sensory deficits in children with DCD according to our reviews. When the stimuli reach the body in children with DCD, sensory detection—the amounts of sensory stimuli or sensory threshold, needed by a person for recognizing and responding—decreases in the touch and proprioceptive areas, but is normal in the visual, auditory, and vestibular areas. This means that children with DCD need a higher threshold to detect the stimuli in touch and proprioceptive perception. The stimuli, after that, enter the brain for analysis, which is usually called sensory integration or sensory process. Here, almost all visual, touch, proprioceptive, auditory, and vestibular integration encounter problems; this sensory processing difficulty (SPD) has also been proven in some DCD phenotypes in earlier studies [24,45,46]. They displayed a decline in sensory discrimination—the ability to recognize various aspects of stimuli and to distinguish one sensory experience from another—in all sensory senses. Maybe this makes them inaccurate at identifying the differences in stimulus. In addition to the problem of sensory integration, DCD children did not actively reply to the sensory stimuli, which was demonstrated by an increase in sensory sensitivity and low registration. In the same manner, they tended to avoid the sensory input because they felt it much more intensely than the children developing typically did. Children with DCD were also overwhelmed by sensory input, so they showed an increase in sensation avoiding. As a result, DCD children struggled to perform motor coordination challenges because of difficulties in sensory process/integration.

4.2. Evidence Synthesis for the Relationship between Motor Performance and Sensory Processing in DCD

There was an association between low thresholds in sensory processing (sensory avoiding and sensory sensitivity) and fine and gross motor problems, and low registration associated with control during movement, general coordination, and the DCDQ-ES total score [23]. Allen and Casey also determined low registration issues in DCD, which was related to approximately 24–33% body awareness and balance difficulties in this population [24]. The association between DCD and passive self-regulation strategies was supported by the above finding. Thus, it was demonstrated that these children might not attempt to respond actively to complexities in stimuli perception.
The association between impairment in tactile integration and fine motor coordination problems in children with DCD was also revealed. Tseng et al., 2019, demonstrated that higher haptic discrimination thresholds were correlated with lower three MABC-2 sub-scores. This indicates that children with higher haptic discrimination thresholds have a tendency to show poorer motor coordination. This finding was similar to a recent study that investigated the correlation between tactile and motor function in children with and without DCD at elementary school [47]. The execution and feedback movement of the upper limb, after receiving stimulus, could be affected by a decrease in tactile spatial perception, proposing that tactile function plays an important role in handwriting proficiency. As a result, inaccurate errors in higher-order processing in the form of spatial tactile perception might contribute to the upper limb coordination problems experienced in DCD children.
Our reviews indicated a connection between proprioceptive deficit and motor in DCD. The severity of proprioceptive deficits in knee and ankle joints was negatively associated with balance performance in children with DCD [48]. This implies that the proprioceptive status in the lower extremities predicted the balance ability in DCD. Proprioceptive JND thresholds were negatively correlated with the manual dexterity score in MABC-2, indicating that children’s wrists with higher thresholds of proprioceptive might have poorer fine motor control [49]. Similarly, upper limb proprioceptive status could serve as a predictor of lower limb balance function, as JND wrist thresholds were also strongly correlated with the MABC-2 balance score.
We also found the tendency that a large number of DCD children showed an increase in visual bias, which might be correlated with their decreased manual dexterity. In the present systematic reviews, children with DCD were illustrated to perform poorer in tasks such as static visual discrimination, visual sequential memory, and sequential coupling of eye and hand. These would cause a decline in the speed of achievement of MABC-2 or daily tasks. According to the mentioned findings, children with DCD could have difficulties in integrating visual signals to accomplish motor duties. Therefore, when evaluating motor functions, it is necessary to carefully assess visual–perceptual ability.
Another finding of one study demonstrated the relationship between auditory processing difficulties and inadequate motor coordination, suggesting that auditory processing (sensory) dysfunction and motor coordination problems originated from the equivalent underlying neural mechanism that involved the cerebellum. Further study would be required to explore the underlying neural mechanism of the association between auditory processing and motor coordination in toddlers with DCD.

4.3. Explain the Underlying Mechanism of the Sensory Impairment

The abnormality of the proprioceptive mechanism underpinning DCD is poorly understood, and no study revealed the identification of a distinct neural signature of DCD [50,51]. Notably, several studies found that DCD children had a tendency to show hypotonia [52,53]. With low muscle tone, the length of the skeletal muscle fiber is longer than normal when the muscle is relaxed and/or during a sustained voluntary contraction. Here, the internal force generated by muscle contraction and position or movement of body limbs may not be sufficient to trigger muscle spindle activation. Therefore, this resulted in a low firing rate of proprioceptive afferents, which could further lead to joint position sense deficits in children with DCD. Moreover, one review pointed out a significant association between motor coordination performance and joint hypermobility/joint hyperlaxity [54]. The excessive joint mobility demonstrated a significantly poorer joint proprioception [55] because of the repetitive stresses on joints. The repeated stress on joints damaged receptors, and it diminished joint proprioceptor activation following from capsular or ligamentous stretching [56]. Additionally, as mentioned previously, a higher tactile threshold could account for a reduction in proprioceptive performance in the joints for children with DCD. Correspondingly, dystonia and Parkinson’s disease, which affect the cortico-basal ganglia–thalamo–cortical circuitry, detected similar insufficiency in the proprioceptive acuity [57]. Maybe this mechanism is influenced in children with DCD. Thus, the significant difference found in proprioceptive acuity between joints may explain the possible lesions in certain afferent pathways and/or atypical activations in the proprioceptive-processing neural area in children with DCD [48]. Future research should systematically investigate the possible causes of impaired proprioception in children with DCD in the peripheral nervous system, e.g., muscle spindle sensitivity, the total number of joint mechanoreceptors, intrafusal, and chain fibers. More studies are required to provide possible mechanisms of proprioception difficulties in children with DCD caused by the central nervous system (neural activity in the premotor cortex, cerebellum, and the proprioceptive regions of the basal ganglia and grey matter volume in the precentral gyri, postcentral gyri, insula, and angular gyri). In addition, another remarkable finding was that compared with children developing typically, children with DCD showed a low registration regarding proprioceptive stimuli detection. Low registration issues frequently include challenges in proprioceptive stimuli. Proprioceptive stimuli detection is important for body awareness and balance, and it involved the conscious and subconscious awareness of spatial and kinesthetic parameters of the musculoskeletal framework [58].
The action system is intimately connected with visual perception concerns, including both object recognition and determination in space. This is why if there is any failure in this processing network, it can influence the restriction in movement planning, correction, and feedback [8]. Bonifacci highlighted a demonstrated lower visual–motor processing capacity, but uninfluenced perceptual skills in children at risk for DCD [6]. However, the visual feedback process in DCD is managed differently and much more slowly compared with normal children, according to recent research [59,60]. Maybe atypical brain function leads children with DCD to tend to have visual bias. Evidently, they activate more visual cortex areas during accomplishment tasks when using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to evaluate the brain activity [61]. This indicates that DCD children depend more on visual feedback when performing tasks. Similarly, diminishing parietal cortex activity while increasing the activity in the visual cortex, compared with peers [27], also indicates that children with DCD mainly depended on their visual sense to achieve tasks [27,61]. Furthermore, DCD revealed a decrease in the inferior parietal lobule [62], which involves visual and tactile integration [63] and is associated with manual dexterity. Accordingly, the moderate association between increased visual bias in DCD and reduced manual dexterity may be correlated with decreased activity in the inferior parietal region.
Finally, the abnormality in the cerebellum may be responsible for underlying mechanisms of motor coordination problems and sensory processing difficulties in children with DCD, especially auditory sensory [64,65]. Besides, the normal action of the cerebellum involves making motor automatic, abnormality in the cerebellum gives a possible explanation of why children with DCD tended to passively counter with the stimuli. However, the mechanism accounting for oral process problems in children with DCD remains unclear.

5. Conclusions

The 10 studies analyzed in the current review reported the general sensory deficits and the link between sensory processing and motor coordination difficulties in DCD. Our reviews also illustrated that the pathophysiology of children with DCD involved an abnormality of sensory integration, and suggest the importance of assessing sensory processing functions. The problems were mainly due to inappropriate methods of integrating information from visual, tactile, proprioceptive, auditory, vestibular, and oral senses. The above challenges caused restriction in the participation of DCD children in daily activities. Therefore, we also recommend adding sensory integration therapy into rehabilitation programs in order to facilitate DCD children’s developmental competence and participation in daily activities.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and methodology, H.-T.T. and S.-D.L.; writing—original draft preparation, H.-T.T., S.-D.L. and P.-J.W.; writing—review and editing, H.-T.T., Y.-C.L., H.-Y.L., S.-D.L. and P.-J.W.; visualization, H.-T.T.; supervision, S.-D.L. and P.-J.W.; project administration, P.-J.W.; funding acquisition, P.-J.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Missiuna, C.; Gaines, R.; McLean, J.; Delaat, D.; Egan, M.; Soucie, H. Description of children identified by physicians as having developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 50, 839–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Blank, R.; Barnett, A.L.; Cairney, J.; Green, D.; Kirby, A.; Polatajko, H.; Rosenblum, S.; Smits-Engelsman, B.; Sugden, D.; Wilson, P.; et al. International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2019, 61, 242–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Tsai, C.L.; Wilson, P.H.; Wu, S.K. Role of visual-perceptual skills (non-motor) in children with developmental coordination disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008, 27, 649–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Lane, S.J.; Miller, L.J.; Hanft, B.E. Toward a consensus in terminology in sensory integration theory and practice: Part 2: Sensory integration patterns of function and dysfunction. Sens. Integr. Spec. Interest Sect. Q. 2000, 23, 1–3. [Google Scholar]
  5. Dunn, W.; Little, L.; Dean, E.; Robertson, S.; Evans, B. The State of the Science on Sensory Factors and Their Impact on Daily Life for Children: A Scoping Review. OTJR (Thorofare N. J.) 2016, 36, 3s–26s. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Bonifacci, P. Children with low motor ability have lowe.r visual-motor integration ability but unaffected perceptual skills. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2004, 23, 157–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Van Waelvelde, H.; De Weerdt, W.; De Cock, P.; Smits-Engelsman, B.C. Aspects of the validity of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2004, 23, 49–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Wilson, P.H.; McKenzie, B.E. Information processing deficits associated with developmental coordination disorder: A meta-analysis of research findings. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1998, 39, 829–840. [Google Scholar]
  9. Sigmundsson, H.; Hansen, P.C.; Talcott, J.B. Do ‘clumsy’ children have visual deficits. Behav. Brain Res. 2003, 139, 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Law, S.H.; Lo, S.K.; Chow, S.; Cheing, G.L. Grip force control is dependent on task constraints in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 2011, 34, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Elbasan, B.; Kayihan, H.; Duzgun, I. Sensory integration and activities of daily living in children with developmental coordination disorder. Ital. J. Pediatr. 2012, 38, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Malloy-Miller, T.; Polatajko, H.; Anstett, B. Handwriting Error Patterns of Children with Mild Motor Difficulties. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 1995, 62, 258–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Carey, L.M.; Oke, L.E.; Matyas, T.A. Impaired Touch Discrimination After Stroke: A Quantiative Test. J. Neurol. Rehabil. 1997, 11, 219–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Xu, D.; Hong, Y.; Li, J.; Chan, K. Effect of tai chi exercise on proprioception of ankle and knee joints in old people. Br. J. Sports Med. 2004, 38, 50–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Bairstow, P.J.; Laszlo, J.I. Kinaesthetic sensitivity to passive movements and its relationship to motor development and motor control. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1981, 23, 606–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Coleman, R.; Piek, J.P.; Livesey, D.J. A longitudinal study of motor ability and kinaesthetic acuity in young children at risk of developmental coordination disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2001, 20, 95–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Li, K.Y.; Su, W.J.; Fu, H.W.; Pickett, K.A. Kinesthetic deficit in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2015, 38, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Piek, J.P.; Coleman-Carman, R. Kinaesthetic sensitivity and motor performance of children with developmental co-ordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1995, 37, 976–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Adams, I.L.; Ferguson, G.D.; Lust, J.M.; Steenbergen, B.; Smits-Engelsman, B.C. Action planning and position sense in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2016, 46, 196–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Smyth, M.M.; Mason, U.C. Planning and execution of action in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 1023–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Hoare, D.; Larkin, D. Kinaesthetic abilities of clumsy children. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1991, 33, 671–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Lord, R.; Hulme, C. Kinaesthetic sensitivity of normal and clumsy children. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1987, 29, 720–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Mikami, M.; Hirota, T.; Takahashi, M.; Adachi, M.; Saito, M.; Koeda, S.; Yoshida, K.; Sakamoto, Y.; Kato, S.; Nakamura, K.; et al. Atypical Sensory Processing Profiles and Their Associations With Motor Problems In Preschoolers With Developmental Coordination Disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2021, 52, 311–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Allen, S.; Casey, J. Developmental coordination disorders and sensory processing and integration: Incidence, associations and co-morbidities. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 2017, 80, 549–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Wilson, P.H.; Smits-Engelsman, B.; Caeyenberghs, K.; Steenbergen, B.; Sugden, D.; Clark, J.; Mumford, N.; Blank, R. Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in developmental coordination disorder: New insights from a systematic review of recent research. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2017, 59, 1117–1129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Zwicker, J.G.; Missiuna, C.; Harris, S.R.; Boyd, L.A. Developmental coordination disorder: A pilot diffusion tensor imaging study. Pediatr. Neurol. 2012, 46, 162–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kashiwagi, M.; Iwaki, S.; Narumi, Y.; Tamai, H.; Suzuki, S. Parietal dysfunction in developmental coordination disorder: A functional MRI study. Neuroreport 2009, 20, 1319–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Julian, P.T.H.; Sally, G. (Eds.) Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK; Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  29. Sanderson, S.; Tatt, L.D.; Higgins, J.P.T. Tools for assessing quality and susceptibility to bias in observational studies in epidemiology: A systematic review and annotated bibliography. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 36, 666–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dunn, W. The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children and their families: A conceptual model. Infants Young Child. 1997, 9, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Jebsen, R.H.; Taylor, N.; Trieschmann, R.B.; Trotter, M.J.; Howard, L.A. An objective and standardized test of hand function. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1969, 50, 311–319. [Google Scholar]
  32. Amundson, S.J. Evaluation Tool of Children’s Handwriting: ETCH Examiner’s Manual; O.T. KIDS: Homer, AK, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  33. Auld, M.L.; Boyd, R.N.; Moseley, G.L.; Johnston, L.M. Tactile assessment in children with cerebral palsy: A clinimetric review. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2011, 31, 413–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Auld, M.L.; Boyd, R.N.; Moseley, G.L.; Ware, R.S.; Johnston, L.M. Impact of tactile dysfunction on upper-limb motor performance in children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2012, 93, 696–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Auld, M.L.; Ware, R.S.; Boyd, R.N.; Moseley, G.L.; Johnston, L.M. Reproducibility of tactile assessments for children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2012, 32, 151–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Bair, W.N.; Kiemel, T.; Jeka, J.J.; Clark, J.E. Development of multisensory reweighting for posture control in children. Exp. Brain Res. 2007, 183, 435–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Chen, F.C.; Li, L.L.; Chu, C.H.; Pan, C.Y.; Tsai, C.L. Finger soaking enhances effects of light touch on reducing body sway in children with developmental coordination disorder. J. Rehabil. Med. 2019, 51, 217–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lincoln, N.B.; Jackson, J.; Adams, S.A. Reliability and Revision of the Nottingham Sensory Assessment for Stroke Patients. Physiotherapy 1998, 84, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Stolk-Hornsveld, F.; Crow, J.L.; Hendriks, E.P.; van der Baan, R.; Harmeling-van der Wel, B.C. The Erasmus MC modifications to the (revised) Nottingham Sensory Assessment: A reliable somatosensory assessment measure for patients with intracranial disorders. Clin. Rehabil. 2006, 20, 160–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Delgado-Lobete, L.; Pértega-Díaz, S.; Santos-Del-Riego, S.; Montes-Montes, R. Sensory processing patterns in developmental coordination disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2020, 100, 103608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Mon-Williams, M.A.; Mackie, R.T.; McCulloch, D.L.; Pascal, E. Visual evoked potentials in children with developmental coordination disorder. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 1996, 16, 178–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hill, E.L. Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2001, 36, 149–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gaines, R.; Missiuna, C. Early identification: Are speech/language-impaired toddlers at increased risk for Developmental Coordination Disorder? Child Care Health Dev. 2007, 33, 325–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Summers, J.; Larkin, D.; Dewey, D. Activities of daily living in children with developmental coordination disorder: Dressing, personal hygiene, and eating skills. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008, 27, 215–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Gomez, A.; Sirigu, A. Developmental coordination disorder: Core sensori-motor deficits, neurobiology and etiology. Neuropsychologia 2015, 79, 272–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Mimouni-Bloch, A.; Offek, H.; Rosenblum, S.; Posener, I.; Silman, Z.; Engel-Yeger, B. Association between sensory modulation and daily activity function of children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and children with typical development. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 83, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Cox, L.E.; Harris, E.C.; Auld, M.L.; Johnston, L.M. Impact of tactile function on upper limb motor function in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2015, 46, 373–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Chen, F.C.; Pan, C.Y.; Chu, C.H.; Tsai, C.L.; Tseng, Y.T. Joint position sense of lower extremities is impaired and correlated with balance function in children with developmental coordination disorder. J. Rehabil. Med. 2020, 52, jrm00088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Tseng, Y.T.; Tsai, C.L.; Chen, F.C.; Konczak, J. Wrist position sense acuity and its relation to motor dysfunction in children with developmental coordination disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 2018, 674, 106–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Biotteau, M.; Chaix, Y.; Blais, M.; Tallet, J.; Péran, P.; Albaret, J.M. Neural Signature of DCD: A Critical Review of MRI Neuroimaging Studies. Front. Neurol. 2016, 7, 227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Peters, L.H.; Maathuis, C.G.; Hadders-Algra, M. Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2013, 55 (Suppl. 4), 59–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hadders-Algra, M. Developmental coordination disorder: Is clumsy motor behavior caused by a lesion of the brain at early age? Neural Plast. 2003, 10, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Vaivre-Douret, L.; Lalanne, C.; Ingster-Moati, I.; Boddaert, N.; Cabrol, D.; Dufier, J.L.; Golse, B.; Falissard, B. Subtypes of developmental coordination disorder: Research on their nature and etiology. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011, 36, 614–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Romeo, D.M.; Venezia, I.; De Biase, M.; Ascione, F.; Lala, M.R.; Arcangeli, V.; Mercuri, E.; Brogna, C. Developmental Coordination Disorder and Joint Hypermobility in Childhood: A Narrative Review. Children 2022, 9, 1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Fatoye, F.; Palmer, S.; Macmillan, F.; Rowe, P.; van der Linden, M. Proprioception and muscle torque deficits in children with hypermobility syndrome. Rheumatology 2009, 48, 152–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Mallik, A.K.; Ferrell, W.R.; McDonald, A.G.; Sturrock, R.D. Impaired proprioceptive acuity at the proximal interphalangeal joint in patients with the hypermobility syndrome. Br. J. Rheumatol. 1994, 33, 631–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Konczak, J.; Corcos, D.M.; Horak, F.; Poizner, H.; Shapiro, M.; Tuite, P.; Volkmann, J.; Maschke, M. Proprioception and motor control in Parkinson’s disease. J. Mot. Behav. 2009, 41, 543–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Chu, V.W.T. Assessing Proprioception in Children: A Review. J. Mot. Behav. 2017, 49, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Wilson, P.H.; Maruff, P.; Butson, M.; Williams, J.; Lum, J.; Thomas, P.R. Internal representation of movement in children with developmental coordination disorder: A mental rotation task. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2004, 46, 754–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Zoia, S.; Castiello, U.; Blason, L.; Scabar, A. Reaching in children with and without developmental coordination disorder under normal and perturbed vision. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005, 27, 257–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Zwicker, J.G.; Missiuna, C.; Harris, S.R.; Boyd, L.A. Brain activation of children with developmental coordination disorder is different than peers. Pediatrics 2010, 126, e678–e686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Fuelscher, I.; Caeyenberghs, K.; Enticott, P.G.; Williams, J.; Lum, J.; Hyde, C. Differential activation of brain areas in children with developmental coordination disorder during tasks of manual dexterity: An ALE meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 86, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Gentile, G.; Petkova, V.I.; Ehrsson, H.H. Integration of visual and tactile signals from the hand in the human brain: An FMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 2011, 105, 910–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Zwicker, J.G.; Missiuna, C.; Harris, S.R.; Boyd, L.A. Brain activation associated with motor skill practice in children with developmental coordination disorder: An fMRI study. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2011, 29, 145–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Baumann, O.; Borra, R.J.; Bower, J.M.; Cullen, K.E.; Habas, C.; Ivry, R.B.; Leggio, M.; Mattingley, J.B.; Molinari, M.; Moulton, E.A.; et al. Consensus paper: The role of the cerebellum in perceptual processes. Cerebellum 2015, 14, 197–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. This is a figure. Schemes follow the same formatting.
Figure 1. This is a figure. Schemes follow the same formatting.
Children 09 01443 g001
Figure 2. Summary diagram of the sensory deficit in children with developmental coordination disorder; (1), (3)–(10): indicated the study in Table 2 show information given in the diagram. Dunn’s Model of Sensory Processing [30].
Figure 2. Summary diagram of the sensory deficit in children with developmental coordination disorder; (1), (3)–(10): indicated the study in Table 2 show information given in the diagram. Dunn’s Model of Sensory Processing [30].
Children 09 01443 g002
Figure 3. Summary diagram of the correlation between sensory processing and motor coordination in children with DCD. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; OR: double off in the right; DL: double left; DR: double right; SL: single in left in the sequential coupling of eye and hand. In this test, a yellow indication and three feasible target circles with the identical diameter of 10 mm were demonstrated on the screen at the left and right side of the midline individually, and the yellow indication was at the far left and far right locations. Three circumstances with two sides (left or right) were considered: single (one target), double (two targets), and double-off (two targets appeared, then disappeared) (abbreviated as SL, SR, DL, DR, OL, and OR, sequentially). (1); (4); (8): indicated the study in Table 3 showed information given in the diagram.
Figure 3. Summary diagram of the correlation between sensory processing and motor coordination in children with DCD. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; OR: double off in the right; DL: double left; DR: double right; SL: single in left in the sequential coupling of eye and hand. In this test, a yellow indication and three feasible target circles with the identical diameter of 10 mm were demonstrated on the screen at the left and right side of the midline individually, and the yellow indication was at the far left and far right locations. Three circumstances with two sides (left or right) were considered: single (one target), double (two targets), and double-off (two targets appeared, then disappeared) (abbreviated as SL, SR, DL, DR, OL, and OR, sequentially). (1); (4); (8): indicated the study in Table 3 showed information given in the diagram.
Children 09 01443 g003
Table 1. Results of risk of bias assessment of the selected studies are based on the Newcastle—Ottawa quality assessment scale.
Table 1. Results of risk of bias assessment of the selected studies are based on the Newcastle—Ottawa quality assessment scale.
STUDYSelectionComparabilityExposureScore/Stars
12341a1b123
1. Mikami et al., 2021 [23]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-6/9
2. Delgado-Lobete et al., 2020 [30]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-7/9
3. Nobusako et al., 2021 [31]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-7/9
4. Chen et al., 2020 [32]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-7/9
5. Tseng et al., 2019 [33]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-7/9
6. Tseng et al., 2018 [34]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-7/9
7. Prunty et al., 2016 [35]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-7/9
8. Cheng et al., 2014 [36]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-7/9
9. Cox et al., 2015 [37]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-6/9
10. Elbasan et al., 2012 [11]Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-Children 09 01443 i001Children 09 01443 i001-6/9
A score with a range of 0–9 was allocated to each study, and those with a score of 6 or more were considered to be high-quality studies.
Table 2. Sensory processing impairment in children with and without developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
Table 2. Sensory processing impairment in children with and without developmental coordination disorder (DCD).
Study Design (Author, year)
Population (Age: M ± SD)
MeasuresSensory Processing Impairment: DCD Group vs. TD or Other Disability Group
TouchProprioceptiveVisualAuditoryVestibularAppendixes
1. CS (Mikami et al., 2021) [23]
63 DCD (64 ± 2 months)
106 TD (64 ± 2 months)
-
Sensory Profile
↓—DCD is poorer--↓—DCD is poorer↓—DCD is poorer↓—DCD is poorer
- oral sensory
(↑ DCD significant higher in the score of ** oral sensory)
↑ DCD significant higher in the score of ** low registration, ** sensory sensitivity, ** sensation avoiding
2. CS (Delgado-Lobete et al., 2020) [30]
46 DCD (10 ± 2 years)
369 TD (9 ± 2 years)
-
Short Sensory Profile-2
-----↑ DCD significant higher in the score of * low registration, * sensory sensitivity, * sensation avoiding, * seeking
3. CS (Nobusako et al., 2021) [31]
19 DCD (9.3 ± 1.4 years)
19 TD (9.3 ± 1.4 years)
-
Visual–tactile TOJ task
↓—DCD is poorer
(↑* DCD significant higher in the score of PSE)
-----
4. CS (Chen et al., 2020) [32]
28 DCD (10.86 ± 1.07 years)
28 TD (10.96 ± 1.18 years)
-
Doing study’s design task
-↓—DCD is poorer
(↑* DCD significant higher in the score of PE at knee; ↑** DCD significant higher in the score of PE at ankle; ↑** DCD significant higher in the score of PEV at ankle)
----
5. CS (Tseng et al., 2019) [33]
20 DCD (10.55 ± 0.72 years)
20 TD (10.65 ± 0.45 years)
-
Haptic detection/discrimination task
↓—DCD is poorer
(↑*** DCD significant higher in the score of DT)
↓—DCD is poorer
(↑*** DCD significant higher in the score of DT)
----
6. CS (Tseng et al., 2018) [34]
20 DCD (10 years 4 months ± 3 months)
30 TD (10 years 5 months ± 3 months)
-
Doing study’s design task
-↓—DCD is poorer
(↑* DCD significant higher in the score of SDPE at the wrist; ↑*** DCD significant higher in the score of JND at the wrist)
----
7. CS (Prunty et al., 2016) [35]
28 DCD (10.61 ± 2.23 years)
28 TD (10.95 ± 2.12 years)
-
The Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration (VMI)
-
The Test of Visual Perceptual Skills (TVPS)
--↓—DCD is poorer
(↓DCD significant lower in the score of *** TVPS; *** VD; * VM; * SR;
** SM; *** VF; ** VC)
---
8. CS (Cheng et al., 2014) [36]
17 DCD (7.25 ± 0.28 years)
17 TD (7.09 ± 0.34 years)
-
Test of Vis-ual Percep-tual Skills-Revised (TVPS-R)
--↓—DCD is poorer
(↓ DCD significant lower in the score of * VD; * FC; *** TVPS; * SM; ** VF)
---
9. CS (Cox et al., 2015) [37]
20 DCD (8.35 ± 1.63 years)
16 TD (8.69 ± 2.24 years)
-
Semmes Weinstein Monofilaments: Registration
-
Single Point Localization (SPL) and two-point discrimination (2PD)
↓—DCD is poorer
(↓ DCD significant lower in the score of * SPL—nondominant)
-----
10. CS (Elbasan et al., 2012) [11]
37 DCD (boys: 10 ± 1.5 years; girls: 10 ± 2 years)
35 TD (boys: 10 ± 0.8 years; girls: 9 ± 1 years)
-
Ayres Southern California Sensory Integration Test
↓—DCD is poorer
(↓ DCD significant lower in the score of * MFP; * FI; * Graphesthesia; * LTS; * DTSP; * Kinesthesia)
-↓—DCD is poorer
(↓ DCD significant lower in the score of * SV; *** Position in space; ** Design copying)
- - -
DCD = developmental coordination disorder; TD = typically developing. ↑: increase sensory processing problem, ↓: decrease sensory processing problem, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; PSE: (point of subjective equality) is a quantitative indicator of perceptual bias; PE: position error; PEV: position error variability; DT: discrimination thresholds; SDPE: standard deviation of wrist joint position error; JND: just noticeable difference threshold; VMI: visual motor integration; TVPS: test of visual perceptual skills; VD: visual discrimination; VM: visual memory; SR: spatial relationships; SM: sequential memory; VF: visual figure-ground; VC: visual closure; FC: form consistency; MFP: manual form perception; FI: finger identification; LTS: localization of tactile stimuli; DTSP: double tactile stimuli perception.
Table 3. Correlations between sensory processing impairment and motor coordination difficulties.
Table 3. Correlations between sensory processing impairment and motor coordination difficulties.
Scheme.Sensory Process Developmental Coordination Disorder
Questionnaire (DCDQ)
Movement Assessment Battery for
Children-2nd Edition (M-ABC2)
CDMFM&MGCTotal ScoreMDA&CBalTotal Score
1. Mikami et al., 2021 [23]AuditoryDCD--−0.44 ***-
TD----
TouchDCD-−0.34 **--
TD----
Multi-sensoryDCD--0.38 **-
TD--−0.19 *
Sensation avoiding scoreDCD−0.42 ***---
TD----
Sensory sensitivity scoreDCD-−0.34 **--
TD----
Sensation
seeking score
DCD0.32 **---
TD----
Low registration scoreDCD----
TD--−0.23 *-
2. Delgado-Lobete et al., 2020 [30]Low registration scoreDCD----
TD----
Both−0.28 ***-−0.17 ***−0.47 ***
Sensory sensitivity scoreDCD----
TD----
Both-−0.14 ***−0.16 ***−0.29 **
3. Nobusako et al., 2021 [31]Visual (PSE)DCDNSNSNSNSNSNSNSNS
TD--------
Both----0.43 **---
4. Chen et al., 2020 [32]Proprioceptive (PE)
at the knee
DCD NSNS–0.47 *-
TDNSNSNS-
Proprioceptive (PE)
at the ankle
DCDNSNS–0.41 *-
TDNSNSNS-
Proprioceptive (PEV) at the kneeDCDNSNS–0.44 *-
TDNSNSNS-
Proprioceptive (PEV) at the ankleDCDNSNS–0.42 *-
TDNSNSNS-
5. Tseng et al., 2019 [33]Touch (discrimination thresholds)DCD ----
TD----
Both−0.43 **−0.40 **−0.49 **-
6. Tseng et al., 2018 [34]Proprioceptive (SDPE)DCD ----
TD----
BothNS−0.29 *−0.30 *-
Proprioceptive
(JND thresholds)
DCD----
TD--–0.40 *-
Both−0.40 **NS−0.50 ***-
Proprioceptive (PE)DCD----
TD----
BothNS−0.08NS-
7. Prunty et al., 2016 [35]Found no association between sensory and motor
8. Cheng et al., 2014 [36]Visual
(discrimination)
DCD NS0.64 **NSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual
(Form constancy)
DCDNSNSNSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual
(Sequential memory)
DCD0.65 **NSNSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual
(Figure ground)
DCDNSNSNSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual
(Single in left—SL)
DCDNSNSNS–0.55 *
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual
(Single in right—SR)
DCDNSNSNSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual (double off in the left—OL)DCDNSNSNSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual (double off in the right—OR)DCDNS−0.60 *NS–0.47 *
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual (double left—DL)DCDNS−0.48 *NSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
Visual (double right—DR)DCDNS–0.49 *NSNS
TDNSNSNSNS
9. Cox et al., 2015 [37]Touch (spatial tactile perception—SPL) in both handsDCD ----
TD----
Both−0.04 ** 1.44 **1.39 **
10. Elbasan et al., 2012 [11]Found no association between sensory and motor
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; DCD, developmental coordination disorder; TD, typical development; Both: combining DCD children and TD children; (-): There is no correlation between sensory impairment and motor coordination problems; Blank square: not statistically analyzed in that area. NS, non-significant difference; CDM: control during movement; FM&H, fine motor/handwriting; GC, general coordination; MD, manual dexterity; A&C, aiming and catching; Bal: Bal.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tran, H.-T.; Li, Y.-C.; Lin, H.-Y.; Lee, S.-D.; Wang, P.-J. Sensory Processing Impairments in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Children 2022, 9, 1443. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9101443

AMA Style

Tran H-T, Li Y-C, Lin H-Y, Lee S-D, Wang P-J. Sensory Processing Impairments in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Children. 2022; 9(10):1443. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9101443

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tran, Huynh-Truc, Yao-Chuen Li, Hung-Yu Lin, Shin-Da Lee, and Pei-Jung Wang. 2022. "Sensory Processing Impairments in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder" Children 9, no. 10: 1443. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9101443

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop