3.1. Correlation Analyses for Questionnaire Subscales
Spearman correlation analyses indicated significant results for all associations. Concerning parents’ ratings, the Conners—IN subscale negatively correlates with the EF Total score (r(639) = −0.64, p < 0.001) and the Conners—H/I subscale also shows a negative correlation (r(639) = −0.45, p < 0.001). Similar results were found also for teachers’ ratings: the Conners—IN subscale negatively correlates with the EF Total score (r(822) = −0.65, p < 0.001), as well as the Conners—H/I (r(822) = −0.46, p < 0.001). Inattention symptoms appear to be more strongly associated with EF impairments than hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms, for both parent and teacher ratings. No meaningful differences emerged between home and school contexts for the total EF scores.
A more detailed analysis of the EFQU-P subscales revealed significant negative correlation between the Conners—IN subscale and four EF domains: Cognitive Self-Regulation (r(639) = −0.65, p < 0.001), Behavioral Self-Control (r(639) = −0.47, p < 0.001), Material Management (r(639) = −0.47, p < 0.001), and Initiative (r(639) = −0.45, p < 0.001). In contrast, for the Conners—H/I subscale, only one significant association emerged, with Behavioral Self-Control (r(639) = −0.47, p < 0.001). For teachers’ ratings, the Conners—IN subscale showed significant negative correlations with all EF domains: Self-Regulation (r(822) = −0.53, p < 0.001), Self-Organization (r(822) = −0.67, p < 0.001), and Material Management (r(822) = −0.55, p < 0.001). The Conners—H/I subscale was significantly associated only with Self-Regulation (r(822) = −0.53, p < 0.001) and Material Management (r(822) = −0.42, p < 0.001). These results show that the Conners—IN subscale has the strongest and most consistent associations with EF difficulties, across multiple executive domains and informants. Although no substantial differences emerged between home and school contexts, teachers’ ratings appeared slightly more strongly associated with EF impairments.
3.2. Factor Analyses
To further investigate the overlap between the EFQU and Conners 3 items, two EFAs were conducted: one for parents’ ratings and one for teachers’ ratings. The analysis used Principal Component Analysis as the extraction method and Oblimin rotation (with Kaiser normalization) to allow for correlation among components. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measures confirmed sampling adequacy, and Bartlett’s tests of sphericity indicated that both correlation matrices were factorable (p < 0.001), supporting the appropriateness of conducting exploratory factor analyses for the parent and teacher datasets.
The first explanatory analysis concerned parents’ ratings, and examined the 32 items of the EFQU-P and the 21 items of the Conners—IN and—H/I rated by parents. It identified eight factors explaining 60.24% of the total variance: (I) Cognitive Self-Regulation, incorporating 11 items from the EFQU-P (1, 4, 7, 8, 11, 14, 17, 18, 20, 22, 25), explaining 32.09% of the total variance; (II) Hyperactivity, comprising eight items from the Conners—P (45, 54, 61, 69, 71, 93, 98, 99), explaining 9.04% of the total variance; (III) Adaptability, incorporating eight items from the EFQU-P (3, 6, 13, 19, 27, 28, 29, 30), explaining 5.64% of the total variance; (IV) Material Management, incorporating five items from the EFQU-P (2, 5, 12, 26, 31) and one from the Conners—P (97), explaining 3.77% of the total variance; (V) Inattention, comprising ten items from the Conners—P (2, 28, 35, 47, 68, 79, 84, 95, 101, 43), explaining 3.11% of the total variance; (VI) Interpersonal Self-Control, incorporating four items from the EFQU-P (15, 16, 21, 24), explaining 2.44% of the total variance; (VII) Impulsivity, incorporating one item from the EFQU-P (9) and three from the Conners—P (3, 61, 104), explaining 2.19% of the total variance; and (VIII) Emotional Control, comprising two items from the EFQU-P (10, 32), accounting for 1.96% of the total variance.
Following the EFA, eight latent factors were identified and retained for the confirmatory analysis. Before proceeding with the CFA, the factor structure was examined conceptually to ensure theoretical coherence across constructs. To improve conceptual clarity, a refinement of the factor structure was applied. Specifically, two items, originally loading on the Adaptability factor (EFQU-P 19 and EFQU-P 28), were reassigned to the Emotional Control factor, with the aim of both clarifying the former and strengthening the latter. Notably, both reassigned items (EFQU-P 19 and EFQU-P 28) loaded strongly on the Emotional Control factor in the CFA (standardized loadings = 0.79 and 0.80, respectively), providing empirical support for the theoretical rationale behind the reassignment. Additionally, the EFQU-P 9 item was reversed to align with the directionality of the other items in the impulsivity factor. The CFA (
Figure 1) was then conducted on this revised eight-factor model. The model showed a good fit to the data: χ
2(1245) = 3927,
p < 0.001, CFI = 0.982, TLI = 0.981, RMSEA = 0.061, SRMR = 0.062. All standardized loadings were statistically significant and above 0.30, except for one item (Conners–P 61), supporting the adequacy of the proposed structure. The low standardized loading of the Conners–P 61 item may be due to its ambiguous content, which, in the EFA, showed moderate cross-loadings on both the Hyperactivity and Impulsivity factors. Although its CFA loading was low, the item was retained within the Hyperactivity factor, given its stronger EFA saturation and theoretical alignment with the original subscale structure of the Conners Rating Scales. Additionally, to assess internal consistency, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were computed for each scale (
Table 3). All factors demonstrated acceptable to excellent reliability, with α values ranging from 0.71 (Emotional Control) to 0.94 (Cognitive Self-Regulation). For clarity, only standardized item loadings are displayed in the path diagrams (
Figure 1 and
Figure 2). Correlated residuals and inter-factor correlations are not shown.
The second EFA concerned teachers’ ratings, and examined the 32 items of the EFQU-T and the 21 items of the Conners—IN and —H/I rated by teachers. It identified six factors, explaining 74.33% of the total variance: (I) Cognitive Self-Regulation, incorporating fourteen items from the EFQU-T (1, 2, 3, 5, 9, 10, 11, 12, 19, 22, 23, 25, 27, 31), explaining 49.89% of the total variance; (II) Impulsivity/Hyperactivity, comprising eleven items from the Conners—T (1, 4, 7, 9, 17, 24, 29, 32, 50, 76, 78), explaining 10.86% of the total variance; (III) Inattention, incorporating ten items from the Conners—T (23, 37, 57, 60, 69, 73, 88, 92, 103, 111), explaining 6.63% of the total variance; (IV) Material Management, incorporating four items from the EFQU-T (4, 8, 14, 30), explaining 2.76% of the total variance; (V) Self-Control, comprising twelve items from the EFQU-T (7, 13, 15, 16, 17, 18, 20, 21, 24, 28, 29, 32), explaining 2.22% of the total variance; (VI) and Adaptability, incorporating one item from the EFQU-T (6), explaining 1.97% of the total variance.
Before proceeding with the CFA, the factor structure was examined conceptually to ensure theoretical coherence across constructs. To improve conceptual clarity, a refinement of the factor structure was applied: the EFQU-T 6 item, originally solely loading on the Adaptability factor, was reassigned to the Cognitive Self-Regulation factor, on which the EFQU-T 6 item had the second-highest load. Additionally, this modification was theoretically motivated, as EFQU-T 6 refers to sustained effort and goal-directed persistence—core components of cognitive self-regulation. The empirical adequacy of this choice was supported by the CFA, where the item showed a strong standardized loading on the new factor (λ = 0.89). The CFA (
Figure 2) was then conducted on this revised five-factor model. The model showed a good fit to the data: χ
2(1264) = 7343,
p < 0.001, CFI = 0.994, TLI = 0.994, RMSEA = 0.080, SRMR = 0.066. All standardized loadings were statistically significant and above 0.30, supporting the adequacy of the proposed structure. Additionally, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients (
Table 4) demonstrated excellent reliability, with α values ranging from 0.94 (Impulsivity/Hyperactivity) to 0.97 (Cognitive Self-Regulation and Self-Control).