This study was conducted with the approval of the Research Ethics Committee of the Universidade Estadual Paulista (UNESP) “Júlio de Mesquita Filho”, Faculty of Philosophy and Sciences, Marília, São Paulo, under protocol number 67680623.9.0000.5406.
To provide a clearer understanding, the methodological design used in this study is presented in two phases: phase 1 focuses on the development of the Remediation Program with WM and Reading (RP-WMR), called “Programa de Remediação com a Memória Operacional e Leitura” (PR-MOL) in Brazilian Portuguese, in students with learning difficulties based on a review of the literature, and phase 2 verifies the applicability of the program developed in phase 1 in a pilot study.
2.1. Phase 1: Development of the PR-MOL
To develop the PR-MOL, a literature review was performed focusing on the following information: (1) the tasks contained in the training programs for WM; (2) descriptions of the tasks and their objectives; (3) the duration of the tasks; and (4) the number of sessions.
The literature review was performed by searching national and international databases, including Scielo, PubMed, Science Direct, and Procast, which are available online.
The literature review period lasted 9 months, between March and December 2023, focusing on studies published in the last 15 years. The selection criteria prioritised peer-reviewed articles, especially those that presented empirical data and systematic reviews. For the search in each database, search strategies included specific descriptors in English (working memory, training, intervention, children, students, reading skills, reading, reading disorders, reading difficulties) and in Portuguese (memória operacional, programa de treinamento, programa de intervenção, escolares, habilidades de leitura, transtornos de aprendizagem, dificuldades de leitura, dificuldade de aprendizagem). The Boolean operators used to effectively connect the descriptors were: (“working memory”) AND (“training” OR “intervention”) AND (“children OR “students” OR “schoolchildren”) AND (“reading skills” OR “reading” OR “reading disorders” OR “reading difficulties”).
With these search strategies, 69 articles were identified in the national and international databases. After reading the titles and abstracts, 38 articles were excluded. The remaining 31 articles were read in full, and the studies were assessed with the inclusion and exclusion criteria, identifying a final number of 18 articles that were effectively analysed in this study. Both authors were involved in the screening of titles and abstracts. A dual screening process was conducted, where each author independently reviewed all identified studies to ensure consistency and reduce bias. In cases of disagreement or uncertainty regarding the application of inclusion/exclusion criteria, discussions were held to reach a consensus.
Regarding the inclusion/exclusion criteria, articles related to the research topic and matching the search descriptors were selected. Studies were included if they focused on children or students, specifically those aged 4–11 years, and involved interventions or training programs aimed at improving working memory, particularly in the context of reading skills, reading disorders, or reading difficulties. Articles that did not meet these criteria were excluded, including those that focused on populations outside the scope of the review, such as the elderly or individuals with intellectual disabilities. Additionally, studies that did not directly address working memory, training interventions, or reading-related skills were excluded. Articles that were not fully available for review were also excluded from the study.
The tasks used for phonological and visuospatial WM training in the included studies are outlined in
Table 1.
In the included studies, the tasks used for phonological WM training were repetition of words, pseudowords, digits, and colours in the same order and/or in reverse order; recall of sequences of phonological representations of graphemes and comparisons between them; naming of figures and digits; execution of simultaneous tasks to perform in a requested order; memorisation of short stories; n-back tasks, in which the recall of one of the elements of the presented sequence is requested according to its position; and interpretation of sentences and memorisation of the last word of each of the sentences. For visuospatial memory, the tasks included immediate repetition of a sequence of visuospatial information in the same order, tracing of locations presented sequentially by a target stimulus, replication of the visuospatial path in a maze, identification and retrieval of hidden objects, retelling of films and stories, memory games, and mental rotation of letters, among others.
Of all the articles analysed, 14 reported a significant improvement in performance on WM tasks and, consequently, an improvement in reading development for both students with learning difficulties and those with learning disorders [
19,
24,
35,
36,
37,
38,
39,
40,
41,
42,
43,
44,
45,
46]. Improvements were observed in decoding tasks, fluency, and especially reading comprehension. These results are similar to those of previous studies [
2,
6] that emphasised the importance of WM in the academic success of schoolchildren.
Selection of Stimuli for the PR-MOL for Students with Learning Difficulties
On the basis of the literature review described above, the remediation program was designed with tasks using linguistic stimuli (real words and pseudowords) and visual stimuli (pictures). The real words were selected from the word bank of E-reading II [
47], which is divided into high-, medium-, and low-frequency words on the basis of the frequency of occurrence of the words in elementary school textbooks. A balanced selection of low-, medium-, and high-frequency words was included. The criteria used to balance the selection of words were based on their syllabic structure to match the different syllabic patterns in the word sequence tasks. Subsequently, this approach ensured that the amount of information that would be processed and manipulated in working memory was balanced by adjusting the syllabic complexity of each word depending on the number of words inserted in the word sequences, without overloading memory. In particular, nouns were selected because of the possibility of being represented by pictures.
The program consisted of a total of 173 words and 262 pseudowords. The real words included 16 monosyllabic, 68 disyllabic, 56 trisyllabic, and 10 polysyllabic words. Owing to the low variability in polysyllabic words in the word bank, 23 polysyllabic words with four and five syllables, taken from the Michaelis online dictionary, were added, for a total of 33 polysyllabic words.
The real words and their respective images were printed on rectangular cards, with the word written in capital letters, lowercase letters, and cursive on one side and the representative picture on the other side.
The PR-MOL consisted of 11 tasks divided into two modules for administration in 10 sessions of 1 h per session. Module 1 included tasks involving phonological WM, and Module 2 included tasks involving visuospatial working memory.
The eleven tasks in the program were cumulative, i.e., they were designed to be performed in the ten program sessions, but in each session, different stimuli were used. In odd-numbered sessions (1, 3, 5, 7, 9), tasks 1 to 6, focusing on phonological WM, were performed, whereas in even-numbered sessions (2, 4, 6, 8, 10), tasks 7 to 11, focusing on visuospatial WM, were performed.
In all the tasks, the student was presented with sequences of stimuli (words or pseudowords) that gradually increased from two to six stimuli according to the storage capacity of WM [
48].
Each of the sequences followed a pattern of syllabic extension of the stimuli, as shown below:
Two stimuli: a monosyllabic word and a disyllabic word
Three stimuli: a monosyllabic word, a disyllabic word, and a trisyllabic word
Four stimuli: a monosyllabic word, a disyllabic word, a trisyllabic word, and a polysyllabic word
Five stimuli: a disyllabic word, a trisyllabic word, and three polysyllabic words
Six stimuli: two trisyllabic words and four polysyllabic words.
To promote the students’ interest in performing the tasks, a playful context was designed with a reward strategy named “Assembling the Memory Treasure”, with the students drawing coins for each task. In addition to the boards used to perform the tasks, a board with a city map, cards for drawing coins and places in the city, personalised coins in a chest, and a board to fill with the coins were prepared.
The PR-MOL was composed of two modules, as shown in
Table 2.
2.2. Phase 2: Applicability of the PR-MOL in a Pilot Study
2.2.1. Participants
Participants in this study were selected from a municipal public school in a city in the interior of the state of São Paulo with a lower-middle socioeconomic level that follows the global literacy program. Initially, the students who had learning difficulties were indicated by the teachers, and the school records were consulted to verify whether the students met the established inclusion and exclusion criteria, described below.
Inclusion criteria:
Exclusion criteria:
Previous participation in speech therapy, pedagogical, or psychopedagogical intervention programs.
Presence of genetic syndromes, intellectual disability, or ADHD, as described in the school records.
2.2.2. Sample Characterisation
This study was a blind, quasi-experimental, cross-sectional study conducted with a convenience sample of students with learning difficulties.
A total of 24 schoolchildren of both sexes, aged between 8 years and 11 years and 11 months, from the third, fourth, and fifth years of elementary school I were selected to participate in phase 2 of this study according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Three of the students were excluded because they changed schools. The students were divided into two groups:
- -
Group I (GI): Eleven elementary school students with learning difficulties, consisting of nine males and two females aged between 8 years and 11 years and 11 months, with four students from the third year, three students from the fourth year, and four students from the fifth year, underwent the PR-MOL and post-testing.
- -
Group II (GII): Ten elementary school students with learning difficulties, consisting of four males and six females from the third to fifth grades, were paired according to age group and education level with the students from GI and did not undergo the PR-MOL.
2.2.3. Instruments
Pre- and post-testing were performed with the PROHMELE assessment [
49] and the Brief Child Neuropsychological Assessment Instrument NEUPSILIN-Inf [
50], as described below.
The PROHMELE assessment is used to evaluate metalinguistic and reading skills. This assessment is composed of fifteen syllabic and phonemic identification and manipulation tests, a pseudoword repetition test, a real-word reading test composed of 133 words, and a pseudoword reading test consisting of 27 pseudowords, which cover context-independent graphophonemic correspondence rules (Rule D1), context-dependent graphophonemic correspondence rules (Rule D2), and the rule of values of the letter “X” exclusively dependent on the mental and orthographic lexicon (Rule D4). The results were corrected and analysed following the instructions and regulations of the protocol manual.
The NEUPSILIN-Inf is a brief neuropsychological assessment tool that assesses orientation, attention, visual perception, memory, arithmetic skills, language, visuoconstructive skills, and executive functions. To evaluate phonological WM, the Repetition of Digit Sequence in Indirect Order (reverse order) and Pseudoword Span tests were used, each composed of eight sequences. To evaluate visuospatial WM, the Corsi block test was used. Both instruments used were validated for content validity (test specifications and construction process with analysis of agreement between judges), construct validity (developmental changes, investigated by means and standard deviations, observed in each age group, validity of internal structure through confirmatory factor analysis), and criterion validity to differentiate children with and without difficulties in the skills assessed. The results were corrected and analysed according to the instructions in the instrument manual.
2.2.4. Procedures
All the subjects who participated in this study completed the pre- and post-testing evaluations in a large, quiet room without distractions. The study was conducted during the three-month period from March 2023 to June 2023 in a public school of the municipal elementary school network located in the interior of the state of São Paulo.
The PR-MOL was administered over sixteen individual sessions, divided into three sessions for pretesting, ten sessions for application of the remediation program for students in the experimental group, and three sessions for post-testing.
To avoid expectation bias, the tendency to interpret the results of the intervention as better or worse according to one’s view of the intervention, the post-testing evaluation was performed by a member of the LIDA Laboratory who was not involved with the pretesting data or the application of the developed program.
The analysis of the results was conducted using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS), version 25.0. The Shapiro–Wilk test was conducted to check for normality of the data distribution. Since the results indicated that the data did not follow a normal distribution, the non-parametric Wilcoxon signed-tank test was used to perform intra-group comparisons between two assessment moments, namely, pre- and post-testing. A significance level of 5% (0.050) was adopted. In cases of statistically significant differences, the data were marked with an asterisk in the tables.