Next Article in Journal
Play Behaviors in Children during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Review of the Literature
Previous Article in Journal
Newborn Incubators Do Not Protect from High Noise Levels in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit and Are Relevant Noise Sources by Themselves
Previous Article in Special Issue
Interrelations of Physical Fitness and Cognitive Functions in German Schoolchildren
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Relationship between the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren (AMBS) and Motor Competence Assessment (MCA) in Brazilian Children

by
Fábio Saraiva Flôres
1,2,*,
Luis Paulo Rodrigues
3,4 and
Rita Cordovil
2,5
1
KinesioLab, Research Unit in Human Movement Analysis, Instituto Piaget, Av. Jorge Peixinho 30 Quinta da Arreinela, 2805-059 Almada, Portugal
2
Faculdade de Motricidade Humana, Universidade de Lisboa, 1499-002 Lisboa, Portugal
3
Escola Superior de Desporto e Lazer de Melgaço, Instituto Politécnico de Viana do Castelo, 4900-347 Viana do Castelo, Portugal
4
Research Center in Sports Sciences, Health and Human Development (CIDESD), 5000-801 Vila Real, Portugal
5
Interdisciplinary Center for the Study of Human Performance (CIPER), Faculdade de Motricidade Humana, Universidade de Lisboa, 1499-002 Lisboa, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2021, 8(8), 705; https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080705
Submission received: 29 June 2021 / Revised: 11 August 2021 / Accepted: 13 August 2021 / Published: 16 August 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sports Science for Children)

Abstract

:
During growth, children are influenced by an extensive network, in which more favorable contexts provide better affordance landscapes, and consequently have a better potential to foster child development. We aimed to examine the affordances provided to children using the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren (AMBS) tool, estimating its association with children’s motor competence, as assessed by the Motor Competence Assessment (MCA) battery. Seventy-two Brazilian children were evaluated using the MCA instrument. Their parents/guardians completed the AMBS. The correlations between the two instruments (sub-scales and total scores) were investigated. ANOVAs were used to compare the motor competence performance of children with Low, Average, and High AMBS scores. Positive associations were found between AMBS and MCA, although weak to moderate in nature. In addition, children whose environments were richer in motor affordances (higher AMBS scores) showed significantly higher levels on the MCA. This study provides evidence that AMBS is a valid tool for assessing motor affordances for schoolchildren, and that those affordances are related to children’s motor competence.

1. Introduction

As children grow, they are influenced by an extensive network, such as their houses, the neighborhood, parents’ work, the house of relatives or friends, school, sports contexts, and culture [1]. During this process, more favorable environments, with better structural and material conditions, provide richer opportunities for action, or affordance landscapes [2], than others [3,4], having a better potential to foster child development [5]. Thus, the importance of the different environments (or microsystems) which with the child interacts during development is widely accepted in the literature [6,7,8].
A previous literature review [8] explored how studies have investigated the potential of different environments (i.e., home, school, and leisure environments) to promote children’s motor competence (MC) and motor development. The authors concluded that most studies have focused mainly on the home microsystem, providing an incomplete framework of the affordances across those environments, especially later in development. Gaps in the literature were identified concerning the study of affordances in schoolchildren’s environments and the lack of an instrument capable of analyzing the different contexts in which 6- to 10-year-old children are engaged. To address this gap, Flôres and colleagues [9] developed the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren (AMBS) questionnaire, which was designed to quantify motor affordances present at home and school environments. The AMBS questionnaire waw shown to represent a valuable and coherent structure of the existent affordances in three main sub-scales (Home, Materials, and School), capable of discriminating between environments according to its motor affordance enrichment. However, to be effective as an evaluation tool, the AMBS also must prove the expected relationship to children’s motor development characteristics, as their MC.
Motor competence is strongly related to the development of fundamental motor skills, comprising locomotor, stability, and manipulative skills that are cornerstones for the acquisition of specialized movements throughout the lifespan [10,11], relevant for developing children’s healthy lifestyles [10,12,13], and sport participation [14,15]. Some authors have suggested considering a broader range of movements that support physical activity engagement across the lifespan (e.g., swimming, cycling, or resistance training skills) [16,17], and the term ‘foundational movement skills’ has been proposed [18]. These foundational movement skills are influenced by socio-cultural and geographic constraints (e.g., learning to cycle might be more important to maintain an active lifestyle in countries where there is a stronger cycling culture). Despite the cultural differences, during their first years, children need to develop a motor repertoire that is sufficiently diverse to be flexibly adapted to different and specific movement contexts later in life [19]; this repertoire should always include locomotor, stability, and manipulative skills. Good levels of MC contribute to the enhanced learning of new skills and a higher motor proficiency on novel motor tasks throughout the lifespan [11]. Additionally, MC has been shown to be influenced by sex [11,20]. Studies also showed that there is a cultural influence on the levels of MC among children around the world [21,22,23], and MC has been positively associated with health-related fitness and developmental outcomes [12].
Even though MC is an extremely important topic in the study of children, a better understanding of the underlying processes that influence its development is still needed. The study of environmental influences on children’s MC has been addressed in some previous studies [24], but the availability of affordances for motor development has mostly been investigated for young children [17,25]. Given the nature of motor affordances, the construct validity of the AMBS must be validated by estimating its association with the expected output of motor affordances (i.e., motor competence). Thus, the present study aimed to examine the AMBS construct validity, estimating its association with children’s motor competence, as assessed by the Motor Competence Assessment (MCA) battery. We hypothesize that children who interact with richer motor affordance contexts (high AMBS) will present higher motor competence scores (MCA) than children living in poorer motor affordance contexts.

2. Materials and Methods

Two hundred and ten Brazilian families were invited (contacted using social media and schools) to participate in the present research. Seventy-two children (35 boys and 37 girls—mean age of 8.2 ± 1.4 years) and their parents or guardians agreed to enroll in this study. Participants were recruited from different cities in southern Brazil. Oral assent was obtained from the participants and written consent from their parents/guardians, before beginning the experiment. None of the participants had any developmental difficulties or medical restrictions to perform the activities. The research was approved by the university ethics committee.
From the 122 parents who agreed to participate, 103 returned the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren (AMBS) questionnaire [9], and 72 of their children completed the motor competence evaluation. Parents also reported their children’s age, height, and weight using the characterization category in the AMBS.
The AMBS is intended to evaluate the motor affordances provided to children by different contexts. The instrument is composed of 72 questions grouped into 11 variables (Inside Space A, Inside Space B, and Outside Space; Sedentary Material, Pretend Play Toys, Educational Toys, Manipulative Materials, and Stability Materials; Space for Movement, Free Space for Movement, and Sedentary Space), which are then organized into three main sub-scales (Home, Materials, and School). Each category raw score is transformed into a standardized score that ranges from 1 (Very Low) to 4 (Very High). The AMBS total score is made up of the sum of the three sub-scales standardized scores. In this study, children were organized into tercile groups according to the AMBS total scores, thus representing a Low (less than 8 points), Average (8 to 10 points), and High (more than 10 points) AMBS.
To assess the motor competence of the children, the Motor Competence Assessment (MCA) was administered to each child on the same day that parents completed the AMBS. Procedures are described elsewhere [11,26]. The MCA was implemented in a sports gym by three independent and fully trained investigators (Physical Education teachers), taking approximately 25 min per child.
This instrument was designed to measure motor competence and comprises six tests of three sub-scales—Stability: Jumping Sideways (JS) and Shifting Platforms (SP); Locomotor: Standing Long Jump (SLJ) and Shuttle Run (SHR), and Manipulative: Ball Kicking Velocity (BKV) and Ball Throwing Velocity (BTV). Its construct validity and normative values have been established from early childhood to young adulthood [11,27,28]. The MCA uses only quantitative and easy-to-assess tests, which diminishes observation errors, and they do not present a ceiling effect over developmental years [11,28]. The individual results (JS, SP, SLJ, SHR, BTV, and BKV) were transformed into age- and sex-related percentiles using the normative values of the MCA instrument (Rodrigues et al., 2019). To find each MCA sub-scale score (Stability, Locomotor, and Manipulative), the average of the two respective test percentile positions was used. Finally, the total MCA was calculated as the average of the three MCA sub-scales.
Descriptive analysis with mean and standard deviation was used to characterize anthropometric data and AMBS and MCA results. The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test confirmed the data normality and all statistical assumptions [29]. Pearson’s correlation was used to analyze the relationship between the MCA sub-scales and total, and the AMBS sub-scales and total. Correlation coefficients <0.30 were considered weak, those between 0.30 and 0.70 were considered moderate, and coefficients >0.70 were considered strong [29]. Univariate ANOVAs were used to find whether the AMBS classification (High, Medium, and Low AMBS) was related to MCA values (sub-scales and total). The Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS), version 25.0, was used, adopting an alpha level of significance of 5%.

3. Results

Our results showed that regarding the income condition of the families, the answers to the AMBS showed that most of the families received less than BRL 3000 (between EUR 1501 and EUR 2500) per month (58.4%), between BRL 3001 and BRL 5000 (between EUR 2501 and EUR 3500) (19.4%), and more than BRL 5001 (over EUR 5000) (22.2%). Concerning parental education, 34.7% of the parents had failed to complete school education, 38.9% had finished high school, and 26.4% had finished higher education.
Table 1 provides information about the sample size, gender, height, weight, and extracurricular activities of the participants. In addition, it presents the data regarding MCA test percentiles, categories, and total MCA. The results showed that the children’s microsystems present low levels of AMBS Total (<8 points).
To test the associations between the AMBS and the MCA, bivariate correlations were used (Table 2). There were significant weak associations between MCA Locomotor and AMBS Materials (r = 0.232, p < 0.05), AMBS School (r = 0.235, p < 0.05), and AMBS Total (r = 0.267, p < 0.05); MCA Manipulative category was weakly associated with AMBS total (r = 0.279, p < 0.05); and Total MCA to AMBS School (r = 0.241, p < 0.05). Additionally, there were significant moderate associations between MCA Stability and AMBS Home (r = 0.317, p < 0.01), AMBS Materials (r = 0.368, p < 0.001), and AMBS total (r = 0.376, p < 0.001); MCA Manipulative category was moderately associated with AMBS Home (r = 0.313, p < 0.01). The Total MCA showed moderate associations with AMBS Home (r = 0.311, p < 0.01), AMBS Materials (r = 0.302, p < 0.01) and AMBS Total (r = 0.359, p < 0.001).
To test for the hypothesized differences in MC according to the AMBS classification, one-way ANOVAs for each MCA sub-scale and total were performed using the three tercile groups of AMBS (Low, Average, and High). Table 3 presents the results of the main effects and post hoc tests, showing that children with AMBS higher scores presented better results in all MCA sub-scales. Children that showed a higher level of motor affordances in their environment were also significantly better on their motor competence, as shown in Figure 1.

4. Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the relationship between the opportunities for action available in children’s environments and their levels of motor competence. The AMBS was used to assess the quality of the different microsystems. Our initial premise was that AMBS, and MCA scores would be related, with associations between AMBS and MCA categories. Our findings confirm a significant association between AMBS and MCA categories and total scores (see Table 2). The association tested was between the opportunities for action assessed by a questionnaire and the level of MC as assessed by the MCA. The rationale was that when in the presence of more motor affordances in their daily life, children will take advantage of these opportunities, increasing their physical activity and movement experience, and with that there is the likelihood of developing a better motor competence.
According to Gibson [2], each environment has objects, places, surfaces, events, and other people that provide different action opportunities, depending on the child’s action capabilities. This concept also shows that children experience a context according to its functionality by detecting meaningful the environmental properties of relevance to the perceiver [2,30]. Additionally, Bronfenbrenner and Ceci [7] noted that the physical, social, or symbolic environmental characteristics invite, permit, or inhibit reciprocal tuning toward a progressively more complex interactional activity in and with the immediate setting. These interactional proximal processes of development are dependent on the mutual interaction between the subject and the environment. Thus, these theoretical models, promote the understanding of MC as a result of proximal processes between a child and their immediate contexts. Powerful as it can be expected, motor affordances in the environment are not supposed to be the only influence on the motor development of children at this age. Several other features of children (personal, daily life, heredity, family, culture, society, biological development, socioeconomic condition, motivation, etc.), certainly influence children’s motor competence. Consequently, the associations found between the AMBS and the MCA, although moderate in nature, signal the important conclusion that the AMBS was able to assess and quantify important characteristics that are related to the actual development of motor competence. Thus, and even though environments are not the only influencing factor (i.e., genetic, or biological conditions are important conditions), children’s motor competence is related to the motor affordances provided in their daily life, and the AMBS is capable of capturing them.
Of the most important to this research is the insight obtained from the AMBS total scores because they consider not only the number, but also the variety of affordances. Despite the Low classification of the average number of affordances provided to children (scoring 6.28 ± 2.82)—meaning that home, materials, and schools are probably not providing all the necessary affordances to average children, school-aged children with higher affordances had significantly better levels of MC than the other two groups (see Table 3 and Figure 1). Similar results were found in several studies, showing that higher and better affordances provide higher levels of motor development in infants [31,32] and young children [33,34,35]. Thus, our study shows that affordances are extremely important to develop motor competence across the lifespan.
The AMBS proved to be an important tool to evaluate and discriminate among different motor affordance profiles. The results showed a common structured organization of potential affordances in the children’s microsystems concerning three sub-scales (Home, Materials, and School), representing a meaningful structure inside and outside the home, resulting from the parents’ decisions or possibilities on how they provide environmental stimuli to their children. Thus, our results revealed that AMBS is a valid indicator of the affordances found in multiple contexts that have the potential to influence schoolchildren’s motor competence.
Regarding the provision of affordances, other factors measured by the AMBS can contribute to the results found in the present research. For example, 58.4% of all families received less than BRL 3000 (between EUR 1501 and EUR 2500) per month, and 38.9% of the fathers and 44.4% of the mothers had only finished high school. Several studies show that financial conditions [36,37] and parental education are important aspects of child development, especially at a young age [38,39,40]. Studies have also shown that children in families with poor financial conditions, low levels of education, and huge provision restrictions present low levels of motor development [41]. In fact, school-aged children who are at risk have been shown to demonstrate developmental delays in their fundamental motor skills [42]. Thus, investigators must pay attention to these characteristics when analyzing the development of children’s MC. Some limitations of this study should be noted. First, although the AMBS is a valid and reliable assessing tool, an in loco assessment of the environments could provide better information. Secondly, the AMBS is a parental self-report instrument and provides data regarding the quantity and variety of the materials inside and outside the home; it is not purposed to assess the number of interactions that children have with the materials, nor the importance assigned to these interactions. Thirdly, affordances for fine motor skills are assessed by the AMBS, but the MCA does not assess those skills. Thus, in future studies, it might be interesting to analyze the association of AMBS with an assessment battery that also includes fine motor skills. Finally, some results in the present study might be explained by specific characteristics of our sample of southern Brazilian children, namely, the differences found between boys and girls relative to their movement activities. Our findings suggest that girls attend more team, individual and combat sports, outdoor activities, and cultural activities than boys in the same age range. These results are surprising, but the literature has shown some controversy in results when comparing boys’ and girls’ MC [21,43]. We believe that studies using AMBS to assess motor affordances provide important information that can be complemented by studies that investigate children’s actual interactions in different settings.
Future investigations could explore the relationship of AMBS with other instruments that assess different aspects of MC (e.g., fine motor skills, foundational movement skills), or with objectively measured physical activity (using accelerometers, for example). Additionally, understanding the importance of child microsystems could be important in order to devise strategies to tackle low levels of MC in school-aged children, preventing children from entering a negative spiral of MC.

5. Conclusions

Our findings provide further evidence that the AMBS is a valid tool for assessing motor affordances for school-aged children, being able to assess and discriminate among different motor affordance profiles. Furthermore, we can state that there is a relationship between affordances in the microsystems and children’s MC. Thus, better contextual conditions are important in the development of schoolchildren’s motor competence. Exploring the relationship of the quality and quantity of the microsystem’s affordances and its influence on the children’s motor competence development can be fundamental for understanding the complex nature of these factors, and the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren questionnaire proved to be a useful tool for such task.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, F.S.F. and R.C.; Methodology, F.S.F., L.P.R. and R.C.; Formal analysis, F.S.F., L.P.R. and R.C.; Investigation, F.S.F. and R.C.; Writing—original draft preparation, F.S.F., L.P.R. and R.C.; Writing—review and editing, F.S.F., L.P.R. and R.C.; Funding acquisition, R.C. and L.P.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

L.P.R. was partly supported by the Portuguese Science Foundation (FCT), under Grant UID04045/2020. R.C. was partly supported by the Portuguese Science Foundation (FCT), under Grant UIDB/00447/2020 (unit 447).

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Universidade Federal de Santa Maria (UFSM, Brazil) with Protocol: 76336117.0.0000.5346.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all parents or tutors of the children, and children gave their verbal assent.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available because not all parents have given proper consent for the public display of data.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the families’ participation in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Bronfenbrenner, U. The bioecological model from a life course perspective: Reflections of a participant observer. In Examining Lives in Context: Perspectives on the Ecology of Human Development; Moen, P., Elder, G.H., Jr., Lüscher, K., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1995; pp. 599–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Gibson, J.J. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  3. Gabbard, C.; Rodrigues, L.P. Affordances for motor development. In Tópicos em Desenvolv. Mot. na Infância e Adolescência; LECSU: Rio Janeiro, Brasil, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  4. Santos, E.; Flôres, F.; Katzer, J.; Valentini, N.; Corazza, S.; Copetti, F.; Soares, E.; Flôres, F.; Katzer, J.; Valentini, N.; et al. Análise das oportunidades de estimulação motora em ambientes domiciliares na região central do Rio Grande do Sul. Rev. Bras. Educ. Física Esporte 2015, 29, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Koller, S. Ecologia do Desenvolvimento Humano: Pesquisa e Intervenção no Brasil, 2nd ed.; Casa do Psicólogo: Belo Horizonte, Brasil, 2004; ISBN 8573963883. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bronfenbrenner, U. Making Human Beings Human: Bioecological Perspectives on Human Development; Sage: London, UK, 2005; ISBN 0761927123. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bronfenbrenner, U.; Ceci, S.J. Heredity, environment, and the question “How?”: A first approximation. In Nature, Nurture & Psychology; Plomin, R., McClearn, G.E., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1993; pp. 313–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Flôres, F.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Copetti, F.; Lopes, F.; Cordovil, R. Affordances for motor skill development in home, school, and sport environments: A narrative review. Percept. Mot. Skills 2019, 126, 003151251982927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Flôres, F.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Cordovil, R. Development and construct validation of a questionnaire for measuring affordances for motor behavior of schoolchildren. J. Mot. Learn. Dev. 2021, 1–19, in press. [Google Scholar]
  10. Luz, C.; Almeida, G.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Cordovil, R. The evaluation of motor competence in typically developing children: An integrative review. J. Phys. Educ. 2017, 28, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Rodrigues, L.P.; Luz, C.; Cordovil, R.; Bezerra, P.; Silva, B.; Camões, M.; Lima, R. Normative values of the motor competence assessment (MCA) from 3 to 23 years of age. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2019, 22, 1038–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Robinson, L.; Stodden, D.; Barnett, L.; Lopes, V.; Logan, S.; Rodrigues, L.P.; D’Hondt, E. Motor competence and its effect on positive developmental trajectories of health. Sport. Med. 2015, 45, 1273–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Stodden, D.; Goodway, J.; Langendorfer, S.; Roberton, M.; Rudisill, M.; Garcia, C.; Garcia, L. A developmental perspective on the role of motor skill competence in physical activity: An emergent relationship. Quest 2008, 60, 290–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ferreira, L.; Vieira, J.; Silva, P.; Chaves, R.; Fernandes, R.; Cheuczuk, F.; Rocha, F.; Caçola, P. The role of sport participation and body mass index in predicting motor competence of school-age children. J. Phys. Educ. 2019, 30, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Flôres, F.; Milani, M.; Copetti, F.; Luz, C.; Cordovil, R. O impacto da prática do futsal na competência motora de crianças. Motrivivência 2020, 32, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dudley, D. A conceptual model of observed physical literacy. Phys. Educ. 2015, 72, 236–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Scheuer, C.; Herrmann, C.; Bund, A. Motor tests for primary school aged children: A systematic review. J. Sports Sci. 2019, 37, 1097–1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hulteen, R.M.; Morgan, P.J.; Barnett, L.M.; Stodden, D.F.; Lubans, D.R. Development of foundational movement skills: A conceptual model for physical activity across the lifespan. Sport. Med. 2018, 48, 1533–1540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Clark, J.E.; Metcalfe, J.S. The mountain of motor development: A metaphor. Mot. Dev. Res. Rev. 2002, 2, 163–190. [Google Scholar]
  20. Kokštejn, J.; Musálek, M.; Tufano, J.J. Are sex differences in fundamental motor skills uniform throughout the entire preschool period? PLoS ONE 2017, 12, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Flôres, F.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Luz, C.; Cordovil, R. Cross-cultural comparisons of motor competence in southern Brazilian and Portuguese schoolchildren. Motriz 2021, 27, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  22. Luz, C.; Cordovil, R.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Gao, Z.; Goodway, J.; Sacko, R.S.; Nesbitt, D.; Ferkel, R.; True, L.; Stodden, D. Motor competence and health-related fitness in children: A cross-cultural comparison between Portugal and the United States. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019, 8, 130–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bardid, F.; Rudd, J.; Lenoir, M.; Polman, R.; Barnett, L. Cross-cultural comparison of motor competence in children from Australia and Belgium. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Nobre, F.; Valentini, N.; Rusidill, M. Applying the bioecological theory to the study of fundamental motor skills. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 2020, 25, 29–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Rodrigues, L.P. Development and Validation of the Ahemd-Sr (Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development–Self Report); Texas A&M University: College Station, TX, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  26. Sá, C.; Luz, C.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Cordovil, R. Motor competence assessment-cultural adaptation for Brazil (MCA-BR). Fisioter. Pesqui. 2021, 28, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rodrigues, L.P.; Cordovil, R.; Luz, C.; Lopes, V. Model invariance of the motor competence assessment (MCA) from early childhood to young adulthood. J. Sports Sci. 2021, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Luz, C.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Almeida, G.; Cordovil, R. Development and validation of a model of motor competence in children and adolescents. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2016, 19, 568–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Field, A. Discovering Statistics with SPSS, 2nd ed.; Sage Publications of London: London, UK, 2005; ISBN 9788536320182. [Google Scholar]
  30. Heft, H. Foundations of an ecological approach to psychology. In The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology; Oxford Unisersity Press: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  31. Abbott, A.L.; Bartlett, D.J.; Fanning, J.E.; Kramer, J. Infant motor development and aspects of the home environment. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 2000, 12, 62–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Saccani, R.; Valentini, N.; Pereira, K.; Müller, A.; Gabbard, C. Associations of biological factors and affordances in the home with infant motor development. Pediatr. Int. 2013, 55, 197–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Haydari, A.; Askari, P.; Nezhad, M.Z. Relationship between affordances in the home environment and motor development in children age 18–42 months. J. Soc. Sci. 2009, 5, 319–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Mori, S.; Nakamoto, H.; Mizuochi, H.; Ikudome, S.; Gabbard, C. Influence of affordances in the home environment on motor development of young children in Japan. Child. Dev. Res. 2013, 2013, 898406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Valadi, S.; Gabbard, C. The effect of affordances in the home environment on children’s fine- and gross motor skills. Early Child. Dev. Care 2018, 190, 1225–1232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hernández, M.M.; Robins, R.W.; Widaman, K.F.; Conger, R.D. School belonging, generational status, and socioeconomic effects on mexican-origin children’s later academic competence and expectations. J. Res. Adolesc. 2016, 26, 241–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Trawick-Smith, J.; Wolff, J.; Koschel, M.; Vallarelli, J. Effects of toys on the play quality of preschool children: Influence of gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Early Child. Educ. J. 2015, 43, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Bradley, R.; Corwyn, R. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2002, 53, 371–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Lung, F.-W.; Shu, B.-C.; Chiang, T.-L.; Lin, S.-J. Child: The development of children at 6, 18 and 36 months: A Taiwan birth cohort pilot study. Child. Care. Health Dev. 2010, 37, 211–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Miquelote, A.; Santos, D.; Caçola, P.; Montebelo, M.; Gabbard, C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 2012, 35, 329–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Halpern, R.; Barros, F.; Horta, B.; Victora, C. Desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor aos 12 meses de idade em uma coorte de base populacional no Sul do Brasil: Diferenciais conforme peso ao nascer e. Cad. Saúde Pública 1996, 12, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Altunsöz, I.; Goodway, J. SKIPing to motor competence: The influence of project successful kinesthetic instruction for preschoolers on motor competence of disadvantaged preschoolers. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 2016, 21, 366–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Bjørndal, C.T.; Ronglan, L.T.; Andersen, S.S. Talent development as an ecology of games: A case study of Norwegian handball. Sport. Educ. Soc. 2017, 22, 864–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. AMBS classification groups regarding MCA values: (a) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in the stability component. (b) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in the locomotor component. (c) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in the manipulative component. (d) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in Total Motor Competence.
Figure 1. AMBS classification groups regarding MCA values: (a) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in the stability component. (b) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in the locomotor component. (c) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in the manipulative component. (d) Mean scores of the AMBS terciles in Total Motor Competence.
Children 08 00705 g001aChildren 08 00705 g001b
Table 1. Descriptive values of the sample.
Table 1. Descriptive values of the sample.
Boys (n = 35)Girls (n = 37)Total (n = 72)
Raw Scores
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Child Characterization
Age7.831.408.531.308.191.39
Height118.8321.78119.7023.16119.2822.34
Weight31.888.3430.669.0631.258.68
Child Movement activities (days per week)
Team Sports0.370.771.401.500.901.30
Individual Sports0.420.811.652.081.051.70
Combat Sports0.00.00.381.110.190.82
Outdoor Activities0.110.530.140.420.130.47
Music Activities0.600.950.510.960.560.94
Cultural/Artistic Activities2.542.103.241.822.901.98
AMBS. Sub-scales and total (raw scores)
Home7.113.157.322.337.222.75
Materials47.9425.6047.6221.8347.7824.09
School9.293.739.354.189.323.94
AMBS total6.232.976.322.746.282.83
MCA tests percentiles
Jumping Sideways362725273028
Shifting Platforms433032193726
Standing Long Jump562858305729
Shuttle Run463340304331
Ball Throwing Velocity623742335236
Ball Kicking Velocity494044344637
MCA. Sub-scales and total (mean of test’s percentiles)
Stability392328193322
Locomotor502449274926
Manipulative553543284932
Total MCA482440204422
MCA—Motor Competence Assessment; AMBS—Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren.
Table 2. Correlation between the MCA (sub-scales and total) and AMBS (sub-scales and total).
Table 2. Correlation between the MCA (sub-scales and total) and AMBS (sub-scales and total).
AMBS Sub-Scales
MCA Sub-ScalesHomeMaterialsSchoolAMBS Total
Stability0.32 **0.37 ***0.180.38 ***
Locomotor0.150.23 *0.24 *0.27 *
Manipulative0.31 **0.200.190.28 *
Total MCA0.31 **0.30 **0.24 *0.36 ***
MCA—Motor Competence Assessment; AMBS—Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Descriptive results for each MCA sub-scale and total, according to the AMBS group classification, and ANOVAs and post hoc tests.
Table 3. Descriptive results for each MCA sub-scale and total, according to the AMBS group classification, and ANOVAs and post hoc tests.
AMBS Classification Groups
(MCA Mean Percentiles)
MCAT1 (Low)
(M ± SD)
T2 (Average)
(M ± SD)
T3 (High)
(M ± SD)
ANOVAs
Post Hoc
Stability26 ± 1728 ± 2047 ± 23F (3,69) = 72.943; p < 0.001
T1 = T2 < T3
Locomotor42 ± 2548 ± 2360 ± 26F (3,69) = 98.014; p < 0.001
T1 = T2, T2 = T3, T1 < T3
Manipulative42 ± 2833 ± 2967 ± 30F (3,69) = 73.029; p < 0.001
T1 = T2 < T3
Total MCA 37 ± 1936 ± 1958 ± 23F (2,69) = 9.252; p < 0.001
T1 = T2 < T3
MCA—Motor Competence Assessment; AMBS—Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Flôres, F.S.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Cordovil, R. Relationship between the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren (AMBS) and Motor Competence Assessment (MCA) in Brazilian Children. Children 2021, 8, 705. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080705

AMA Style

Flôres FS, Rodrigues LP, Cordovil R. Relationship between the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren (AMBS) and Motor Competence Assessment (MCA) in Brazilian Children. Children. 2021; 8(8):705. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080705

Chicago/Turabian Style

Flôres, Fábio Saraiva, Luis Paulo Rodrigues, and Rita Cordovil. 2021. "Relationship between the Affordances for Motor Behavior of Schoolchildren (AMBS) and Motor Competence Assessment (MCA) in Brazilian Children" Children 8, no. 8: 705. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080705

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop