The Relationship between Giftedness and Sex and Children’s Theory of Mind Skills and Social Behavior
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire
2.2.2. ToM Tests
2.2.3. Anadolu Sak Intelligence Scale
2.3. Procedure and Data Collection
2.4. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Renzulli, J.S. The three-ring conception of giftedness: A developmental model for creative productivity. In Conceptions of Giftedness; Davidson, R.J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005; pp. 246–279. [Google Scholar]
- Rinn, A.N.; Majority, K.L. The Social and Emotional World of the Gifted. In Handbook of Giftedness in Children: Psychoeducational Theory, Research, and Best Practices, 2nd ed.; Pfeiffer, S.I., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2018; pp. 49–63. [Google Scholar]
- Cross, T.L.; Coleman, L.J.; Terhaar-Yonkers, M. The social cognition of gifted adolescents in schools: Managing the stigma of giftedness. J. Educ. Gift. 1991, 15, 44–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hebert, T.P. Understanding the Social and Emotional Lives of Gifted Students; Prufrock Press Inc.: Austin, TX, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Scrimgeour, M.B.; Blandon, A.Y.; Stifter, C.A.; Buss, K.A. Cooperative coparenting moderates the association between parenting practices and children’s prosocial behavior. J. Fam. Psychol. 2013, 27, 506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Knafo, A.; Plomin, R. Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: Genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 42, 771–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poelker, K.E.; Kuebli, J.E. Does the Thought Count? Gratitude Understanding in Elementary School Students. J. Genet. Psychol. 2014, 175, 431–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacFarlane, B.; Mina, K. Cyberbullying and the Gifted: Considerations for Social and Emotional Development. Gift. Child Today 2018, 41, 130–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noten, M.M.P.G.; Van der Heijden, K.B.; Huijbregts, S.C.J.; Van Goozen, S.H.M.; Swaab, H. Associations between empathy, inhibitory control, and physical aggression in toddlerhood. Dev. Psychobiol. 2020, 62, 871–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huang, M.Y.; Tu, H.Y.; Wang, W.Y.; Chen, J.F.; Yu, Y.T.; Chou, C.C. Effects of cooperative learning and concept mapping intervention on critical thinking and basketball skills in elementary school. Think. Skills Creat. 2017, 23, 207–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood, V.; Laycraft, K. How can we better understand, identify, and support highly gifted and profoundly gifted students? A literature review of the psychological development of highly-profoundly gifted individuals and overexcitabilities. Ann. Cogn. Sci. 2020, 4, 143–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shin, J.; Lee, B.Y. The effects of adolescent prosocial behavior interventions: A meta-analytic review. Asia Pac. Educ. Rev. 2021, 22, 565–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mesurado, B.; Guerra, P.; Richaud, M.C.; Rodriguez, L.M. Effectiveness of Prosocial Behavior Interventions: A Meta-analysis. In Psychiatry and Neuroscience Update: From Translational Research to a Humanistic Approach; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; Volume 3, pp. 259–271. [Google Scholar]
- Malti, T.; Chaparro, M.P.; Zuffianò, A.; Colasante, T. School-Based Interventions to Promote Empathy-Related Responding in Children and Adolescents: A Developmental Analysis. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2016, 45, 718–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nedim Bal, P. Examining the Permanence of the Effect of an Empathy Program for the Acquisition of Empathy Skills on Gifted Adolescents. Educ. Res. Rev. 2015, 10, 2314–2323. [Google Scholar]
- Chan, H.H.K.; Kwong, H.Y.C.; Shu, G.L.F.; Ting, C.Y.; Lai, F.H.Y. Effects of Experiential Learning Programmes on Adolescent Prosocial Behaviour, Empathy, and Subjective Well-being: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 709699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wellman, H.M.; Cross, D.; Watson, J. Meta-Analysis of Theory-of-Mind Development: The Truth about False Belief. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 655–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Walker, S. Gender Differences in the Relationship Between Young Children’s Peer-Related Social Competence and Individual Differences in Theory of Mind. J. Genet. Psychol. 2005, 166, 297–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Burnside, K.; Wright, K.; Poulin-Dubois, D. Social orienting predicts implicit false belief understanding in preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 175, 67–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burt, K.B.; Obradović, J.; Long, J.D.; Masten, A.S. The Interplay of Social Competence and Psychopathology Over 20 Years: Testing Transactional and Cascade Models. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 359–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hughes, C. Theory of mind grows up: Reflections on new research on theory of mind in middle childhood and adolescence. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2016, 149, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Devine, R.T.; Hughes, C. Silent Films and Strange Stories: Theory of Mind, Gender, and Social Experiences in Middle Childhood. Child Dev. 2013, 84, 989–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slaughter, V.; Imuta, K.; Peterson, C.C.; Henry, J.D. Meta-Analysis of Theory of Mind and Peer Popularity in the Preschool and Early School Years. Child Dev. 2015, 86, 1159–1174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Banerjee, R.; Watling, D.; Caputi, M. Peer Relations and the Understanding of Faux Pas: Longitudinal Evidence for Bidirectional Associations. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1887–1905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hughes, C.; Leekam, S. What are the Links Between Theory of Mind and Social Relations? Review, Reflections and New Directions for Studies of Typical and Atypical Development. Soc. Dev. 2004, 13, 590–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Apperly, I.A. What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2012, 65, 825–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liddle, B.; Nettle, D. Higher-order theory of mind and social competence in school-age children. J. Cult. Evol. Psychol. 2007, 4, 231–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abdulla Alabbasi, A.M.; Alaa, A.E.; Ziegler, A. Are gifted students more emotionally intelligent than their non-gifted peers? A meta-analysis. High Abil. Stud. 2021, 32, 189–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Košir, K.; Horvat, M.; Aram, U.; Jurinec, N. Is being gifted always an advantage? Peer relations and self-concept of gifted students. High Abil. Stud. 2016, 27, 129–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shantz, C. The Development of Social Cognition; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Eren, F.; Ömerelli Çete, A.; Avcil, S.; Baykara, B. Emotional and behavioral characteristics of gifted children and their families. Arch. Neuropsychiatry 2018, 55, 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shechtman, Z.; Silektor, A. Social Competencies and Difficulties of Gifted Children Compared to Nongifted Peers. Roeper. Rev. 2012, 34, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sak, U.; Sezerel, B.B.; Dulger, E.; Sozel, K.; Ayas, M.B. Validity of the Anadolu-Sak Intelligence Scale in the identification of gifted students. Psychol. Test Assess. Model. 2019, 61, 263–283. [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, A.; Goodman, R. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a Dimensional Measure of Child Mental Health. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2009, 48, 400–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masoom Ali, S.; Yildirim, M.; Abdul Hussain, S.; Vostanis, P. Self-reported mental health problems and post-traumatic growth among children in Pakistan care homes. Asia Pac. J. Soc. Work. Dev. 2020, 30, 62–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Güvenir, T.; Özbek, A.; Baykara, B.; Arkar, H.; Şentürk, B.; İncekaş, S. Güçler ve güçlükler anketi’nin (gga) Türkçe uyarlamasinin psikometrik özellikleri. Turk. J. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2008, 15, 65–74. [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen, S.; Jolliffe, T.; Mortimore, C.; Robertson, M. Another Advanced Test of Theory of Mind: Evidence from Very High Functioning Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 813–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Girli, A. Psychometric Properties of the Turkish Child and Adult Form of “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test”. Psychology 2014, 2014, 1321–1337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron-Cohen, S.; O’Riordan, M.; Stone, V.; Jones, R.; Plaisted, K. Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 1999, 29, 407–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sahin, B.; Onal, B.S.; Hosoglu, E. Adaptation of Faux Pas Recognition Test Child Form to Turkish and investigation of psychometric properties/Gaf Tanima Testi Cocuk Formunun Turkceye uyarlanmasi ve psikometrik ozelliklerinin incelenmesi. Anadolu. Psikiyatri. Derg. 2020, 21 (Suppl. S2), 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sözel, H.K.; Öpengin, E.; Sak, U.; Karabacak, F. The Discriminant Validity of the Anadolu-Sak Intelligence Scale ASIS for Gifted and Other Special Education Groups. Talent 2018, 8, 160–180. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, W.J.; McGrew, K.S. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues, 3rd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 99–144. [Google Scholar]
- Tamul, Ö.F.; Bal Sezerel, B.; Sak, U.; Karabacak, F. Anadolu-Sak Zekâ Ölçeği’nin (ASIS) sosyal geçerlik çalışması. Pamukkale Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Derg. 2020, 49, 393–412. [Google Scholar]
- Arslan, D.; Sak, U. Factorial Invariance of the Anadolu Sak Intelligence Scale Across Gender. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2023, 41, 542–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bastien, L.; Théoret, R.; Godbout, R. Association between sleep and problematic behaviours in gifted children: A polysomnographic study. J. Sleep Res. 2023, 32, e13807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slaughter, V.; Dennis, M.J.; Pritchard, M. Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2002, 20, 545–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strauss, S.; Ziv, M.; Stein, A. Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Cogn. Dev. 2002, 17, 1473–1487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Şahin, B.; Önal, B.S.; Hoşoğlu, E. Gaf Tanıma Testi Çocuk Formunun Türkçeye uyarlanması ve psikometrik özelliklerinin incelenmesi. Anatol. J. Psychiatry/Anadolu Psikiyatr. Derg. 2020, 21, 54–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tesfaye, R.; Gruber, R. The Association between Sleep and Theory of Mind in School Aged Children with ADHD. Med. Sci. 2017, 5, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Francis, R.; Hawes, D.J.; Abbott, M. Intellectual giftedness and psychopathology in children and adolescents: A systematic literature review. Except. Child. 2016, 82, 279–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bell, S.M.; Schindler, W.J. Collaboration at a professional development school: Investigation of variables affecting student achievement and adjustment. Tenn. Educ. 2002, 31, 5–10. [Google Scholar]
- Lacour, A.G.; Zdanowicz, N. IQ Over 130 and Phobia: Correlation, Consequences and Other Psychopathologies. Psychiatr. Danub. 2019, 31, 386–389. [Google Scholar]
- Brouwer, J.; Engels, M.C. The role of prosocial attitudes and academic achievement in peer networks in higher education. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2022, 37, 567–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gómez-Pérez, M.M.; Calero, M.D. The influence of intelligence and sex on interpersonal skills and executive functions in children. High Abil. Stud. 2023, 34, 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van der Graaff, J.; Carlo, G.; Crocetti, E.; Koot, H.M.; Branje, S. Prosocial Behavior in Adolescence: Gender Differences in Development and Links with Empathy. J. Youth Adolesc. 2018, 47, 1086–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tasca, I.; Guidi, M.; Turriziani, P.; Mento, G.; Tarantino, V. Behavioral and Socio-Emotional Disorders in Intellectual Giftedness: A Systematic Review. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2022, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Waller, R.; Hyde, L.W.; Baskin-Sommers, A.R.; Olson, S.L. Interactions between Callous Unemotional Behaviors and Executive Function in Early Childhood Predict later Aggression and Lower Peer-liking in Late-childhood. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2017, 45, 597–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coplan, R.J.; Weeks, M. Shy and soft-spoken: Shyness, pragmatic language, and socio-emotional adjustment in early childhood. Infant. Child Dev. 2009, 18, 238–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holmes, C.J.; Kim-Spoon, J.; Deater-Deckard, K. Linking Executive Function and Peer Problems from Early Childhood through Middle Adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2016, 44, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roiser, J.P.; Sahakian, B.J. Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectr. 2013, 18, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stone, V.E.; Gerrans, P. What’s domain-specific about theory of mind? Soc. Neurosci. 2006, 1, 309–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Di Tella, M.; Ardito, R.B.; Dutto, F.; Adenzato, M. On the (lack of) association between theory of mind and executive functions: A study in a non-clinical adult sample. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 17283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Boys | t or z or χ² | p | d | Girls | t or z or χ² | p | d | Total Sample | t or z or χ² | p | d/V | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gifted children (n = 22) | Non-Gifted (n = 18) | Gifted children (n = 23) | Non-Gifted (n = 27) | Gifted children (n = 45) | Non-Gifted (n = 45) | ||||||||||
Sex (boy/girl) | 22/0 | 18/0 | - | - | 0/23 | 0/27 | - | - | 22/23 | 18/27 | 0.720 | 0.396 | 0.089 | ||
Age (years) | 11.7 ± 0.5 | 11.5 ± 0.5 | 1.337 | 0.189 | 0.425 | 11.6 ± 0.6 | 11.6 ± 0.6 | −0.115 | 0.909 | 0.033 | 11.6 ± 0.6 | 11.5 ± 0.5 | 0.711 | 0.481 | 0.150 |
SDQ | |||||||||||||||
Peer problems | 2.3 ± 1.5 | 4.2 ± 1.9 | −3.437 | 0.001 | 1.092 | 2.4 ± 2.1 | 2.7 ± 1.7 | −0.810 | 0.418 | 0.229 | 2.4 ± 1.8 | 3.3 ± 1.9 | −0.244 | 0.017 | 0.515 |
Prosocial behavior | 8.1 ± 1.6 | 6.8 ± 2.1 | 2.127 | 0.040 | 0.676 | 8.0 ± 2.1 | 8.7 ± 1.6 | −0.994 | 0.320 | 0.281 | 8.1 ± 1.9 | 7.9 ± 2.0 | −0.190 | 0.849 | 0.040 |
ToM tests | |||||||||||||||
RMET-C | 20.0 ± 2.3 | 17.2 ± 3.2 | 3.178 | 0.003 | 1.009 | 20.7 ± 2.9 | 19.6 ± 3.8 | 1.133 | 0.263 | 0.322 | 20.4 ± 2.6 | 18.7 ± 3.7 | 2.515 | 0.014 | 0.530 |
FPRT-C | 15.9 ± 2.2 | 12.5 ± 2.6 | 4.403 | <0.001 | 1.399 | 16.9 ± 1.9 | 14.8 ± 2.7 | 2.927 | 0.006 | 0.808 | 16.3 ± 2.1 | 13.9 ± 2.8 | 4.545 | <0.001 | 0.958 |
Gifted | Non-Gifted | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boys (n = 22) | Girls (n = 23) | t or z | p | d | Boys (n = 18) | Girls (n = 27) | t or z | p | d | |
SDQ | ||||||||||
Peer problems | 2.3 ± 1.5 | 2.4 ± 2.1 | −0.150 | 0.881 | 0.045 | 4.2 ± 1.9 | 2.7 ± 1.7 | −2.427 | 0.015 | 0.724 |
Prosocial behaviors | 8.1 ± 1.6 | 8.0 ± 2.1 | 0.140 | 0.889 | 0.042 | 6.8 ± 2.1 | 8.7 ± 1.6 | 2.849 | 0.004 | 0.809 |
ToM tests | ||||||||||
RMET-C | 20.0 ± 2.3 | 20.7 ± 2.9 | 1.088 | 0.277 | 0.323 | 17.2 ± 3.2 | 19.6 ± 3.8 | 2.187 | 0.029 | 0.652 |
FPRT-C | 15.9 ± 2.2 | 16.9 ± 1.9 | 1.603 | 0.109 | 0.478 | 12.5 ± 2.6 | 14.8 ± 2.7 | 2.701 | 0.007 | 0.805 |
MS | F | η2p | MS | F | η2p | MS | F | η2p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | Sex | Group × Sex Interaction | |||||||
SDQ | |||||||||
Peer problems | 28.066 | 8.237 ** | 0.087 | 11.247 | 3.301 | 0.037 | 13.599 | 3.991 * | 0.044 |
Prosocial behaviors | 2.205 | 0.613 | 0.007 | 15.483 | 4.300 * | 0.048 | 21.015 | 5.837 * | 0.064 |
ToM tests | |||||||||
RMET-C | 85.044 | 8.374 ** | 0.089 | 52.279 | 5.148 * | 0.056 | 15.868 | 1.563 | 0.018 |
FPRT-C | 154.204 | 26.832 ** | 0.238 | 58.199 | 10.127 ** | 0.105 | 11.051 | 1.923 | 0.022 |
ToM Tests | SDQ | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Peer Problems | Prosocial Behaviors | |||
Gifted | RMET-C | r p | −0.032 0.833 | −0.116 0.450 |
FPRT-C | r p | −0.278 0.064 | 0.227 0.134 | |
Non-Gifted | RMET-C | r p | −0.445 0.002 | 0.464 0.001 |
FPRT-C | r p | −0.471 0.001 | 0.346 0.020 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bozkurt, A.; Ayık, Z. The Relationship between Giftedness and Sex and Children’s Theory of Mind Skills and Social Behavior. Children 2024, 11, 253. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11020253
Bozkurt A, Ayık Z. The Relationship between Giftedness and Sex and Children’s Theory of Mind Skills and Social Behavior. Children. 2024; 11(2):253. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11020253
Chicago/Turabian StyleBozkurt, Abdullah, and Zekai Ayık. 2024. "The Relationship between Giftedness and Sex and Children’s Theory of Mind Skills and Social Behavior" Children 11, no. 2: 253. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11020253
APA StyleBozkurt, A., & Ayık, Z. (2024). The Relationship between Giftedness and Sex and Children’s Theory of Mind Skills and Social Behavior. Children, 11(2), 253. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11020253