Next Article in Journal
Dermatology Self-Medication in Nursing Students and Professionals: A Multicentre Study
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Korean Medicine Treatment on Patients with Postherpetic Neuralgia: A Retrospective Chart Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
How Does Trauma Make You Sick? The Role of Attachment in Explaining Somatic Symptoms of Survivors of Childhood Trauma
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus, Psychopathology, Uncertainty and Alexithymia: A Clinical and Differential Exploratory Study

by
Emanuele Maria Merlo
1,*,
Rita Tutino
2,
Liam Alexander MacKenzie Myles
3,
Angela Alibrandi
4,
Maria Carmela Lia
2 and
Domenico Minasi
2
1
Department of Biomedical and Dental Sciences and Morphofunctional Imaging, University of Messina, 98125 Messina, Italy
2
Pediatric Unit of Ospedali Riuniti Presidium, Grande Ospedale Metropolitano Bianchi Melacrino Morelli, 89124 Reggio Calabria, Italy
3
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 2JD, UK
4
Department of Economics, University of Messina, 98122 Messina, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Healthcare 2024, 12(2), 257; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020257
Submission received: 20 November 2023 / Revised: 19 December 2023 / Accepted: 17 January 2024 / Published: 19 January 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Clinical Psychology: The State of the Art)

Abstract

:
Type 1 diabetes mellitus (T1DM) represents a complex pathology affecting a large number of people. Research suggests that psychological factors influence coping with T1DM. This study aimed to investigate the presence and role of psychopathology, alexithymia and uncertainty in people affected by T1DM. The sample consisted of 137 patients (88 females, 49 males) affected by T1DM aged from 11 to 19 years old (Mean: 13.87; SD: 2.40). The diagnostic protocol consisted of a sociodemographic questionnaire, Self-administration Psychiatric Scales for Children and Adolescents (SAFA), Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20) and Intolerance to Uncertainty Scale-12 (IUS-12). Descriptive, differential, correlational and regression analyses were performed in order to examine the relationships between these variables. The results suggested the sample had high levels of psychopathological indexes, alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty. Also, there were significant differences between TAS-20 and IUS-12 distributions with respect to psychopathology. Correlations and multivariate linear regressions indicated age, gender and education significantly predicted alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty. This data suggest the presence of elevated psychopathology, alexithymia and uncertainty in people with diabetes.

1. Introduction

In line with various studies in the literature, chronic medical difficulties can cause psychological difficulties [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8]. Indeed, the role of psychological variables has been studied with regard to the physical consequences of chronic medical difficulties, the role that psychopathology plays in the progression of chronic medical difficulties and finally, the management of physical therapies [9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17].
This research suggests that psychological facets exercise a central influence over the quality of life of patients with diabetes. In particular, Rubin and colleagues [9] studied the prevalence, manifestation, consequences, and treatment of psychological disorders in persons with diabetes. They found that dietary restrictions, self-monitoring of blood glucose, taking insulin injections, and lack of support from family and health care professionals predicted decreases in compliance and adherence to treatments as well as the development of psychological difficulties. Moreover, Atlantis and colleagues [10] and Talaei [13] highlighted that many patients suffering from psychopathology due to chronic diseases feel as though they are a burden.
Several studies in the literature have focused on variables such as alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty, but there is a lack of knowledge regarding the relationship between emotional experience and pathology [18,19,20,21,22]. Authors such as Gibson and colleagues [19] have described the characteristics of intolerance of uncertainty in people suffering from diabetes, highlighting that prospective and inhibitory anxiety have an impact on these individuals through reduced quality of life and adverse biological factors. Marchini and colleagues [18] highlight the role of loss in the context of diabetes, studied both in dynamic and psychopathological terms, where loss produces mournful and depressive feelings in people with diabetes.
Barchetta and colleagues [22] highlight that diabetes can contribute to cognitive biases for past and present events, which can result in engagement in behaviors contrary to those indicated by medical professionals. As stated by Di Giuseppe and colleagues [23], the defensive patterns that patients can develop constitute a danger where the defense mechanisms cease to exercise a defensive role and take on psychopathogenic characteristics, as in the case of illness denial and psychosomatic manifestations of distress [24].
Alexithymia concerns the difficulty in describing, interpreting and identifying affective experiences. The role of alexithymia in chronic medical difficulties has often been emphasized in clinical research but must be considered within the context of the developmental stage of the medical condition [23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32]. The role of alexithymia in mental health difficulties is known in the literature, especially with reference to people experiencing chronic conditions [25,27]. Shang and colleagues [26] testify to the relationships between alexithymia and chronic conditions, identifying alexithymia as a significant predictor for the onset and maintenance of chronic conditions. The role of alexithymia and affective dysregulation is known in various healthcare settings and contexts, including the phenomenology related to diabetes.
Type 1 diabetes represents a condition with early onset and has the potential to give rise to psychological conditions that affect management of the difficulty and the quality of life of patients [33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42]. In particular, research suggests that management of chronic medical difficulties is influenced by alexithymia [43,44,45,46]. In a study published in 2014, Karukvi and Saarijärvi [43] identified the presence of alexithymia as a risk and prognostic factor for patient ill health. Particular reference must be made to the context of adolescence, where the ability to express issues related to affectivity is linked to the degree of development and psychoeducation of the individual [45]. Indeed, alexithymia’s role in diabetes and in the field of psychosomatic conditions has been well highlighted by previous studies [47,48,49,50,51,52,53]. As evidenced by studies in the literature, such as in the case of Taylor and colleagues [47], the physical implications of alexithymia on somatization of disorders are of fundamental importance. It is useful to note that not all populations affected by chronic conditions undergo psychodiagnostic evaluations useful for capturing adverse phenomena operating in a subclinical sense or fully active. Studies allow us to understand the phenomena on the basis of explanatory models that make it possible to use the concepts in the diagnostic-clinical and rehabilitation fields [49].
Despite self-management and empowerment policies, people with diabetes can transgress medical advice, which can produce organic damage [54,55,56,57,58]. From a clinical psychological point of view, it is important to understand what psychological facets contribute to transgressions of medical advice. Uncertainty may be a key variable since prospective and inhibitory anxiety due to a lack of knowledge can affect an individual’s mental health and behavior.
In line with current literature, the present study aimed to evaluate the presence of adverse factors in the quality of life of patients with T1DM. The variables taken into consideration included alexithymia, intolerance of uncertainty and the possible presence of psychopathology. The hypotheses of the study are presented below.

Study Hypotheses

Hypothesis 1:
Psychopathology, alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty are present in people with T1DM according to the normal and pathological group scores included in the validation studies and cut-offs.
Hypothesis 2:
There are significant differences among the total scale scores of anxiety, depression, obsession, psychogenic eating disorders, somatic symptoms and hypochondria quartiles in people scoring highly on measures of alexithymia and uncertainty.
Hypothesis 3:
There are significant differences among each of the provided psychopathology quartiles in people scoring highly on measures of alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty (Hypothesis 2 is confirmed).
Hypothesis 4:
There are significant correlations among age, education, age of the diagnosis, alexithymia, difficulty in identifying and describing feelings, and eternally oriented thinking.
Hypothesis 5:
Age, gender and education predict alexithymia and uncertainty.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedures

The sample consisted of 137 patients with a female sex prevalence (Female: 88; Male: 46) aged from 11 to 19 years old (Mean: 13.87; SD: 2.40). The study was conducted from April 2023 to October 2023, all participants were affected by type 1 diabetes mellitus and were patients of the Pediatric Unit of the Ospedali Riuniti of Reggio Calabria. Recruitment was conducted during normal clinical activities of the Pediatric Unit, directed by Doctor Minasi. Inclusion criteria included age (10 to 20 years old), T1DM, and an absence of comorbidity. People presenting with other pathologies were excluded from the study. All patients were visited by an expert MD and involved in the study according to their consensus to participate. Participants were under pharmacological treatment for T1DM. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and parents/tutors for minors. All participants were informed about the anonymous nature of the data and fully completed the protocol.

2.2. Ethics

All procedures were consistent with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the local Ethical Committee (Comitato Etico Regionale—Sezione Area Sud, Grande Ospedale Metropolitano “Bianchi-Melacrino-Morelli” of Reggio Calabria, No.: 19-2022, from 27 April 2022 onwards).

2.3. Instruments

The sociodemographic characteristics of the sample were obtained through a self-administration questionnaire examining age, gender, education (expressed in years) and age at diagnosis of type 1 diabetes mellitus.

2.3.1. Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-12 (IUS-12) Text

The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale 12 (IUS-12) [59] is a 12-item scale dedicated to the clinical study of intolerance of uncertainty. Intolerance of uncertainty can be described as the tendency to react negatively to uncertainty regarding emotional, behavioral and cognitive feedback. IUS-12 is a self-report instrument using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “Not at all characteristic of me” to Entirely characteristic of me” (e.g., Item 1: Unforeseen events upset me greatly). The items are highly representative of the contents. The scale is composed of two main factors, prospective and inhibitory anxiety, and derives from a previous 27-item version of the scale known as IUS-27 [60,61]. The Italian version was adapted and validated through studies provided by Bottesi, Lauriola and colleagues [62,63]. According to Carleton and colleagues, who developed and validated the original version, the scale demonstrated consistent construct validity for total and subscale scores, internal reliability, and test–retest reliability (Cronbach’s α of 0.91, total scale, 0.85 for both subscale scores, r = 0.77) [59,64]. The Italian version reported high scores with reference to internal reliability, 0.80 for the IUS-12 total scale, 0.68 for prospective anxiety and 0.79 for inhibitory anxiety.

2.3.2. Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20)

The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) [65] is a 20-item self-report scale based on a 5-point Likert scale assessing alexithymia. Alexithymia can be described as the impossibility or severe difficulty in identifying and describing feelings and affective dynamics, followed by externally oriented thinking, which means a greater tendency to direct the thought to external dynamics rather than internal functioning. The 20 items are reported on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from Strongly Disagree to Strongly Agree (e.g., Item 1: “I am often confused about what emotion I am feeling”). The original version had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.81 and its structure emerged as three main factors accounting for 31% of the total variance. TAS-20 represents a well-known and useful instrument to detect the presence of alexithymia in a wide range of groups. Regarding the three-factor structure, the main dimensions of the scale are difficulty identifying feelings (0.78), difficulty describing feelings (0.75) and externally oriented thinking (0.66). According to Bressi and colleagues (Italian Validation) [66], the cross-validation, including clinical and nonclinical samples, reported 0.77, 0.67 and 0.52 for the first, second and third factors, respectively; the scores of the clinical sample were 0.82 for the full scale, and 0.79, 0.68 and 0.54 for the three factors. Further studies have analyzed the psychometric properties of the scale, highlighting the good consistency and reliability of the three-factor structure.

2.3.3. SAFA Scales

SAFA is a clinical instrument developed by Cianchetti and Sannio Fascello [67,68]. As a clinical psychometric test, it was validated in 2001. Its structure allows clinicians to complete a clinical investigation of the psychopathological conditions of tested participants. Its composition, despite being commonly presented as a unitary tool, is based on different scales assessing anxiety (SAFA Anxiety), depression (SAFA Depression), obsession (SAFA Obsession), somatic symptoms and hypochondria (SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria), psychogenic eating disorders (SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders) and phobias (as nominal variables, not considered scale). Considering the used scales, SAFA Anxiety is composed of 50 items, SAFA Depression is composed of 50 items, SAFA Obsession is composed of 38 items, Safa Psychogenic eating disorders is composed of 30 items and SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria of 25 items. All items are reported on a 3-point Likert Scale ranging from Not at all to Entirely. Referring to reliability, the original validation study considered both clinical and nonclinical participants. In these terms, the Cronbach’s alphas for SAFA Anxiety were 0.887 for the nonclinical sample and 0.956 for the clinical sample (test–retest Pearson r: 0.913, highly significant), 0.909 for the nonclinical sample and 0.943 for the clinical sample (test–retest Pearson r: 881, highly significant) of the SAFA Depression scale, 0.916 for the nonclinical sample and 0.895 for the clinical sample (test–retest Pearson r: 0.820) for the SAFA Obsession Scale, 0.814 for the nonclinical sample (test–retest Pearson r: 0.740, highly significant) for the SAFA Psychogenic eating disorder scale, and 0.876 for the nonclinical sample and 0.797 for the clinical sample (test-retest Pearson r: 0.567, highly significant) of the Somatic symptoms and hypochondria scale.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Numerical data were expressed as means and standard deviations, and the categorical variables as numbers and percentages. The Kruskal–Wallis test was applied to assess statistically significant differences among quartiles of the SAFA Scales for the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scores. After the emergence of significant differences among quartiles, the Mann–Whitney test was adopted for the analysis of each couple. The Pearson correlation coefficient (r) test was used for the correlational analyses. Multivariate linear regressions were applied in order to highlight dependencies among a set of predictors (age, education and gender) and dependent variables referred to alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty. A p-value smaller than 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant for multivariate linear regressions, and Kruskal–Wallis and Pearson correlation coefficient (r) tests. Using Bonferroni’s correction, a p-value smaller than 0.008 was considered to be significant for the Mann–Whitney test. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 26 for Windows package.

3. Results

3.1. Hypothesis 1

Descriptive statistics regarding Education, Age of the Diagnosis, SAFA, TAS-20 and IUS-12 variables are reported in Table 1.
Internal reliability coefficients (Cronbach alphas) for the used scale were 0.936 for SAFA Anxiety, 0.947 for SAFA Depression, 0.958 for SAFA Obsession, 0.934 for SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders, 0.953 for SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria, 0.730 for TAS-20 total score, 0.792 for TAS-20 difficulty identifying feelings, 0.727 for TAS-20 difficulty describing feelings, 0.685 for TAS-20 externally oriented thinking, 0.765 for IUS-12 total score, 0.630 for IUS-12 prospective anxiety and 0.740 for IUS-12 inhibitory anxiety.
Considering the first hypothesis, the scores in the scales were, in most cases, over the normal scores included in the clinical validation studies and manuals. With reference to SAFA scales, the scores were higher than reference normal groups [67] for Safa Anxiety (study score: 51.255; reference mean normal group score: 25.44; pathological mean group score: 63.36), SAFA Depression (study score: 58.350; reference mean normal group score: 23.5; pathological mean group score: 69.89), SAFA Obsession (study score: 34.817; reference mean normal group score: 21.73; pathological mean group score: 56.44), SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders (study score: 27.963; reference mean normal group score: 15.17; pathological mean group score: 45.02) and SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria (study score: 23.306; reference mean normal group score: 11.02; pathological mean group score: 24.43). Considering TAS-20 scores, the mean score was over the normal score cut-off (51) [66], demonstrating a borderline score for the whole sample and the presence of alexithymia over normal levels. With reference to the IUS-12 scores, in our sample, they were higher than in Italian validation studies [63] both for the IUS-12 total score (study score: 35.678; validation study score: 29.69), IUS-12 prospective anxiety (study score: 22.416; validation study score: 18.79) and IUS-12-inhibitory anxiety (study score: 13.622; validation study score 10.90).

3.2. Hypothesis 2

The Kruskal–Wallis test was used to identify statistically significant differences among SAFA quartiles compared to TAS-20 and IUS-12 factors. SAFA scale scores were divided into quartiles and considered with reference to each of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 factors. Table 2, Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6 report data referred to the Kruskal-Wallis analyses.
Table 2 reports descriptive statistics for TAS-20 and IUS-12 factors and total scores, including the Kruskal–Wallis analysis for the SAFA Anxiety scale. There were significant differences among quartiles for the TAS-20 total score, difficulty identifying feelings, IUS-12 total score and inhibitory anxiety. This data showed it was possible to notice precise points of variation among factors’ quartiles of the SAFA Anxiety scale.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Anxiety.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Anxiety.
Descriptive Statistics
SAFA
Quartiles
TAS-20
Total Score
TAS-20 Difficulty
Identifying Feelings
TAS-20 Difficulty
Describing Feelings
TAS-20 Externally-Oriented ThinkingIUS-12
Total Score
IUS-12
Prospective Anxiety
IUS-12
Inhibitory Anxiety
<Q1Mean47.05414.97311.83720.24332.70221.40511.297
SD12.4696.3354.9974.8159.0025.3405.054
Q1 ÷ Q2Mean61.88222.58817.50021.76437.64722.38215.264
SD9.4315.5764.6004.5398.7655.9284.826
Q2 ÷ Q3Mean57.69419.91615.38822.38838.63824.16614.472
SD7.5885.3954.1354.0097.1754.9364.601
>Q3Mean48.13315.13312.26612.26633.56633.56611.966
SD10.7027.0255.3624.3159.2164.7455.423
TotalMean53.76618.19714.27021.35035.67822.41622.416
SD11.9076.8325.2644.4648.8405.3285.190
Kruskal–Wallis
p value<0.001<0.001<0.0010.3040.0070.117<0.001
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Anxiety
Q1 = 36Q2 = 51Q3 = 69
Significant p value < 0.05.
Table 3 reports the data emerged considering possible differences among TAS-20 and IUS-12 factors of SAFA Depression quartiles. Several significant differences emerged, showing significant differences for TAS-20 total score, difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, IUS-12 total score and inhibitory anxiety. No significant differences emerged with reference to externally oriented thinking and prospective anxiety.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Depression.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Depression.
Descriptive Statistics
SAFA
Quartiles
TAS-20 Total ScoreTAS-20 Difficulty
Identifying Feelings
TAS-20 Difficulty
Describing Feelings
TAS-20 Externally-Oriented ThinkingIUS-12 Total ScoreIUS-12
Prospective Anxiety
IUS-12
Inhibitory Anxiety
<Q1Mean48.64715.70512.44120.50035.14722.14713.000
SD11.3086.9695.4724.2729.9635.6625.892
Q1 ÷ Q2Mean58.97521.31716.43921.21937.46323.14614.317
SD11.8625.8245.0005.0128.2675.0674.317
Q2 ÷ Q3Mean57.00020.32115.60721.96437.67823.35714.321
SD10.1505.8884.2633.9957.4485.4785.106
>Q3Mean49.94115.17612.38221.85232.41121.02911.382
SD10.8156.5294.9604.36328.7115.0784.905
TotalMean53.76618.19714.27021.35035.67822.41613.262
SD11.9076.8325.26434.4648.8405.3285.190
Kruskal–Wallis
p value<0.001<0.0010.0010.7180.0400.2120.046
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Depression
Q1 = 41.5Q2 = 58Q3 = 78
Significant p value < 0.05.
Table 4. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Obsession.
Table 4. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Obsession.
Descriptive Statistics
SAFA
Quartiles
TAS-20 Total ScoreTAS-20 Difficulty
Identifying Feelings
TAS-20 Difficulty
Describing Feelings
TAS-20 Externally-Oriented ThinkingIUS-12 Total ScoreIUS-12
Prospective Anxiety
IUS-12
Inhibitory Anxiety
<Q1Mean50.14216.40013.48520.25733.54220.85712.685
SD14.7097.0135.9775.00210.0095.7095.909
Q1 ÷ Q2Mean57.50020.41614.97222.08339.00024.38814.611
SD10.1916.2494.8194.6496.9524.5814.264
Q2 ÷ Q3Mean58.87520.84316.21821.81236.93722.96813.968
SD8.2096.4864.6474.2308.4085.6085.183
>Q3Mean48.73515.20512.50021.26433.17621.41111.764
SD10.3665.9984.9373.8168.7334.8435.039
TotalMean53.76618.19714.27021.35035.67822.41613.262
SD11.9076.8325.2644.4648.8405.32813.262
Kruskal–Wallis
p value<0.001<0.0010.0200.4950.0080.0220.087
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Obsession
Q1 = 14Q2 = 38Q3 = 48.5
Significant p value < 0.05.
Considering SAFA Obsession, several significant differences were found involving TAS-20 and IUS-12 factors. In particular, the significant differences concerned the TAS-20 total score, difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, IUS-12 total score and prospective anxiety. No significant differences emerged with reference to externally oriented thinking and inhibitory anxiety.
Table 5. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders.
Table 5. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders.
Descriptive Statistics
SAFA
Quartiles
TAS-20 Total ScoreTAS-20 Difficulty
Identifying Feelings
TAS-20 Difficulty
Describing Feelings
TAS-20 Externally-Oriented ThinkingIUS-12 Total ScoreIUS-12
Prospective Anxiety
IUS-12
Inhibitory Anxiety
<Q1Mean50.94716.34213.10521.50034.55221.60512.947
SD14.4187.3275.6744.9908.4294.5535.331
Q1 ÷ Q2Mean57.45721.34215.85721.00039.48524.42815.057
SD9.5896.1215.2474.5048.2975.1584.739
Q2 ÷ Q3Mean57.06620.73315.63320.66636.70023.26613.433
SD10.2345.5514.0294.1718.9875.7355.197
>Q3Mean50.20514.79412.73522.14732.11720.50011.617
SD10.8595.8555.2064.0988.3435.2875.093
TotalMean53.76618.19714.27021.35035.67822.41613.262
SD11.9076.8325.2644.4648.8405.3285.190
Kruskal–Wallis
p value0.011<0.0010.0150.4780.0090.0080.046
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders
Q1 = 13Q2 = 31Q3 = 39.5
Significant p value < 0.05.
Kruskal–Wallis test applied to SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders and considering possible differences among TAS-20 and IUS-12 factors’ quartiles, evidenced significant differences in the TAS-20 total score, difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, IUS-12 total score, prospective anxiety and inhibitory anxiety. In this case, the only variable not showing significant differences among quartiles was externally oriented thinking.
Table 6. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria.
Table 6. Descriptive statistics and Kruskal–Wallis analysis for SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria.
Descriptive Statistics
SAFA
Quartiles
TAS-20 Total ScoreTAS-20 Difficulty Identifying FeelingsTAS-20 Difficulty
Describing Feelings
TAS-20 Externally-Oriented ThinkingIUS-12
Total Score
IUS-12
Prospective Anxiety
IUS-12
Inhibitory Anxiety
<Q1Mean48.62815.34212.68520.60034.25721.65712.600
SD12.8406.9195.4494.5259.2745.3905.4567
Q1 ÷ Q2Mean62.05822.67617.50021.85239.97024.61715.352
SD11.1355.7935.05275.0467.4324.7224.715
Q2 ÷ Q3Mean55.76419.67614.50021.58835.76422.00013.764
SD7.6475.1034.2874.2719.5575.8515.393
>Q3Mean48.76415.17612.44121.38232.76421.41111.352
SD10.1626.5204.7744.0527.5484.8684.478
TotalMean53.76618.19714.27021.35035.67822.41613.262
SD11.9076.8325.2644.4648.8405.3285.190
Kruskal–Wallis
p value<0.001<0.001<0.0010.8200.0030.0150.012
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria
Q1 = 11Q2 = 23Q3 = 35.5
Significant p value < 0.05.
Exploring differences referred to SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria, TAS-20, total score, difficulty identifying feelings and difficulty describing feelings emerged as significantly different. In the IUS-12 case, all factors presented significant differences. In line with all analyses performed through the Kruskal–Wallis test, externally oriented thinking has never presented significant differences.

3.3. Hypothesis 3

According to the Hypothesis 3, the Mann–Whitney test was used to highlight the statistically significant differences among TAS-20 and IUS-12 quartiles that emerged through the Kruskal–Wallis test, as reported within Table 7, Table 8, Table 9, Table 10 and Table 11.
In the case of Anxiety, the Mann-Whitney test indicated several differences among TAS-12 and IUS-12 quartiles compared to the SAFA Anxiety scale. Starting with TAS-20 total score, significant differences were found between first and second, first and third, second and fourth, and third and fourth quartiles. The same results were obtained with reference to difficulty identifying feelings and difficulty describing feelings, highlighting the scale’s high consistency. Considering IUS-12 scales, the Kruskal–Wallis test showed significant differences only for IUS-12 total score and inhibitory anxiety. The only significant difference in the IUS-12 total score was between the first and third quartiles. In the case of inhibitory anxiety, significant values were found in the case of first and second, first and third, and second and fourth quartiles.
Table 8. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Depression.
Table 8. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Depression.
Q1 vs. Q2Q1 vs. Q3Q1 vs. Q4Q2 vs. Q3Q2 vs. Q4Q3 vs. Q4
TAS-20 Total score<0.0010.0010.5600.094<0.0010.037
TAS-20 Difficulty identifying feelings<0.0010.0040.7680.493<0.0010.002
TAS-20 Difficulty describing feelings0.0020.0170.9750.3510.0010.008
IUS-12 Total Score0.2590.2630.2460.8260.0110.013
IUS-12 Inhibitory anxiety0.3130.4110.3370.8250.0070.014
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Depression
Q1 = 41.5Q2 = 58Q3 = 78
Significant p-value < 0.008 after Bonferroni’s correction. Bold values were significant values.
Considering Depression, several significant differences emerged among IUS-12 and TAS-20 quartiles. Starting with TAS-20 total score, significant differences emerged among the first and second, first and third, and second and fourth quartiles. The same results emerged considering difficulty identifying feelings, including the significant difference between the third and fourth quartiles. Significant differences involving difficulty describing feelings’ quartiles involved the first and second and second and fourth quartiles. In the case of IUS-12, the only significant differences that emerged were those between the second and fourth quartiles for both IUS-12 total score and inhibitory anxiety.
Table 9. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Obsession.
Table 9. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Obsession.
Q1 vs. Q2Q1 vs. Q3Q1 vs. Q4Q2 vs. Q3Q2 vs. Q4Q3 vs. Q4
TAS-20 Total score0.0120.0030.8290.0020.002<0.001
TAS-20 Difficulty identifying feelings0.0090.0080.5600.0010.0010.001
TAS-20 Difficulty describing feelings0.2940.0640.5200.0290.0290.003
IUS-12 Total Score0.0120.1060.8050.0030.0030.030
IUS-12 Prospective anxiety0.0080.1640.8950.0090.0090.182
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Obsession
Q1 = 14Q2 = 38Q3 = 48.5
Significant p-value < 0.008 after Bonferroni’s correction. Bold values were significant values.
Analyses considering obsession of TAS-20 and IUS-12 quartiles showed fewer significant values with regard to previous analyses. Significant differences in TAS-20 total score were among the first and third, second and third, second and fourth, and third and fourth quartiles. TAS-20 identifying feelings quartiles showed significant differences in the case of the second and third, second and fourth, and third and fourth quartiles. The only significant difference that emerged considering difficulty describing feelings was between the third and fourth quartiles. The IUS-12 total score showed significant differences emerged between the second and third and second and fourth quartiles. No significant differences emerged considering IUS-12 prospective anxiety quartiles.
Table 10. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders.
Table 10. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders.
Q1 vs. Q2Q1 vs. Q3Q1 vs. Q4Q2 vs. Q3Q2 vs. Q4Q3 vs. Q4
TAS-20 Total score0.0220.0380.9190.9000.0090.016
TAS-20 Difficulty identifying feelings0.0030.0040.4660.659<0.001<0.001
TAS-20 Difficulty describing feelings0.0280.0390.7990.6680.0150.016
TAS-20 Externally-oriented thinking0.0350.5320.2020.2570.0010.034
IUS-12 Total Score0.0150.3120.1730.3870.0020.036
IUS-12 Prospective anxiety0.4000.2570.7170.9470.2860.181
IUS-12 Inhibitory anxiety0.1240.8090.2830.1980.0040.122
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders
Q1 = 13Q2 = 31Q3 = 39.5
Significant p-value < 0.008 after Bonferroni’s correction. Bold values were significant values.
Considering Eating Disorders, the statistically significant differences that emerged were regarding TAS-20 difficulty identifying feelings with reference to the first and second, first and third, second and fourth, and third and fourth quartiles, externally oriented thinking in the case of the second and fourth quartiles and IUS-12 total score showing a significant difference involving the second and fourth quartiles. No significant differences were found in the case of the other factors and scales.
Table 11. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria.
Table 11. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria.
Q1 vs. Q2Q1 vs. Q3Q1 vs. Q4Q2 vs. Q3Q2 vs. Q4Q3 vs. Q4
TAS-20 Total score<0.0010.0050.9430.004<0.0010.009
TAS-20 Difficulty identifying feelings<0.0010.0020.9900.025<0.0010.004
TAS-20 Difficulty describing feelings<0.0010.1470.9420.011<0.0010.108
IUS-12 Total Score0.0070.4170.4900.061<0.0010.132
IUS-12 Prospective anxiety0.0080.7180.7320.0440.0030.132
IUS-12 Inhibitory anxiety0.0370.3740.3960.1900.0010.055
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria
Q1 = 11Q2 = 23Q3 = 35.5
Significant p-value < 0.008 after Bonferroni’s correction. Bold values were significant values.
SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria concern somatic symptoms and hypochondria, so the emerged differences among TAS-20 and IUS-12 quartiles were regarding these fields. Most of the significant differences emerged between the first and second quartiles as well as the second and fourth quartiles. Surprisingly all values referred to the differences between the second and fourth quartiles were significant. Starting from the TAS-20 total score, differences among the first and second, second and third, and second and fourth quartiles were significant. Difficulty identifying feelings showed significant differences in the first and second, first and third, second and fourth, and third and fourth quartiles. Difficulty describing feelings showed two significant values regarding the first and second and second and fourth quartiles. All IUS-12 variables showed significant differences. IUS-12 total score highlighted significant values regarding the first and second and second and fourth quartiles. Prospective anxiety and inhibitory anxiety both showed significant differences between the second and fourth quartiles.

3.4. Hypothesis 4

Correlational analyses referred to Hypothesis 3 are reported in Table 12.
Correlational analysis showed several significant relationships. Starting from age, significant and positive correlations emerged involving TAS-20 total score, its externally oriented thinking factor and IUS-12 prospective anxiety. Education showed to be significant to age of the diagnosis and externally oriented thinking (TAS-20) and the emerged values were negative demonstrating opposite directions. Age of the diagnosis had a positive and significant correlation with Difficulties describing feelings (TAS-20). Considering these demographic variables, directions assumed by the above-mentioned significant correlations were positive for age and age of the diagnosis and negative for education. In this last case, education appeared to assume an opposite direction with reference to alexithymia. Several positive and significant correlations emerged for TAS-20 and IUS-12 total scores and related factors. Alexithymia showed the same direction assumed by intolerance of uncertainty, as in the case of the IUS-12 total score and prospective and inhibitory anxiety. The same significant and positive correlations were shown by both TAS-20 difficulty identifying and describing feelings of the IUS-12 total score and inhibitory anxiety. No significant correlations emerged for IUS-12 and TAS-20 externally oriented feelings.

3.5. Hypothesis 5

Multivariate linear regression analysis referred to Hypothesis 4 are reported in Table 13.
Multivariate linear regressions were used in order to highlight possible dependencies among the selected predictors. The TAS-20 and IUS-12 factors showed four significant values. Starting from age, a significant and positive dependency emerged with TAS-20 externally oriented thinking, showing how increasing age constitutes a causal predictor of improving this alexithymic factor. This datum appears to be in line with the literature considering externally oriented thinking regarding development. The second significant and positive dependency was with regard to gender (as a predictor) and TAS-20 difficulty identifying feelings. Considering the predictor’s values, this datum refers to female participants involved in the study. The third significant dependency to emerge was negative and involved education (predictor) and TAS-20 total score, highlighting how education assumes a positive role in diminishing alexithymia expression. The last one referred to education and externally oriented thinking, which was negative and in line with the previously stated, reinforcing the role of education in the decline of alexithymia.

4. Discussion

The results obtained through the analyses highlighted significant relationships between the variables. The first hypothesis concerned the presence of psychopathology, alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty in diabetes. The results demonstrated the presence of psychopathology, alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty in people with TWDM, consistent with the literature [10,13,14,69,70,71,72,73,74]. In particular, Popa-Varela and colleagues [11] highlighted the presence of psychopathology in people affected by T1DM. The study highlighted not only psychopathology but also the need for intervention to decrease pathological levels of anxiety, depression and somatic issues. Moreover, the authors defined this need as a challenge. Talaei [13] suggested the need to refer to adults, adolescents and children for support, with a view to understanding how the reality of people suffering from T1DM and T2DM is characterized by problems relating to mental health. Although in some studies the analyses stop at correlations, it is important to understand how some psychological variables play a causal role with respect to the chronic pathology and the quality of life of the participants. Şahin and colleagues [69] highlighted how issues relating to psychopathology not only impact those affected by the chronic disease but also how the issue extends to families and caregivers. In particular, the authors refer to the attitudes of parents and communities, emphasizing that awareness of such issues is necessary. The analyses of the studies cited, such as in the case of Skočić and colleagues [71] and Turin and Radobuljac [72], took into account the defense and coping phenomena of people affected by T1DM. As stated previously, peoples’ defensive patterns and coping methods can represent a strength. Likewise, the improper use of defenses and coping strategies can represent a risk factor. The use of coping and defenses can be protective, but it is equally known that strategies based on avoidance and improperly used defenses constitute an opportunity for the emergence of psychological problems. Any interventions must take into account not only the nature of these phenomena but also the fact that they essentially refer to developmental phases in which the participants’ cognitive functioning is not comparable to that of an adult. In terms of consequences, the study by Van Duinkerken and colleagues [75] highlights how clear the cognitive and psychological effects of living with a chronic pathology such as diabetes are, consistent with the results in this study.
The second hypothesis concerned the presence of significant differences between the scores related to psychopathology on measures of alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty. Referring to this first step, we expected the emergence of significant differences among quartiles. The subsequent hypotheses highlighted precisely where the differences were present and significant. In all cases, significant differences were found in the psychopathological domain (e.g., Depression, Obsession, Somatic Symptoms; Table 2, Table 3, Table 4, Table 5, Table 6, Table 7, Table 8, Table 9, Table 10 and Table 11). Through subsequent differential analyses, it was possible to deduce the exact points of differentiation. Although it was possible to find clear indications about the relationships existing among depression, anxiety, obsessive thought, somatic symptoms and eating disorders [74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85], this approach to considering differences regarding alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty in people with diabetes represented an innovative process. Indeed, it is possible to deduce precise distance thresholds in the distributions of the same scales. The detection of the presence of subthreshold symptoms represents an important starting point for the configuration of interventions since we are not faced with stable and evident psychopathological configurations on a symptomatic level [86]. In terms of primary prevention, testing for the presence of these subthreshold variables represents the first step. Unfortunately, in many cases, the study highlighted the presence of scores above the threshold, with a clear indication of how variables such as alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty play a differential role in the expression of anxiety, depression, somatic symptoms, eating disorders, hypochondriasis and thought disorders. In other words, the emergence of psychopathological variables beyond the threshold calls for psychotherapeutic intervention and psychological support aimed at the conditions.
The third hypothesis concerned correlational analyses that included both demographic variables and those referring to alexithymia and uncertainty. The relationships that emerged predicted opposite directions between age and alexithymia, confirming a greater tendency to modulate affectivity corresponding to adulthood [57]. The same negative and significant direction was taken during the study years, confirming the constructive role of education on mental functioning [86,87,88,89,90,91]. With respect to the correlations between alexithymia and uncertainty, the data are consistent with previous studies. In fact, both on an epistemological and a clinical psychological level, the poor processing of the affective experience and the growing intolerance of uncertainty take the same maladaptive direction. Even more so, poor processing of emotions, feelings and mood dynamics would place the individual in the presence of both prospective and inhibitory anxiety, exactly as described by the factors implicated in the study of the scales. Although the presence of an increased risk of alexithymia, intolerance of uncertainty and psychopathology in people with T1DM is clear, and it is important to clarify how the issues manifest themselves and what relationships exist between the variables.
The last hypothesis took into consideration age, gender and education. The role of these predictors with respect to alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty was statistically significant in four cases. Firstly, age appeared to be a significant predictor of externally oriented thinking, consistent with the literature. Gender significantly predicted difficulty in identifying feelings, with women finding it easier to identify feelings. The last two statistically significant findings involved education as a predictor of the total score and externally oriented thinking of the TAS scale. Both correlations were negative, suggesting that the role of education takes the opposite direction compared to alexithymia. These results appeared to be consistent with previous studies [45,85,86,87,88,92,93]. Nevertheless, further studies considering precisely participants affected by type 1 diabetes mellitus are needed. The role of the predictors highlighted by these analyses allows us to understand how variables such as age, education and gender influence equally important variables such as alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty. In line with the literature, education represents a fundamental point of reference in the decrease of psychopathological phenomena and conditions adverse to the health of participants.
However, the study has limitations that need to be discussed. Being a cross-sectional study, it is not possible to determine causal relationships between the variables. Moreover, this study failed to examine the relationship between the aforementioned variables and biological variables related to T1DM. Future studies should consider biological variables and whether they influence participants’ biological status, compliance and adherence to treatments.

5. Conclusions

The present study considered clinically significant variables in a sample composed of those suffering from type 1 diabetes mellitus. The hypotheses of the study suggested the presence of consistent levels of psychopathology, alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty. Beyond the descriptive statistics, several significant differences emerged in the scores on measures of alexithymia and intolerance of uncertainty, with respect to the psychopathological domains taken into consideration. It was possible to highlight a certain degree of suffering in the participants, often covered by variables of the order of alexithymia and expressed through anxiety and inhibition. Further studies should consider the statistical analyses performed in light of these hypotheses and the results in order to guide clinical decisions that are useful for helping people with diabetes and improving their quality of life.

Author Contributions

E.M.M., R.T. and D.M. made a significant contribution to the conception and the design of the study. E.M.M. and A.A. made a significant contribution to the design of the diagnostic protocol, statistical analysis of the data and drafting of the manuscript. R.T. and E.M.M. provided the acquisition of data useful for the statistical analysis. E.M.M. and L.A.M.M. made a significant contribution to the interpretation of data and comparison with literature. E.M.M., L.A.M.M., M.C.L., R.T. and D.M. revised the manuscript and gave their approval. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee Comitato Etico Regionale—Sezione Area Sud, Grande Ospedale Metropolitano “Bian-chi-Melacrino-Morelli” of Reggio Calabria, N°: 19-2022, from 27 April 2022 onwards.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent for publication has been waived due to the anonymous nature of data.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, E.M.M., upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Stanton, A.L.; Revenson, T.A.; Tennen, H. Health Psychology: Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007, 58, 565–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Marchini, F.; Caputo, A.; Napoli, A.; Balonan, J.T.; Martino, G.; Nannini, V.; Langher, V. Chronic Illness as Loss of Good Self: Underlying Mechanisms Affecting Diabetes Adaptation. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2018, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Geist, R.; Grdisa, V.; Otley, A. Psychosocial Issues in the Child with Chronic Conditions. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Gastroenterol. 2003, 17, 141–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Schneiderman, N.; Antoni, M.H.; Saab, P.G.; Ironson, G. Health Psychology: Psychosocial and Biobehavioral Aspects of Chronic Disease Management. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001, 52, 555–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Dobbie, M.; Mellor, D. Chronic Illness and Its Impact: Considerations for Psychologists. Psychol. Health Med. 2008, 13, 583–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Merlo, E.M. Opinion Article: The Role of Psychological Features in Chronic Diseases, Advancements and Perspectives. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2019, 7, 2341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Sicari, F.; Merlo, E.M.; Gentile, G.; Nucera, R.; Portelli, M.; Settineri, S.; Myles, L.A.M.; Militi, A. Body Image and Psychological Impact of Dental Appearance in Adolescents with Malocclusion: A Preliminary Exploratory Study. Children 2023, 10, 1691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Spratt, C.J.; MacKenzie Myles, L.A.; Merlo, E.M. Eating Disorders in Men: A Comprehensive Summary. J. Mind Med. Sci. 2022, 9, 249–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Rubin, R.R.; Peyrot, M. Psychological Issues and Treatments for People with Diabetes. J. Clin. Psychol. 2001, 57, 457–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Atlantis, E. Excess Burden of Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes Due to Psychopathology. J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 142, S36–S41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Popa-Velea, O.; Bubulac, L.; Petrescu, L.; Purcarea, R.M. Psychopathology and Psychotherapeutic Intervention in Diabetes: Particularities, Challenges, and Limits. J. Med. Life 2016, 9, 328. [Google Scholar]
  12. Myles, L.; Merlo, E. Elucidating the Cognitive Mechanisms Underpinning Behavioural Activation. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 2022, 15, 126–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Talaei, A. Comparison of Psychopathology in Type1 and Type2 Diabetic Patients. Eur. Psychiatry 2009, 24, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Garrett, C.J.; Ismail, K.; Fonagy, P. Understanding Developmental Psychopathology in Type 1 Diabetes through Attachment, Mentalisation and Diabetes Distress. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2021, 26, 682–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Vita, R.; Caputo, A.; Quattropani, M.C.; Watt, T.; Feldt-Rasmussen, U.; Puleio, P.; Benvenga, S.; Martino, G. Quality of Life in Patients with Hyperthyroidism: Where Do We Stand? Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2020, 8, 2521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Myles, L.; Merlo, E. Incongruities between Perceived Control and Desire for Control: Accounting for Depressive Symptomology in Adolescence. Psychiatr. Psychol. Klin. J. Psychiatry Clin. Psychol. 2022, 22, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Martino, G.; Caputo, A.; Vicario, C.M.; Feldt-Rasmussen, U.; Watt, T.; Quattropani, M.C.; Benvenga, S.; Vita, R. Alexithymia, Emotional Distress, and Perceived Quality of Life in Patients with Hashimoto’s Thyroiditis. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 667237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Marchini, F.; Langher, V.; Napoli, A.; Balonan, J.T.; Fedele, F.; Martino, G.; Amorosi, F.R.; Caputo, A. Unconscious Loss Processing in Diabetes: Associations with Medication Adherence and Quality of Care. Psychoanal. Psychother. 2021, 35, 5–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gibson, B.; Rosser, B.A.; Schneider, J.; Forshaw, M.J. The Role of Uncertainty Intolerance in Adjusting to Long-Term Physical Health Conditions: A Systematic Review. PLoS ONE 2023, 18, e0286198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Martino, G.; Caputo, A.; Vicario, C.M.; Catalano, A.; Schwarz, P.; Quattropani, M.C. The Relationship between Alexithymia and Type 2 Diabetes: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fayazbakhsh, E.; Mansouri, A. Effectiveness of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy on Intolerance of Uncertainty, Experiential Avoidance, and Symptoms of Generalized Anxiety Disorder in Individuals with Type II Diabetes. Int. Arch. Health Sci. 2019, 6, 30–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Barchetta, S.; Martino, G.; Craparo, G.; Salehinejad, M.A.; Nitsche, M.A.; Vicario, C.M. Alexithymia Is Linked with a Negative Bias for Past and Current Events in Healthy Humans. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2021, 18, 6696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Di Giuseppe, M.; Conversano, C. Psychological Components of Chronic Diseases: The Link between Defense Mechanisms and Alexithymia. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2022, 10, 3602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Myles, L.A.M.; Merlo, E.M. Alexithymia and Physical Outcomes in Psychosomatic Subjects: A Cross-Sectional Study. J. Mind Med. Sci. 2021, 8, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Silvestro, O.; Ricciardi, L.; Catalano, A.; Vicario, C.M.; Tomaiuolo, F.; Pioggia, G.; Squadrito, G.; Schwarz, P.; Gangemi, S.; Martino, G. Alexithymia and Asthma: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1221648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Shang, B.; Chen, R.; Luo, C.; Lv, F.; Wu, J.; Shao, X.; Li, Q. The Relationship between Alexithymia, Depression, Anxiety, and Stress in Elderly with Multiple Chronic Conditions in China: A Network Analysis. Front. Psychiatry 2023, 14, 1209936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ricciardi, L.; Spatari, G.; Vicario, C.M.; Liotta, M.; Cazzato, V.; Gangemi, S.; Martino, G. Clinical Psychology and Clinical Immunology: Is There a Link between Alexithymia and Severe Asthma? Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2023, 11, 3704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Gangemi, S.; Ricciardi, L.; Caputo, A.; Giorgianni, C.; Furci, F.; Spatari, G.; Martino, G. Alexithymia in an Unconventional Sample of Forestry Officers: A Clinical Psychological Study with Surprising Results. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2021, 9, 3245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Tesio, V.; Goerlich, K.S.; Hosoi, M.; Castelli, L. Alexithymia: State of the Art and Controversies. Clinical and Neuroscientific Evidence. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Muzi, S.; Pace, C.S. A Pilot Study on Alexithymia in Adopted Youths: Prevalence and Relationships with Emotional-Behavioral Problems. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2020, 8, 2583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Teixeira, R.J.; Marques, M.V.; Brandão, T.; Pinto, R.; Pereira, A. Exploratory Effects of Structured Mindfulness Programs (MBCT and MBSR) on Posttraumatic Growth, Alexithymia and Type D Personality. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2022, 10, 3570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Mezzalira, S.; Scandurra, C.; Santaniello, A.; Mezza, F.; Saturnino, C.; Cristiano, S.; Pizzo, R.; Maldonato, N.M.; Menna, L.F.; Bochicchio, V. Affiliative Bonds and Cruel Behavior in Childhood Interspecific Relationships: A Conceptual Framework on the Psychodynamics of Affect Regulation. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2022, 10, 3547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Moroianu, L.A.; Motofei, I.G.; Cecilia, C.; Barbu, R.E.; Toma, A. The Impact of Anxiety and Depression on the Pediatric Patients with Diabetes. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2020, 8, 2435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lumley, M.A.; Stettner, L.; Wehmer, F. How Are Alexithymia and Physical Illness Linked? A Review and Critique of Pathways. J. Psychosom. Res. 1996, 41, 505–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Aaron, R.V.; Fisher, E.A.; Palermo, T.M. Alexithymia in Adolescents with and without Chronic Pain. Rehabil. Psychol. 2019, 64, 469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Baiardini, I.; Abbà, S.; Ballauri, M.; Vuillermoz, G.; Braido, F. Alexithymia and Chronic Diseases: The State of the Art. G. Ital. Med. Lav. Ergon. 2011, 33, A47–A52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Feiguine, R.J.; Johnson, F.A. Alexithymia and Chronic Respiratory Disease: A Review of Current Research. Psychother. Psychosom. 1985, 43, 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Pușcașu, A.; Bolocan, A.; Păduraru, D.N.; Salmen, T.; Bica, C.; Andronic, O. The Implications of Chronic Psychological Stress in the Development of Diabetes Mellitus Type 2. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2022, 10, 3544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Catchlove, R.F.; Cohen, K.R.; Braha, R.E.; Demers-Desrosiers, L.A. Incidence and Implications of Alexithymia in Chronic Pain Patients. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1985, 173, 246–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Barone, M.; Savarese, L.; Freda, M.F. Living with a Chronic Disease: The Role of Maternal Mediation in the Meaning-Making Process of Their Children’s Type 1 Diabetes. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2019, 7, 2187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ademoyegun, A.B.; Afolabi, O.E.; Aghedo, I.A.; Adelowokan, O.I.; Mbada, C.E.; Awotidebe, T.O. The Mediating Role of Sedentary Behaviour in the Relationship between Social Support and Depression among Individuals with Diabetes. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2022, 10, 3420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kojima, M. Alexithymia as a Prognostic Risk Factor for Health Problems: A Brief Review of Epidemiological Studies. Biopsychosoc. Med. 2012, 6, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Karukivi, M.; Saarijärvi, S. Development of Alexithymic Personality Features. World J. Psychiatry 2014, 4, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M. Psychoanalysis and Empirical Research: The Example of Alexithymia. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 2013, 61, 99–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Runcan, R. Alexithymia in Adolescents: A Review of Literature. Agora Psycho-Pragmatica 2020, 14, 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  46. Hadji-Michael, M.; McAllister, E.; Reilly, C.; Heyman, I.; Bennett, S. Alexithymia in Children with Medically Unexplained Symptoms: A Systematic Review. J. Psychosom. Res. 2019, 123, 109736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D. The Alexithymia Construct: A Potential Paradigm for Psychosomatic Medicine. Psychosomatics 1991, 32, 153–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Stephenson, R. Introducing Alexithymia: A Concept within the Psychosomatic Process. Disabil. Rehabil. 1996, 18, 209–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. López-Muñoz, F.; Pérez-Fernández, F. A History of the Alexithymia Concept and Its Explanatory Models: An Epistemological Perspective. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 10, 1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Catrone, C. An Integrated Treatment of Psychosomatic Symptoms and Disorders of the Self: The Role of Alexithymia. Psychoanal. Soc. Work 2021, 28, 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gorobets, E.A.; Esin, O.R. Alexithymia and Psychosomatic Diseases in Adolescents: Primary Headaches. Acta Biomed. Sci. 2023, 8, 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. O’Malley, P. Alexithymia in Children/Adolescents and Psychosomatic Families. In Handbook of Mind/Body Integration in Child and Adolescent Development; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2023; pp. 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Šago, D.; Babić, G. Roots of Alexithymia. Arch. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 55, 71–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Settineri, S.; Frisone, F.; Merlo, E.M.; Geraci, D.; Martino, G. Compliance, Adherence, Concordance, Empowerment, and Self-Management: Five Words to Manifest a Relational Maladjustment in Diabetes. J. Multidiscip. Healthc. 2019, 12, 299–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Alvarado-Martel, D.; Ruiz Fernández, M.; Cuadrado Vigaray, M.; Carrillo, A.; Boronat, M.; Exposito Montesdeoca, A.; Wägner, A.M. Identification of Psychological Factors Associated with Adherence to Self-Care Behaviors amongst Patients with Type 1 Diabetes. J. Diabetes Res. 2019, 2019, 6271591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Martino, G.; Bellone, F.; Langher, V.; Caputo, A.; Catalano, A.; Quattropani, M.C.; Morabito, N. Alexithymia and Psychological Distress Affect Perceived Quality of Life in Patients with Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2019, 7, 2328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Almeida, A.C.; Leandro, M.E.; Pereira, M.G. Adherence and Glycemic Control in Adolescents with Type 1 Diabetes: The Moderating Role of Age, Gender, and Family Support. J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 2020, 27, 247–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Popoviciu, M.S.; Marin, V.N.; Vesa, C.M.; Stefan, S.D.; Stoica, R.A.; Serafinceanu, C.; Merlo, E.M.; Rizvi, A.A.; Rizzo, M.; Busnatu, S. Correlations between Diabetes Mellitus Self-Care Activities and Glycaemic Control in the Adult Population: A Cross-Sectional Study. Healthcare 2022, 10, 174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Carleton, R.N.; Norton, M.P.J.; Asmundson, G.J. Fearing the Unknown: A Short Version of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. J. Anxiety Disord. 2007, 21, 105–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Buhr, K.; Dugas, M.J. The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale: Psychometric Properties of the English Version. Behav. Res. Ther. 2002, 40, 931–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Norton, P.J. A Psychometric Analysis of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale among Four Racial Groups. J. Anxiety Disord. 2005, 19, 699–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Bottesi, G.; Ghisi, M.; Carraro, E.; Barclay, N.; Payne, R.; Freeston, M.H. Revising the Intolerance of Uncertainty Model of Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Evidence from UK and Italian Undergraduate Samples. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Lauriola, M.; Mosca, O.; Carleton, R.N. Hierarchical Factor Structure of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale Short Form (IUS-12) in the Italian Version. TPM-Test. Psychom. Methodol. Appl. Psychol. 2016, 23, 377–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Khawaja, N.G.; Yu, L.N.H. A Comparison of the 27-item and 12-item Intolerance of Uncertainty Scales. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 14, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D.; Taylor, G.J. The Twenty-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item Selection and Cross-Validation of the Factor Structure. J. Psychosom. Res. 1994, 38, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Bressi, C.; Taylor, G.; Parker, J.; Bressi, S.; Brambilla, V.; Aguglia, E.; Allegranti, I.; Bongiorno, A.; Giberti, F.; Bucca, M. Cross Validation of the Factor Structure of the 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: An Italian Multicenter Study. J. Psychosom. Res. 1996, 41, 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Cianchetti, C.; Sannio Fascello, G. Scale Psichiatriche Di Autosomministrazione per Fanciulli e Adolescenti (SAFA); Organizzazioni Speciali: Firenze, Italy, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  68. Franzoni, E.; Monti, M.; Pellicciari, A.; Muratore, C.; Verrotti, A.; Garone, C.; Cecconi, I.; Iero, L.; Gualandi, S.; Savarino, F. SAFA: A New Measure to Evaluate Psychiatric Symptoms Detected in a Sample of Children and Adolescents Affected by Eating Disorders. Correlations with Risk Factors. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2009, 5, 207–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Şahin, N.; Öztop, D.B.; Yilmaz, S.; Altun, H. Assessment of Psychopathology, Quality of Life, and Parental Attitudes in Adolescents with Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus. Nöro Psikiyatri Arş. 2015, 52, 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Chamorro, M.M.; Martinez, I.L.; Tomas, C.L. Psychopathology and Child and Adolescents Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus Outcome. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 2002, 30, 175–181. [Google Scholar]
  71. Skočić, M.; Rudan, V.; Brajković, L.; Marčinko, D. Relationship among Psychopathological Dimensions, Coping Mechanisms, and Glycemic Control in a Croatian Sample of Adolescents with Diabetes Mellitus Type 1. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 19, 525–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Turin, A.; Radobuljac, M.D. Psychosocial Factors Affecting the Etiology and Management of Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus: A Narrative Review. World J. Diabetes 2021, 12, 1518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Kafali, H.Y.; Altinok, Y.A.; Ozbaran, B.; Ozen, S.; Kose, S.; Tahillioglu, A.; Darcan, S.; Goksen, D. Exploring Emotional Dysregulation Characteristics and Comorbid Psychiatric Disorders in Type 1 Diabetic Children with Disordered Eating Behavior Risk. J. Psychosom. Res. 2020, 131, 109960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Farooqi, A.; Gillies, C.; Sathanapally, H.; Abner, S.; Seidu, S.; Davies, M.J.; Polonsky, W.H.; Khunti, K. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis to Compare the Prevalence of Depression between People with and without Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes. Prim. Care Diabetes 2022, 16, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Van Duinkerken, E.; Snoek, F.J.; De Wit, M. The Cognitive and Psychological Effects of Living with Type 1 Diabetes: A Narrative Review. Diabet. Med. 2020, 37, 555–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Herder, C.; Hermanns, N. Subclinical Inflammation and Depressive Symptoms in Patients with Type 1 and Type 2 Diabetes. Semin. Immunopathol. 2019, 41, 477–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Amiri, S.; Behnezhad, S. Diabetes and Anxiety Symptoms: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Cobham, V.E.; Hickling, A.; Kimball, H.; Thomas, H.J.; Scott, J.G.; Middeldorp, C.M. Systematic Review: Anxiety in Children and Adolescents with Chronic Medical Conditions. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 59, 595–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Winston, A.P. Eating Disorders and Diabetes. Curr. Diab. Rep. 2020, 20, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Pursey, K.M.; Hart, M.; Jenkins, L.; McEvoy, M.; Smart, C.E. Screening and Identification of Disordered Eating in People with Type 1 Diabetes: A Systematic Review. J. Diabetes Complications 2020, 34, 107522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Benton, M.; Cleal, B.; Prina, M.; Baykoca, J.; Willaing, I.; Price, H.; Ismail, K. Prevalence of Mental Disorders in People Living with Type 1 Diabetes: A Systematic Literature Review and Meta-Analysis. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 2023, 80, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Ding, H.; Ouyang, M.; Wang, J.; Xie, M.; Huang, Y.; Yuan, F.; Jia, Y.; Wang, J.; Liu, N.; Zhang, N. Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and Metabolic Disorders: Overview and Future Perspective. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2022, 210, 951–959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Myles, L.A.M.; Merlo, E.M.; Obele, A. Desire for Control Moderates the Relationship between Perceived Control and Depressive Symptomology. J. Mind Med. Sci. 2021, 8, 299–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Myles, L.A.M.; Connolly, J.; Stanulewicz, N. The Mediating Role of Perceived Control and Desire for Control in the Relationship between Personality and Depressive Symptomology. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2020, 8, 2589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Lane, R.D.; Sechrest, L.; Riedel, R. Sociodemographic Correlates of Alexithymia. Compr. Psychiatry 1998, 39, 377–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Scull, A. American psychiatry in the new millennium: A critical appraisal. Psychol. Med. 2021, 51, 2762–2770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Pasini, A.; Delle Chiaie, R.; Seripa, S.; Ciani, N. Alexithymia as Related to Sex, Age, and Educational Level: Results of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale in 417 Normal Subjects. Compr. Psychiatry 1992, 33, 42–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Mattila, A.K.; Salminen, J.K.; Nummi, T.; Joukamaa, M. Age Is Strongly Associated with Alexithymia in the General Population. J. Psychosom. Res. 2006, 61, 629–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Salminen, J.K.; Saarijärvi, S.; Äärelä, E.; Toikka, T.; Kauhanen, J. Prevalence of Alexithymia and Its Association with Sociodemographic Variables in the General Population of Finland. J. Psychosom. Res. 1999, 46, 75–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Joukamaa, M.; Taanila, A.; Miettunen, J.; Karvonen, J.T.; Koskinen, M.; Veijola, J. Epidemiology of Alexithymia among Adolescents. J. Psychosom. Res. 2007, 63, 373–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Erdem, H.; Engin, E. Examination of the Level of Alexithymia in Adult Individuals in Terms of Different Demographic Factors. Psychol. Res. Educ. Soc. Sci. 2020, 1, 65–74. [Google Scholar]
  92. Kauhanen, J.; Kaplan, G.A.; Julkunen, J.; Wilson, T.W.; Salonen, J.T. Social Factors in Alexithymia. Compr. Psychiatry 1993, 34, 330–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Parker, J.D.; Taylor, G.J.; Bagby, R.M. The Alexithymia Construct: Relationship with Sociodemographic Variables and Intelligence. Compr. Psychiatry 1989, 30, 434–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for clinical variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for clinical variables.
MeanSDMin-Max
(Scales Scores)
Min-Max
(Study Scores)
SkewnessKurtosis
Education8.772.407-5–130.334−1.003
Age of the diagnosis7.913.394-0–16−0.029−0.719
SAFA Anxiety51.25520.9160–1005–96−0.005−0.850
SAFA Depression58.35024.0270–1129–101−0.035−0.819
SAFA Obsession34.81720.8990–761–75−0.029−1.088
SAFA Psychogenic eating disorders27.96315.4400–602–54−0.110−1.177
SAFA Somatic symptoms and hypochondria23.30614.9130–500–50−0.004−0.164
TAS-20 Total score53.76611.90720–10022–82−0.096−0.362
TAS-20 Difficulty identifying feelings18.1976.8327–357–330.097−1.161
TAS-20 Difficulty describing feelings14.2705.2645–255–250.032−0.846
TAS-20 Externally-oriented thinking21.3504.4648–408–30−0.247−0.353
IUS-12 Total score35.6788.84012–6015–550.086−0.311
IUS-12 Prospective anxiety22.4165.3286–308–34−0.029−0.151
IUS-12 Inhibitory anxiety13.2625.1906–305–250.249−0.843
Table 7. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Anxiety.
Table 7. Differences among each factor of the TAS-20 and IUS-12 scales referring to SAFA Anxiety.
Q1 vs. Q2Q1 vs. Q3Q1 vs. Q4Q2 vs. Q3Q2 vs. Q4Q3 vs. Q4
TAS-20 Total score<0.001<0.0010.6820.066<0.0010.001
TAS-20 Difficulty identifying feelings<0.001<0.0010.8500.047<0.0010.004
TAS-20 Difficulty describing feelings<0.0010.0020.7910.043<0.0010.007
IUS-12 Total Score0.0250.0030.8800.5520.0520.013
IUS-12 Inhibitory anxiety0.0020.0090.5310.4330.0100.048
Quartiles’ scores
SAFA Anxiety
Q1 = 36Q2 = 51Q3 = 69
Significant p-value < 0.008 after Bonferroni’s correction. Bold values were significant values.
Table 12. The Pearson correlation coefficient (r) analysis.
Table 12. The Pearson correlation coefficient (r) analysis.
1.2.3.4.5.6.7.8.9.10.
1. Age-
2. Education0.977 **-
3. Age of the diagnosis0.216 *0.189 *-
4. TAS-20 Total score−0.178 *−0.214 *0.087-
5. TAS-20 Difficulty identifying feelings−0.037−0.0610.1610.824 **-
6. TAS-20 Difficulty describing feelings−0.009−0.0270.175 *0.811 **0.648 **-
7. TAS-20 Externally-oriented thinking−0.315 **−0.354 **−0.1270.396 **−0.002−0.051-
8. IUS-12 Total Score0.0780.051−0.0100.446 **0.450 **0.360 **0.137-
9. IUS-12 Prospective anxiety0.177 *0.156−0.0260.252 **0.309 **0.1550.0460.845 **-
10. IUS-12 Inhibitory anxiety−0.071−0.0730.0440.501 **0.450 **0.453 **0.0940.836 **0.412 **-
Significant p-value: * <0.05; ** <0.001.
Table 13. Multivariate linear regression analysis (Age, Gender and Education are selected predictors).
Table 13. Multivariate linear regression analysis (Age, Gender and Education are selected predictors).
TAS-20 Total ScoreTAS-20
Difficulty
Identifying Feelings
TAS-20
Difficulty
Describing Feelings
TAS-20
Externally-Oriented Thinking
IUS-20 Total ScoreIUS-12
Prospective Anxiety
IUS-12
Inhibitory
Anxiety
AgeB(CI: 95%)3.134
(−0.824/7.093)
914
(−1.393/3.221)
0.712
(−1.096/2.519)
1.471
(0.054/2.887)
2.365
(−0.654/5.384)
1.472
(−0.320/3.265)
0.893
(−0.877/2.662)
p0.1200.4350.4380.0420.1240.1070.320
VIF23.44523.44523.44523.44523.44523.44523.445
Tolerance0.0430.0430.0430.0430.0430.0430.043
GenderB(CI: 95%)1.653
(−2.614/5.921)
2.465
(−0.023/4.952)
0.583
(−1.366/2.532)
−0.875
(−2.402/0.652)
−0.278
(−3.533/2.977)
−1.261
(−3.194/0.672)
0.983
(−0.925/2.890)
p0.4450.0420.5550.2590.8660.1990.310
VIF1.0891.0891.0891.0891.0891.0891.089
Tolerance0.9180.9180.9180.9180.9180.9180.918
EducationB(CI: 95%)−4.183
(−8.105/−0.261)
−1.156
(−3.442/1.130)
−0.776
(−2.567/1.015)
−2.061
(−3.464/−0.658)
−2.113
(−5.104/0.879)
−1.047
(−2.823/0.729)
−1.065
(−2.818/0.687)
p0.0370.3190.3930.0040.1650.2460.231
VIF23.02523.02523.02523.02523.02523.02523.025
Tolerance0.0430.0430.0430.0430.0430.0430.043
F3.4481.9740.4408.1340.9302.3151.114
Durbin-Watson2.0881.9052.0371.8011.6941.6901.768
Significant p-value < 0.05; Degree of Freedom: Regression DF = 3, Residual DF = 133, Total DF = 136 for all performed models.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Merlo, E.M.; Tutino, R.; Myles, L.A.M.; Alibrandi, A.; Lia, M.C.; Minasi, D. Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus, Psychopathology, Uncertainty and Alexithymia: A Clinical and Differential Exploratory Study. Healthcare 2024, 12, 257. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020257

AMA Style

Merlo EM, Tutino R, Myles LAM, Alibrandi A, Lia MC, Minasi D. Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus, Psychopathology, Uncertainty and Alexithymia: A Clinical and Differential Exploratory Study. Healthcare. 2024; 12(2):257. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020257

Chicago/Turabian Style

Merlo, Emanuele Maria, Rita Tutino, Liam Alexander MacKenzie Myles, Angela Alibrandi, Maria Carmela Lia, and Domenico Minasi. 2024. "Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus, Psychopathology, Uncertainty and Alexithymia: A Clinical and Differential Exploratory Study" Healthcare 12, no. 2: 257. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020257

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop