Next Article in Journal
Interventions for Compassion Fatigue in Healthcare Providers—A Systematic Review of Randomised Controlled Trials
Next Article in Special Issue
Using Myofascial Therapy to Improve Psychological Outcomes, Quality of Life, and Sexual Function in Women with Chronic Pelvic Pain—A Case Series
Previous Article in Journal
The Role of Emotion Dysregulation in Understanding Suicide Risk: A Systematic Review of the Literature
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sexual Rewards and Costs in Heterosexual and Gay Hispanic Adults

by
Cristobal Calvillo
1,
Juan Carlos Sierra
2,
María del Mar Sánchez-Fuentes
2,*,† and
Reina Granados
3
1
Department of Health Behavior & Health Education, Fay W. Boozman College of Public Health, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, AR 72205, USA
2
Mind, Brain, and Behavior Research Center, University of Granada, 18011 Granada, Spain
3
Nursing Department, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Granada, 18071 Granada, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Current address: Faculty of Psychology, University of Granada, 18011 Granada, Spain.
Healthcare 2024, 12(2), 170; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020170
Submission received: 6 November 2023 / Revised: 6 January 2024 / Accepted: 8 January 2024 / Published: 11 January 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sexual Functioning, Sexual Satisfaction and Health)

Abstract

:
The Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction proposes that sexual satisfaction can be explained by the balance of sexual rewards or costs exchange. The Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction Questionnaire (IEMSSQ) was developed from this model. The IEMSSQ includes the Reward/Costs Checklist (RCC). The RCC assesses different sexual exchanges that are categorized into sexual rewards and/or costs. Analyses and comparisons of sexual rewards and costs in the Hispanic cisgender population based on gender (man or woman) and sexual orientation (heterosexual and homosexual) with this measure have not been conducted. The main goal was to analyze and rank the sexual rewards, costs, and both, indicated by a sample of 1996 Hispanic partnered participants (heterosexual men, gay men, heterosexual women, and lesbians). The predominant sexual exchanges that were reported involved emotional aspects, both as rewards and costs. When participants indicated that the exchange was both a reward and a cost, they were referring to aspects specifically related to the sexual relationship itself. Heterosexual men and women had greater rewards and higher costs, respectively. Gay men obtained a greater number of items reported as rewards and costs. The results provide further insight into sexual satisfaction related to gender and sexual orientation.

1. Introduction

Social Exchange Theories are based on the economic theories of rewards and costs. In this way, just as an economic exchange is motivated by profit, social exchange is influenced by the rewards that these relationships can bring [1,2,3]. The purpose of these theories is to predict and explain human behavior, considering the aspects that are positive (rewards) and negative (costs). According to these theories, individuals prefer reward exchanges to cost exchanges. People carry out a rewards–cost analysis, wherein they evaluate the pros and cons of their interpersonal relationships [4].
In the sexuality field, these types of exchange theories can be very useful in explaining sexual relations because they consider the influence of the interpersonal context [5]. Despite their relevance in the sexuality context, very few researchers have used these theories for their studies. However, some have considered that Social Exchange Theories explain traditional gender roles and sexual behaviors [6,7]. In this sense, sexual relations in heterosexual people can be understood as a context in which men acquire sex from women by offering resources in exchange. Therefore, for these societies, traditional gender roles are defined as if men were the acquires or receivers and women were the providers or contributors to these interactions, thus giving value to factors such as virginity or fidelity in the case of women but not in men [6,7]. Social Exchange Theories have also been applied to the study of the selection of partners [8], the beginning of activity and sexual rejection [9], extradyadic sexual behavior [10], and sexual coercion [11]. In addition, some authors have considered using Social Exchange Theories to develop specific theoretical models related to sexuality, such as the Equity Theory [12], the Investment Model [13,14], and the Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction (IEMSS) [15,16].
The IEMSS, developed from Social Exchange Theories [3], is one of the most outstanding models for the study of sexual satisfaction due to its consistent empirical support and its applicability in men and women [17,18,19,20,21,22]. According to this model, sexual exchanges are situations, behaviors, and/or thoughts that occur during sexual relations and can be considered positive or negative [15]. Positive exchanges (rewards) are those that are pleasurable or gratifying, while negative sexual exchanges (costs) are those that cause discomfort, pain, shame, or anxiety or require physical or mental effort [16]. Thus, it is understood that sexual satisfaction is the result of the general evaluation that individuals make of the exchanges experienced as sexual rewards and costs and not of specific exchanges. However, the evaluation of specific sexual exchanges can be useful for designing treatments and studying exchanges that are considered positive or negative for people. For example, Lawrance and Byers [16] found that the most frequently reported sexual rewards in a Canadian heterosexual sample were related to emotional and relational exchanges, as well as physical and behavioral exchanges, while more than 50% of the sample considered having sexual relations as a sexual cost when they or their partner were not in the mood. Another study carried out in a heterosexual Chinese population found no statistically significant differences between men and women in sexual exchanges that were considered rewards, but there were differences in exchanges perceived as costs. Men reported more physical and emotional exchanges as sexual costs compared to women [23]. Other research conducted in a gay Canadian population showed that women rated exchanges associated with emotional and relational aspects as rewards more frequently than men, while exchanges with physical and behavioral aspects were rated as sexual costs for these women [24]. The first study conducted in a heterosexual Hispanic population [20] found that men identified physical exchanges as rewards more frequently than women, whereas women identified these exchanges as sexual costs. However, coinciding with the study by Cohen et al. [24], women more frequently indicated exchanges that referred to emotional aspects as rewards [20]. Finally, it is worth mentioning two recent studies carried out on the Hispanic gay population, in which both studies revealed that gay men reported sexual intercourse as a cost more frequently than lesbians [25,26].
Given this, it can be stated that there are diverse results regarding sexual exchanges considered as rewards and costs depending on gender (i.e., men and women) and sexual orientation (i.e., heterosexual and homosexual). According to previous research [24], gender and culture are variables that can influence the consideration of the most prominent sexual exchanges by individuals. Likewise, sexual minorities may experience different types of exchanges, such as sexual rewards and costs, compared to heterosexual individuals. This can be attributed mainly to cultural aspects that provide little social legitimacy to same-sex relationships [27], as well as the presence of more similar gender socialization experiences in same-sex couples than in the case of opposite-sex couples. Additionally, same-sex couples tend to rely more on idiosyncratic expectations and goals for their relationships [28]. There are no previous investigations that have analyzed the most frequent sexual exchanges considered as rewards and costs indicated by Hispanic men and women who have sexual relations with people of the same sex and different sex. The present research aims to find out the most frequent rewards and costs based on gender and the type of relationship that individuals maintain. The specific objectives are: (a) to examine the sexual exchanges most frequently considered as rewards, costs and rewards and costs in a sample constituted by men and women who maintain a heterosexual couple relationship; (b) to analyze the sexual exchanges most frequently considered as rewards, costs and rewards and costs in a sample constituted by men and women who maintain a relationship with a person of the same sex; and (c) to examine if there are statistically significant differences in sexual exchanges among these four groups: heterosexual men, heterosexual women, gay men, and lesbians.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

For the present study, a non-probability convenience sampling was used, and a total of 1996 Hispanic participants were recruited (1042 cisgender men and 954 cisgender women). The sociodemographic characteristics of each subsample are displayed in Table 1. To participate in this study, individuals had to meet the following inclusion criteria at the evaluation time: (1) aged 18 years or older; (2) Spanish language as their mother tongue; (3) cisgender identity; (4) sexually active; and (5) involved in a steady relationship for at least a three-month duration with another cisgender person of the same or different sex.

2.2. Measures

The Sociodemographic and Sexual History questionnaire includes questions about gender, age, level of education, nationality, relationship status (not involved in a steady relationship or in a steady relationship), number of sexual partners, and age of first sexual relations.
The Kinsey Scale [29] was used to assess with whom the study participants have sexual relations. This scale uses eight response options for sexual practices, from “exclusively heterosexuals” (option 1) to “exclusively homosexuals” (option 7). An eighth option was included to account for asexuality. This study specifically included participants who chose either option 1 or option 7. Out of those who selected different response options (2 to 6 or 8), the gender distribution revealed 292 men and 342 women.
In terms of the rewards/costs checklist [30], we employed the Spanish adaptation [25,31]. This checklist comprises 58 items to assess different sexual exchange types. According to their current relationship, the participants value each item as a sexual reward, sexual cost, both (a reward and cost), or neither (no reward and no cost). For example, Exchange 12: “Oral sex: extent to which your partner stimulates you” could be considered a sexual reward if the person considers said behavior to be pleasant, or it could be considered a sexual cost if this behavior does not occur, and the person would like to receive oral sex from their partner. Likewise, it could be considered a reward and a cost if, for example, it is positive because the person enjoys it, but it is also negative at the same time because the person considers that it should be a more frequent practice. Lastly, this exchange may not be relevant to the person, so they may not respond. It is important to mention that some of the items were modified so that people who have a relationship with a person of the same sex could answer. For example, “Ability/inability to have a child” was replaced with “Ability/inability to have a child (either adopted or biological)” or “Method of protection (from sexually transmitted infections and/or pregnancy) used by you and your partner” to “Method of protection (from sexually transmitted infections) used by you and your partner”. In the present research, a sexual exchange was considered a reward if the participants indicated it as a reward, a cost if the participants indicated it as a cost, and a reward and a cost if the participants indicated the exchange as a reward and a cost. The rewards/costs checklist is not a measure of a latent construct but rather a list of sexual exchanges. Therefore, it lacks evidence of internal structure validity and internal consistency reliability.

2.3. Procedure

Instruments were administered in both the traditional pencil-and-paper format and the online format. The participants who completed the questionnaires in the paper-and-pencil format were contacted at educational and social centers and in public places. In addition, the gay participants were also contacted through lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender associations. The evaluator delivered and collected the questionnaires. After answering the scales, the participants returned them to the evaluators in a sealed envelope to guarantee maximum confidentiality. For the online version, the questionnaire battery was created using the Limesurvey™ software (Limesurvey GmbH, Hamburg, Germany) and distributed via virtual platforms (Facebook™, Twitter™, WhatsApp™, and e-mail). To access the questionnaire battery, the participants had to confirm a random sum. For both formats, all the participants voluntarily completed the questionnaires, and their anonymity and the confidentiality of their answers were always guaranteed. All those who decided to contribute to this study had to read an informed consent form and indicate their agreement to participate in the study by selecting the option confirming their understanding and willingness to take part. The estimated time to complete the questionnaires was 10 min.

2.4. Data Analyses

A comprehensive data analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics v. 29, a predictive analytics software, to examine the characteristics of the sample. Descriptive analyses were performed to assess the frequencies of exchanges categorized as rewards, costs, and rewards and costs, with the sample stratified by gender and sexual orientation. These frequencies were then ranked in descending order based on the observed percentage values within each group. Subsequent analyses focused on comparing rewards, costs, and rewards and costs among the four groups, building upon the ranking results. Custom tables were employed to facilitate this comparison, and a chi-square analysis was conducted to assess the differences between the groups for each category of exchange (reward, cost, and reward and cost). To address the potential increase in type I errors due to multiple testing, the Benjamini–Hochberg correction method was applied [32]. This correction method is deemed particularly effective when tests are either independent or only weakly dependent. In the specific case of the analysis of the RCC measure across several subgroups, it should be noted that these subgroups are not interrelated. Consequently, the results of one test are independent of the results of others within this context. Corresponding values and p-values for each category were obtained through a chi-square analysis. Subsequently, the obtained p-values were subjected to the Benjamini–Hochberg correction to assess specific group differences. For the calculation of the p-value threshold in conjunction with the Benjamini–Hochberg correction method, the analysis was conducted using SPSS v. 29 software, with the custom tables option selected and the Benjamini–Hochberg correction method chosen within the test statistics section. This approach for independent multiple comparisons resulted in a significance level set at 0.05. Additionally, the strength of association between variables was quantified using Cramer’s V coefficient [33]. To interpret the magnitude of associations, predefined ranges were considered, as outlined by Rea and Parker [34], providing a framework to evaluate the intensity of connections between variables.

3. Results

3.1. Sexual Rewards, Costs, and Rewards and Costs

The results indicate notable patterns in reported sexual exchanges as rewards across different subgroups. Among heterosexual men, the top five exchanges with the highest percentages were identified as follows: item 1 (96.6%), item 57 (95.2%), item 41 (95.1%), item 47 (95%), and item 32 (94.6%). Notably, item 1 pertains to the “Level of affection you and your partner express during sexual activities”. Conversely, among heterosexual women, the most prevalent exchanges were item 32 (94.6%), item 35 and item 14 (both at 94.4%), item 42 (93.4%), item 8 (93.2%), and item 45 (92.5%). The leading exchange (item 32) relates to “How comfortable you and your partner feel with each other”. In the case of gay men, the top five exchanges were item 32 (94.2%), item 14 (93.5%), item 1 (91.9%), item 42 (91.7%), and item 41 (91.3%). Remarkably, the item with the highest percentage among gay men aligned with the one reported by heterosexual women. Finally, within the lesbian subsample, the most prevalent exchanges were item 32 (96.7%), item 16 (95.2%), item 23 (95%), item 14 (94.7%), and items 29 and 35 (both at 94.4%). Appendix A includes the percentages of sexual exchanges as a sexual reward reported by the four subsamples.
Appendix B presents a detailed breakdown of the reported percentages of sexual exchanges as sexual costs within each subgroup. Among heterosexual men, the exchanges associated with the highest percentage values across all 58 items were item 27 (62.7%), item 26 (56%), item 5 (50%), item 44 (49.4%), and item 15 (49.2%). Notably, the exchange with the highest percentage (item 27) pertains to “Having sex when your partner is not in the mood”. Similarly, among heterosexual women, the costliest exchanges were item 26 (68.8%), item 27 (62.1%), item 15 (58%), item 34 (55.3%), and item 5 (54.4%). In this case, the primary costly exchange (item 26) refers to “Having sex when you’re not in the mood”. Comparatively, gay men identified the costliest items as item 27 (70.9%), item 26 (61.4%), item 21 (60.6%), item 15 (55.4%), and item 5 (53.4%). The exchange item with the highest percentage among gay men aligns with the one reported by heterosexual men, as mentioned previously. Moreover, in the lesbian subgroup, the most prevalent exchanges were 27 (63.8%), item 26 (61.6%), item 15 (60.9%), item 52 (53.8%), and item 30 (53.3%).
Additionally, considering the exchanges that were reported as a reward and cost (which is referred to as “reward and cost”), among heterosexual men, the highest percentages were associated with item 10 (13.6%), item 4 (12.5%), item 36 (11.7%), item 17 (10.5%), and item 9 (9.2%). Notably, item 10, which refers to “Frequency of sexual activities”, was the primary item selected as reward and cost. For heterosexual women, the highest percentages were reported for item 10 (13.1%), item 4 (12.6%), item 17 (9.9%), items 36 and 48 (9.6%), and item 6 (8.3%). Again, item 10 was the number one exchange selected. Among gay men, the most rewarding and costly items were item 58 (12.5%), item 17 (11.8%), item 51 (11.3%), item 4 (10.8%), and items 10 and 22 (10.5%). Markedly, item 58 was the highest exchange, and it refers to the “Extent to which you and your partner are sexually compatible” (i.e., well matched in terms of your sexual likes/dislikes). Finally, in the lesbian subgroup, the highest percentages were reported for item 10 (14.1%), item 17 (12.8%), item 4 (11.6%), item 6 (9.8%), and item 36 (9.4%). Item 10, once again, was the most selected in this subsample. Appendix C includes the percentages of sexual exchanges as a sexual reward and cost reported by the four subsamples.

3.2. Differences between Groups

Table 2 presents a comparative analysis encompassing four distinct groups, namely heterosexual men, heterosexual women, gay men, and lesbians. The examination focuses on the percentages associated with the 58 sexual exchanges in terms of sexual rewards, costs, and rewards and costs. Moreover, in relation to the magnitude of the associations, four exchanges presented moderate magnitudes, as established by Rea and Parker [34]. These were items 21, 30, 31, and 54. This suggests that in these items, there is some relationship between the variables, but it is not extremely strong. The rest of the exchanges showed lower magnitudes. Among these 58 sexual exchange items, statistical analysis reveals significant differences in percentage values across the four groups for 38 of them. Out of these 38 items, heterosexual men exhibit the highest percentage values in 15 instances related to sexual rewards, followed by lesbians with 12, heterosexual women with 8, and gay men with 2.
In terms of costs, among the 38 sexual exchanges exhibiting significant differences, heterosexual women recorded the highest percentage value in 10 items, followed by gay men with 8, and finally, both heterosexual men and lesbians with 6 each.
Finally, in the sexual exchanges categorized as rewards and costs, the group of gay men displays the highest percentage value in 10 items, while heterosexual women rank second with 3 items. Heterosexual men and lesbians each exhibit one item.

4. Discussion

According to the IEMSS, sexual satisfaction is an affective response resulting from the subjective evaluation of positive and negative aspects experienced during sexual intercourse [16]. Sexual relations in the couple context can present advantages and disadvantages for the people who participate in them. Therefore, this research aims to contribute to a better understanding of sexual satisfaction by assessing sexual exchanges as rewards (i.e., pleasant or positive), costs (i.e., negative or unpleasant), or rewards and costs in a Spanish-speaking adult sample. For this, a ranking was conducted of the positive (rewards), negative (costs), and positive and negative (rewards and costs) exchanges reported by men and women in a sexual relationship with people of different or the same sex. Likewise, possible differences in the 58 sexual exchanges, such as rewards and costs, depending on gender and sexual orientation, were examined.
In general, most reward exchanges in the four groups were generally related to emotional and physical aspects within the context of sexual and partner relationships. This could be the result of a dynamic interpersonal process based on intimacy. This intimacy within couple relationships can be characterized by openness (e.g., items 41 and 42), affection (e.g., item 1 and item 14), support and union and silent company (e.g., item 32), and sexual intimacy (e.g., item 29). Previous research has shown that intimacy is directly associated with sexual satisfaction [35,36,37] and with relationship satisfaction [38,39] in both heterosexual and gay populations. According to all this, intimacy seems to play an important role in sexual relationships and relationships regardless of people’s gender and/or sexual orientation.
More specifically, for rewards, it was found that the most beneficial exchange for gay men, heterosexual women, and lesbians was item 32 (“How comfortable you and your partner feel with each other”); this item also appears among the highest-scored in heterosexual men. This exchange refers to the level of comfortability that the person and his/her partner feel with each other. This reward does not seem to depend on the gender or sexual orientation of the respondents. In heterosexual men, item 1, referring to the level of affection that the person and their partner express during sexual activity, was considered the most rewarding exchange. This item was also one of the most valued by gay men (third place). However, although heterosexual and lesbian women rate this exchange among the ten most valued as rewards, it seems not to be as relevant for them. This may be due to socialization based on stereotyped gender roles that, in the case of women, unlike men, are more oriented towards the well-being of others [20,40,41]. In this sense, the level of affection achieved in a sexual relationship can be considered by the group of men as an achievement, while, in the case of women, there are rewards more related to the well-being of the couple, for example, their frequency of orgasms (item 8), being able to please her (item 45), and having fun with it (item 16).
In terms of costs, four groups agree on three exchanges of the costliest exchanges. These refer to having sex when you are not in the mood, having sex when your partner is not in the mood, and feeling physical discomfort/pain during/after sex (items 26, 27, and 15, respectively). The consideration of these exchanges as costs reveals social progress in the sexual field, in terms of sexual and reproductive rights, related to the freedom to decide on sexuality and the free exercise of it in a pleasant and safe way [42,43,44,45]. On the other hand, item 5, referring to the use of sex toys, appears as a cost in heterosexual women and in heterosexual and gay men, but to a lesser extent in lesbians. Considering the use of sex toys by lesbians as less expensive may be related to item 16 of fun with the partner chosen as the second most beneficial exchange by them. This result could also be because lesbians, compared to heterosexual women, use sex toys more frequently [46].
The four groups place four exchanges in the top five of the rewards and costs ranking (except for item 36, which ranks 7th in gay men). The content of these exchanges refers to the variety (item 4) and frequency (item 10) of sexual activities, which initiates the sexual activity (item 17), and the time dedicated to it (item 36). These aspects of sexual activity in the context of a couple would be valued positively, but in turn, the respondents would like them to be different (e.g., more/less diversity, more/less frequency, or more/less time spent). It should be noted that the most valued exchange as rewards and cost by gay men is item 58, related to the degree of sexual compatibility between the couple; that is if they coincide in terms of sexual likes/dislikes. In this line, this group also reports item 51, related to participation in sexual activities that are pleasurable but displeasing to the partner among the exchanges, most scored as rewards and cost. It seems that, in these couples, the compatibility between both members, despite being valued as a reward, sometimes does not meet the expectations created, so it would also be valued as a cost.
When comparing rewards and costs, the heterosexual men group reported the most rewards compared to the other groups, while heterosexual women reported more costs. As previously mentioned, gender socialization may be influencing, such that heterosexual women in this study might be putting their partner’s sexual needs before their own [47]. This result may be related to sexual indulgence. This term has been defined as consensual but unwanted sexual activity [48], which is carried out to fulfill the sexual desire of a sexually interested partner. According to Impett and Peplau [49], this complacency can be explained from three perspectives: gender, motivation, and relationship maintenance. From a gender perspective, sexual complacency may be due to an existing asymmetry of power in couples and is more frequent between men and women. Likewise, it should be noted that the women who support a double sexual standard present less autonomy, sexual assertiveness, and sexual satisfaction [50,51,52,53]. On the other hand, it is observed that gay men are the ones who report more exchanges as reward and cost compared to the rest of the groups. The content of these most scored exchanges refers to affection (item 1) and intimacy with the couple (item 9), fun in the sexual relationship (item 16), exclusivity (item 35), if they have broken stereotypes of gender (item 39) and couple compatibility, concern about sexually transmitted infections and partner attraction (items 58, 52, 57, respectively). These exchanges are considered by gay men as rewards, but at the same time, they are being evaluated as sexual costs. This may be due to a “dissatisfaction” based on the desire to be able to achieve greater satisfaction in these exchanges; that is, they are already rewards for them, but they would like to experience them in another way (e.g., more frequency, greater intensity, etc.). In this sense, according to a previous study [26], in gay men, the positive perception of sexual exchanges must be high enough for them to feel satisfied with their relationship. In the same way, it can happen with the valuation of sexual exchanges as sexual costs; perhaps this negative perception of sexual exchanges should be high enough to be considered a cost and that it affects sexual satisfaction. All this can also explain why these exchanges are valued as rewards and costs by these people at the same time.
These findings carry significant implications as they reveal patterns and differences in the occurrence of exchanges perceived as rewards, costs, or a combination of both among the analyzed groups. Given that this checklist is grounded in a well-established theoretical model of sexual satisfaction, we can identify and gain a deeper understanding of the aspects (represented by the list of exchanges) that positively or negatively influence sexual satisfaction according to the theoretical model. Furthermore, these findings can inform programs and approaches aimed at enhancing the quality of sexual experiences.
Finally, it is worth mentioning that, in terms of sociodemographic information, men across different age ranges and sexual orientations had a significantly higher mean age than women. This aligns with findings from a recent study by Muñoz-García et al. [54] involving a Hispanic population with varied sexual orientations. Concerning the number of sexual partners, men consistently reported a significantly higher count than women, irrespective of sexual orientation. This pattern could be linked with factors linked to sexual sensation seeking, as supported by prior research highlighting the relation between sexual sensation seeking and an increased number of sexual partners [55,56]. Moreover, existing studies suggest that heterosexual men are more likely to exhibit sexual sensation-seeking tendencies compared to their female counterparts [57,58,59], and individuals belonging to sexual minority groups demonstrate a heightened inclination toward sexual sensation-seeking in comparison to heterosexual individuals [58,59,60]. In light of those findings, it could be contended that the observed higher number of sexual partners among male participants in this study is intricately tied to their proclivity for seeking sensory sexual experiences. Lastly, concerning the level of education, significant gender disparities were identified solely within the heterosexual group. Despite a lower representation of women in the first two education levels, a higher proportion of women demonstrated higher education compared to men. That reflects evolving social norms, indicating a perceptible change in women’s attitudes towards higher education. Women now enjoy increased support and accessibility to educational pathways, overcoming previous limitations imposed by cultural or social norms in Hispanic countries.
The interplay of these characteristics could potentially impact the assessment of specific sexual exchanges, such as engaging in sexual activity when one’s partner is not in the mood or evaluating the level of affection during intimate moments. Given that engaging in sexual sensation seeking reflects a readiness to pursue novel and exhilarating experiences, potentially contributing to an augmented number of sexual partners, men (compared to women), particularly those of a certain age bracket reporting a higher number of sexual partners, might be inclined towards actively seeking diversity and freshness in their sexual encounters with their romantic partner to attain a specific level of affection, regardless of their sexual orientation. Conversely, in terms of perceived costs, exploring new experiences might be a drawback for men if they sense their partner is not in the mood. Nevertheless, a more in-depth investigation is warranted to comprehensively explore these constructs.
The present study is not without its limitations. First, the results cannot be generalized because intentional non-probabilistic sampling was used. In addition, as most of the participants had university studies, it would be interesting for future research to find out if the level of education is related in any way to the types of rewards and costs reported by both men and women. Another limitation is that there is no evidence available regarding the invariance of the scale used by gender and sexual orientation, so it is possible that it is not neutral. In addition, we did not compare whether there were differences between participants who completed the paper-and-pencil versus online questionnaires. Finally, future research with clinical samples is recommended (e.g., sexual dysfunction, general sexual dissatisfaction, and sexual dissatisfaction with a partner), in which the relationship of sexual exchanges identified as rewards and/or costs with other variables such as sexual attitudes attachment, or relationship dynamics are analyzed. We also propose that future research should consider the comparison level against previous sexual activity and would feature questions like “Compared to your past experiences with oral sex, how rewarding is oral sex now with your partner?”, and the comparison level for alternatives and might feature questions like “How likely would oral sex with other possible partners be better than it is with your current partner?”. These questions could also be classified by gender and sexual orientation. For example, “How likely is it that you would have more frequent sexual activity if your partner was of a different sexual orientation than your current partner?” or “How likely is it that you would have less frequent sexual activity if your partner was of a different sexual orientation?”.

5. Conclusions

This research extends knowledge on (1) one of the few theoretical models on sexual satisfaction that exists, (2) aspects related to sexual relations in the Spanish-speaking population, and (3) dimensions that contribute to sexual satisfaction in people with different sexual orientations. Although there are differences in the number of rewards and costs between the examined groups, all the participants generally reported similarities in the type of sexual rewards and costs. It should be noted that the emotional/interpersonal and physical aspects of sexual relations and the couple’s relationship appeared in the two most reported sexual exchanges by the four groups in both the form of rewards and costs. This reiterates that these aspects play a crucial role in sexual satisfaction. Finally, it was evident that the groups made up of heterosexual men and heterosexual women were those with the most rewards and costs, respectively.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.C.S.; methodology, C.C. and J.C.S.; formal analysis, C.C., J.C.S., M.d.M.S.-F. and R.G.; investigation, C.C., J.C.S., M.d.M.S.-F. and R.G.; writing—original draft preparation, C.C., J.C.S., M.d.M.S.-F. and R.G.; writing—review and editing, C.C., J.C.S., M.d.M.S.-F. and R.G.; supervision, J.C.S.; project administration, C.C., J.C.S. and M.d.M.S.-F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Granada (No. 110/CEIH/2015, 12 November 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A. Percentage of Heterosexual Men and Women, Gay Men, and Lesbians Who Report Each Sexual Exchange as a Sexual Reward

Sexual ExchangesHeterosexual menHeterosexual womenGay MenLesbians
RewardsRankRewardsRankRewardsRankRewardsRank
1. Level of affection you and your partner express during sexual activities96.6%192.2%791.9%393.5%8
2. Degree of emotional intimacy (feeling close, sharing feelings)92.5%990%1690.2%694.3%6
3. Extent to which you and your partner communicate about sex75.9%3582.1%2675.8%3079.3%29
4. Variety in sexual activities, locations, times57%4662.4%4459.8%4059.5%37
5. Extent to which you and your partner use sex toys43.8%5339.8%5341.6%4754.5%40
6. Sexual activities you and your partner engage in to arouse each other76.4%3377.6%3374.9%3179.3%29
7. How often you experience orgasm (climax)90.3%1478.7%3285.2%1682.5%27
8. How often your partner experiences orgasm (climax)80.7%2793.2%483.6%1987.3%21
9. Extent to which you and your partner engage in intimate activities (e.g., talking, cuddling) after sex83.9%2581.3%2882.7%2092.8%10
10. Frequency of sexual activities57.8%4463.9%4358.3%4157.3%38
11. How much privacy you and your partner have for sex80.1%2879%3185.3%1584.4%23
12. Oral sex: extent to which your partner stimulates you70.4%3972%3876%2976.3%32
13. Oral sex: extent to which you stimulate your partner73.1%3872.9%3780.9%2477.2%31
14. Physical sensations from touching, caressing, hugging90.5%1394.4%293.5%294.7%4
15. Feelings of physical discomfort or pain during/after sex47.6%5136.6%5440.1%4933.4%51
16. How much fun you and your partner experience during sexual interactions92.9%891.1%987.5%1395.2%2
17. Who initiates sexual activities67%4175.2%3667.1%3672.2%35
18. Extent to which you feel stressed/relaxed during sexual activities87.3%2283.4%2578%2782%28
19. Extent to which you and your partner express enjoyment about your sexual interactions91.2%1292.1%889%893.5%8
20. Extent to which you and your partner communicate your sexual likes and dislikes to each other78.2%3080.4%3074.3%3285.1%22
21. Ability/inability to conceive a child74.4%3668%4231.3%5143.9%47
22. Extent to which you and your partner engage in role-playing or act out fantasies55.9%4856%4552.6%4255.7%39
23. How you feel about yourself during/after engaging in sexual activities with your partner93.6%692.4%689%895%3
24. Extent to which your partner shows consideration for your wants/needs/feelings82%2684.6%2475.8%3089.4%17
25. How your partner treats you (verbally and physically) when you have sex91.5%1190.1%1588.4%993%9
26. Having sex when you’re not in the mood38.5%5524.9%5633.3%5034.7%50
27. Having sex when your partner is not in the mood30.8%5632.1%5524.4%5231.4%52
28. Extent to which you let your guard down with your partner93.1%790.4%1389.6%793.7%7
29. Extent to which your partner lets their guard down with you87.7%2090.5%1287.9%1294.4%5
30. Method of protection (from sexually transmitted infections and/or pregnancy) used by you and your partner77.4%3180.8%2960.9%3943.5%48
31. Extent to which you and your partner discuss and use protection (from sexually transmitted diseases and/or pregnancy)79.3%2981.4%2768.3%3545.9%44
32. How comfortable you and your partner feel with each other94.6%594.6%194.2%196.7%1
33. Extent to which/way in which your partner influences you to engage in sexual activity76.7%3276.4%3475.8%3083.1%25
34. Extent to which you and your partner argue after engaging in sexual activity51.5%4942.5%5148.5%4645.5%45
35. Extent to which you and your partner are/are not sexually exclusive (i.e., have sex only with each another)87.4%2194.4%271.6%3394.4%5
36. How much time you and your partner spend engaging in sexual activities60.8%4269.6%4162.6%3867.5%36
37. How easy it is for you to have an orgasm (climax)89.7%1575.5%3581.9%2173.7%34
38. How easy it is for your partner to have an orgasm (climax)76%3490.8%1179.4%2583%26
39. Extent to which your sexual relationship with your partner reflects or breaks down stereotypical gender roles (the way women and men are expected to behave sexually)76.4%3371.8%3966.3%3778.8%30
40. How your partner responds to your initiation of sexual activity73.4%3790.3%1477.5%2883.6%24
41. Being naked in front of your partner95.1%385.6%2391.3%589.6%16
42. Your partner being naked in front of you89.2%1793.4%391.7%492.4%13
43. Extent to which your partner talks to other people about your sex life56.3%4744.1%4949.9%4448.4%42
44. Extent to which you and your partner read/watch sexually explicit material (e.g., erotic stories, pornographic videos)43.2%5441.1%5248.8%4544.9%46
45. Pleasing/trying to please your partner sexually91.8%1092.5%587.1%1492.7%11
46. Extent to which sexual interactions with your partner make you feel secure in the relationship90.5%1389.5%1788.1%1089.7%15
47. Extent to which you get sexually aroused95%490.3%1487.1%1492.2%14
48. Amount of spontaneity in your sex life68.7%4069.9%4069%3474.8%33
49. Extent of control you feel during/after sexual activity89.3%1687%2188%1189.4%17
50. Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you dislike but your partner enjoys57.4%4543.5%5050.7%4355.7%39
51. Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you enjoy but your partner dislikes50.6%5044.2%4840.8%4847.9%43
52. Worry that you or your partner will obtain a sexually transmitted infection from each other57.9%4345.7%4748.5%4642%49
53. How confident you feel in terms of your ability to please your partner sexually88.3%1988.8%1983.9%1888.1%19
54. Extent to which you and your partner engage in anal sex/anal play47.4%5251.5%4681.6%2252%41
55. Your partner’s ability to please you sexually88.7%1888.5%2081.4%2387.6%20
56. Extent to which you think your partner is physically attracted to/sexually desires you87.1%2389.3%1884.9%1788.3%18
57. Extent to which you are physically attracted to/sexually desire your partner95.2%290.9%1084.9%1792.5%12
58. Extent to which you and your partner are sexually compatible (i.e., well matched in terms of your sexual likes/dislikes)85.9%2486.9%2279.4%2689.6%16

Appendix B. Percentage of Heterosexual Men and Women, Gay Men, and Lesbians Reporting Each Sexual Exchange as a Sexual Cost

Sexual ExchangesHeterosexual menHeterosexual WomenGay MenLesbians
CostsRankCostsRankCostsRankCostsRank
1. Level of affection you and your partner express during sexual activities1.9%574.4%494%552.2%49
2. Degree of emotional intimacy (feeling close, sharing feelings)4.4%516.6%404.2%542.8%48
3. Extent to which you and your partner communicate about sex17.5%2415.1%2516.3%2615.5%24
4. Variety in sexual activities, locations, times30.5%1325%1429.3%1528.9%16
5. Extent to which you and your partner use sex toys50%354.4%553.4%537.7%15
6. Sexual activities you and your partner engage in to arouse each other15%2914.1%2715.9%2811%28
7. How often you experience orgasm (climax)5.7%4513.2%308.5%3910%31
8. How often your partner experiences orgasm (climax)12.4%324%509.9%367.5%34
9. Extent to which you and your partner engage in intimate activities (e.g., talking, cuddling) after sex11.1%3312.6%3112%344.5%42
10. Frequency of sexual activities28.7%1423%1631.2%1428.5%17
11. How much privacy you and your partner have for sex14.9%3015.8%249.9%3610.8%29
12. Oral sex: extent to which your partner stimulates you23.7%1621.2%1717.1%2317.2%21
13. Oral sex: extent to which you stimulate your partner19.3%2220.8%1913.1%3216.3%22
14. Physical sensations from touching, caressing, hugging6%433.9%513.9%5625%18
15. Feelings of physical discomfort or pain during/after sex49.2%558%355.4%460.9%3
16. How much fun you and your partner experience during sexual interactions5.1%485.6%435.8%511.8%50
17. Who initiates sexual activities22.5%1714.9%2621.1%2015%25
18. Extent to which you feel stressed/relaxed during sexual activities8.6%3712.1%3215.4%2910.7%30
19. Extent to which you and your partner express enjoyment about your sexual interactions4.7%504.9%475.9%503.5%46
20. Extent to which you and your partner communicate your sexual likes and dislikes to each other15.4%2813.7%2917.8%229.6%32
21. Ability/inability to conceive a child19.6%2128.1%1360.6%349.4%8
22. Extent to which you and your partner engage in role-playing or act out fantasies38.1%1137.7%1237%1238%14
23. How you feel about yourself during/after engaging in sexual activities with your partner4.1%524.5%484.5%532.8%48
24. Extent to which your partner shows consideration for your wants/needs/feelings12.5%319.7%3516.1%275.5%40
25. How your partner treats you (verbally and physically) when you have sex5.5%467.3%377.1%463.8%45
26. Having sex when you’re not in the mood56%268.8%161.4%261.6%2
27. Having sex when your partner is not in the mood62.7%162.1%270.9%163.8%1
28. Extent to which you let your guard down with your partner5.3%477.6%366.3%493.8%45
29. Extent to which your partner lets their guard down with you9.3%347.3%378%423.8%45
30. Method of protection (from sexually transmitted infections and/or pregnancy) used by you and your partner17.3%2513.8%2832.7%1353.3%5
31. Extent to which you and your partner discuss and use protection (from sexually transmitted diseases and/or pregnancy)16.6%2716.3%2327.1%1750.5%7
32. How comfortable you and your partner feel with each another3.9%533.1%523%572.8%48
33. Extent to which/way in which your partner influences you to engage in sexual activity17.9%2318%2216.4%2413.5%26
34. Extent to which you and your partner argue after engaging in sexual activity45.5%755.3%447%751%6
35. Extent to which you and your partner are/are not sexually exclusive (i.e., have sex only with each other)7.7%394.5%4819.8%213.1%47
36. How much time you and your partner spend engaging in sexual activities27.4%1520.9%1827.7%1623.1%19
37. How easy it is for you to have an orgasm (climax)7.6%4019.2%2112.8%3320.3%20
38. How easy it is for your partner to have an orgasm (climax)17.1%265.2%4514.1%3012.7%27
39. Extent to which your sexual relationship with your partner reflects or breaks down stereotypical gender roles (the way women and men are expected to behave sexually)20.2%2023.3%1523.4%1813.5%26
40. How your partner responds to your initiation of sexual activity20.3%196%4216.4%2510.8%29
41. Being naked in front of your partner3.8%5410.9%335.7%527.1%36
42. Your partner being naked in front of you8.8%355%466.5%485.8%38
43. Extent to which your partner talks to other people about your sex life39.9%950.5%842.5%1145.2%11
44. Extent to which you and your partner read/watch sexually explicit material (e.g., erotic stories, pornographic videos)49.4%453.9%644.1%947.2%9
45. Pleasing/trying to please your partner sexually4.9%495%466.7%474.1%44
46. Extent to which sexual interactions with your partner make you feel secure in the relationship5.9%446.6%407.2%455.5%40
47. Extent to which you get sexually aroused3.1%556.5%417.4%435%41
48. Amount of spontaneity in your sex life22.1%1820.5%2022.8%1916%23
49. Extent of control you feel during/after sexual activity8.5%389.8%348.8%386.8%37
50. Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you dislike but your partner enjoys36.7%1248.9%1043.8%1042.5%12
51. Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you enjoy but your partner dislikes42.4%849.7%947.9%645.9%10
52. Worry that you or your partner will obtain a sexually transmitted infection from each other38.7%1051.9%744.7%853.8%4
53. How confident you feel in terms of your ability to please your partner sexually5.7%457.1%389.6%377.6%33
54. Extent to which you and your partner engage in anal sex/anal play45.7%643.4%1113.2%3140.8%13
55. Your partner’s ability to please you sexually7%425.6%4311.9%357.4%35
56. Extent to which you think your partner is physically attracted to/sexually desires you8.8%366.9%398.4%405.7%39
57. Extent to which you are physically attracted to/sexually desire your partner2.7%565.5%447.3%444.3%43
58. Extent to which you and your partner are sexually compatible (i.e., well matched in terms of your sexual likes/dislikes)7.1%416.6%408.1%415%41

Appendix C. Percentage of Heterosexual Men and Women, Gay Men, and Lesbians, Reporting Each Sexual Exchange as a Sexual Reward and Cost

Sexual ExchangesHeterosexual MenHeterosexual WomenGay MenLesbians
Rewards and CostsRankRewards and CostsRankRewards and CostsRankRewards and CostsRank
1. Level of affection you and your partner express during sexual activities1.5%413.3%324.2%364.2%28
2. Degree of emotional intimacy (feeling close, sharing feelings)3.1%333.3%325.6%253%33
3. Extent to which you and your partner communicate about sex6.6%122.9%357.9%125.2%23
4. Variety in sexual activities, locations, times12.5%212.6%210.8%411.6%3
5. Extent to which you and your partner use sex toys6.3%155.8%155%317.9%7
6. Sexual activities you and your partner engage in to arouse each other8.6%68.3%59.2%89.8%4
7. How often you experience orgasm (climax)4%278.1%66.3%227.5%9
8. How often your partner experiences orgasm (climax)6.9%112.8%366.5%205.3%22
9. Extent to which you and your partner engage in intimate activities (e.g., talking, cuddling) after sex5%236.1%125.2%292.7%35
10. Frequency of sexual activities13.6%113.1%110.5%514.1%1
11. How much privacy you and your partner have for sex5%235.3%204.8%324.8%26
12. Oral sex: extent to which your partner stimulates you6%176.8%86.9%166.6%13
13. Oral sex: extent to which you stimulate your partner7.6%76.4%106%246.5%14
14. Physical sensations from touching, caressing, hugging3.5%301.7%432.7%413.3%32
15. Feelings of physical discomfort or pain during/after sex3.2%325.4%194.5%345.6%20
16. How much fun you and your partner experience during sexual interactions2%383.3%326.7%183%33
17. Who initiates sexual activities10.5%49.9%311.8%212.8%2
18. Extent to which you feel stressed/relaxed during sexual activities4.1%264.5%246.6%197.3%10
19. Extent to which you and your partner express enjoyment about your sexual interactions4.1%263%345.1%303%33
20. Extent to which you and your partner communicate your sexual likes and dislikes to each other6.4%145.9%147.9%125.3%22
21. Ability/inability to conceive a child5.9%183.9%268.1%116.7%12
22. Extent to which you and your partner engage in role-playing or act out fantasies6%176.3%1110.5%56.3%16
23. How you feel about yourself during/after engaging in sexual activities with your partner2.3%363%346.5%202.3%37
24. Extent to which your partner shows consideration for your wants/needs/feelings5.5%205.7%168.1%115%25
25. How your partner treats you (verbally and physically) when you have sex3%342.7%374.5%343.3%32
26. Having sex when you’re not in the mood5.5%206.3%115.3%283.7%30
27. Having sex when your partner is not in the mood6.5%135.8%154.7%334.8%26
28. Extent to which you let your guard down with your partner1.6%401.9%424.1%372.5%36
29. Extent to which your partner lets their guard down with you3%342.2%414.1%371.8%38
30. Method of protection (from sexually transmitted infections and/or pregnancy) used by you and your partner5.3%225.3%206.4%213.3%32
31. Extent to which you and your partner discuss and use protection (from sexually transmitted diseases and/or pregnancy)4.1%262.3%404.7%333.6%31
32. How comfortable you and your partner feel with each another1.5%412.2%412.8%400.5%39
33. Extent to which/way in which your partner influences you to engage in sexual activity5.4%215.6%177.8%133.3%32
34. Extent to which you and your partner argue after engaging in sexual activity3%342.2%414.4%353.6%31
35. Extent to which you and your partner are/are not sexually exclusive (i.e., have sex only with each other)4.9%241.2%458.5%92.5%36
36. How much time you and your partner spend engaging in sexual activities11.7%39.6%49.7%79.4%5
37. How easy it is for you to have an orgasm (climax)2.7%355.4%195.3%286.1%18
38. How easy it is for your partner to have an orgasm (climax)6.9%114%256.5%204.3%27
39. Extent to which your sexual relationship with your partner reflects or breaks down stereotypical gender roles (the way women and men are expected to behave sexually)3.3%324.9%2310.3%67.7%8
40. How your partner responds to your initiation of sexual activity6.3%153.7%296.1%235.5%21
41. Being naked in front of your partner1.2%423.4%313%393.3%32
42. Your partner being naked in front of you2%381.5%441.8%421.8%38
43. Extent to which your partner talks to other people about your sex life3.8%285.5%187.7%146.4%15
44. Extent to which you and your partner read/watch sexually explicit material (e.g., erotic stories, pornographic videos)7.4%85%227.2%157.9%7
45. Pleasing/trying to please your partner sexually3.3%322.5%386.3%223.3%32
46. Extent to which sexual interactions with your partner make you feel secure in the relationship3.6%293.9%274.7%334.8%26
47. Extent to which you get sexually aroused1.9%393.2%335.4%272.8%34
48. Amount of spontaneity in your sex life9.2%59.6%48.3%109.2%6
49. Extent of control you feel during/after sexual activity2.3%363.2%333.2%383.8%29
50. Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you dislike but your partner enjoys5.8%197.6%75.5%261.8%38
51. Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you enjoy but your partner dislikes7%106.1%1211.3%36.2%17
52. Worry that you or your partner will obtain a sexually transmitted infection from each other3.4%312.4%396.8%174.3%27
53. How confident you feel in terms of your ability to please your partner sexually6.1%164%256.5%204.3%27
54. Extent to which you and your partner engage in anal sex/anal play7%105.1%215.3%287.2%11
55. Your partner’s ability to please you sexually4.3%256%136.7%185.1%24
56. Extent to which you think your partner is physically attracted to/sexually desires you4.1%263.8%286.6%196%19
57. Extent to which you are physically attracted to/sexually desire your partner2.1%373.6%307.7%143.3%32
58. Extent to which you and your partner are sexually compatible (i.e., well matched in terms of your sexual likes/dislikes)7.1%96.5%912.5%15.5%21

References

  1. Blau, P.N. Exchange and Power in Social Life; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1964. [Google Scholar]
  2. Homans, G.C. Social Behavior. Its Elementary Forms; Harcourt, Brace & World: New York, NY, USA, 1961. [Google Scholar]
  3. Thibaut, J.W.; Kelley, H.H. The Social Psychology of Groups; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1959. [Google Scholar]
  4. Stafford, L. Social exchange theories. Calculating the rewards and costs of personal relationships. In Engaging Theories in Interpersonal Communication: Multiple Perspectives; Baxter, L.A., Braithwaite, D.O., Eds.; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008; pp. 377–389. [Google Scholar]
  5. Byers, E.S.; Wang, A. Understanding sexuality in close relationships from the social exchange perspective. In The Handbook of Sexuality in Close Relationships; Harvey, J.H., Wenzel, A., Sprecher, S., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2004; pp. 203–234. [Google Scholar]
  6. Baumeister, R.F.; Vohs, K.D. Sexual economics: Sex as female resource for social exchange in heterosexual interactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 8, 339–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Baumeister, R.F.; Vohs, K.D. Sexual economics, culture, men, and modern sexual trends. Society 2012, 49, 520–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kenrick, D.T.; Groth, G.E.; Trost, M.R.; Sadalla, E.K. Integrating evolutionary and social exchange perspectives on relationships: Effects of gender, self-appraisal, and involvement level on mate selection criteria. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 64, 951–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Sprecher, S. Social exchange theories and sexuality. J. Sex. Res. 1998, 35, 32–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Walster, E.; Traupmann, J.; Walster, G.W. Equity and extramarital sexuality. Arch. Sex. Behav. 1978, 7, 127–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. He, S.; Tsang, S. Male partners’ investment and alternatives as correlates of women’s coerced first sexual intercourse in Chinese dating relationships: A social exchange perspective. J. Interpers. Violence 2019, 34, 182–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Walster, E.; Walster, G.W.; Berscheid, E. Equity: Theory and Research; Allyn and Bacon: Boston, MA, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  13. Rusbult, C.E. Commitment and satisfaction in romantic associations: A test of the Investment Model. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 16, 172–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rusbult, C.E. A longitudinal test of the Investment Model: The development (and deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 45, 101–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lawrance, K.A.; Byers, E.S. Development of the interpersonal exchange model of sexual satisfaction in long-term relationships. Can. J. Hum. Sex. 1992, 1, 123–128. [Google Scholar]
  16. Lawrance, K.A.; Byers, E.S. Sexual satisfaction in long-term heterosexual relationships: The Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction. Pers. Relatsh. 1995, 2, 267–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Byers, E.S.; Rehman, U.S. Sexual well-being. In APA, Handbook of Sexuality and Psychology; Tolman, L.D., Hamilton, L.M., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2014; pp. 317–337. [Google Scholar]
  18. Péloquin, K.; Byers, E.S.; Callaci, M.; Tremblay, N. Sexual portrait of couples seeking relationship therapy. J. Marital. Fam. Ther. 2019, 45, 120–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Rausch, D.; Rettenberger, M. Predictors of sexual satisfaction in women: A systematic review. Sex. Med. Rev. 2021, 9, 365–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Santos-Iglesias, P. Sexual satisfaction in Spanish heterosexual couples: Testing the interpersonal exchange model of sexual satisfaction. J. Sex. Marital. Ther. 2016, 42, 223–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Santos-Iglesias, P.; Byers, E.S. Sexual satisfaction of older adults: Testing the Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction in the ageing population. Ageing Soc. 2021, 43, 180–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Yucel, D.; Gassanov, M.A. Exploring actor and partner correlates of sexual satisfaction among married couples. Soc. Sci. Res. 2010, 39, 725–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Renaud, C.A.; Byers, E.S.; Pan, S. Sexual and relationship satisfaction in mainland China. J. Sex. Res. 1997, 34, 399–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Cohen, J.N.; Byers, E.S.; Walsh, L.P. Factors influencing the sexual relationships of lesbian and gay men. Int. J. Sex. Health 2008, 20, 162–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Calvillo, C.; Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Parrón-Carreño, T.; Sierra, J.C. Validation of the Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction Questionnaire in adults with a same-sex partner. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2020, 20, 140–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Calvillo, C.; Sánchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Sierra, J.C. An explanatory model of sexual satisfaction in adults with a same-sex partner: An analysis based on gender differences. Int. J. Environ. Re. Public Health 2020, 17, 3393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. James, S.E.; Murphy, B.C. Gay and lesbian relationships in a changing social context. In Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identities in Families: Psychological Perspectives; Patterson, C.J., D’Augelli, A.R., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 99–121. [Google Scholar]
  28. Peplau, L.A.; Cochran, S.D. A relational perspective on homosexuality. In Homosexuality/Heterosexuality: Concepts of Sexual Orientation; McWhirter, D.P., Sanders, S.A., Reinisch, J.M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990; pp. 321–349. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kinsey, A.C.; Pomeroy, W.B.; Martin, C.E. Sexual Behavior in the Human Male; Indiana University Press: Bloomington, IN, USA, 1998; Original Published in 1948. [Google Scholar]
  30. Lawrance, K.; Byers, E.S.; Cohen, J.N. Interpersonal exchange model of sexual satisfaction questionnaire. In Handbook of Sexuality-Related Measures, 3rd ed.; Fisher, T.D., Davis, C.M., Yarber, W.L., Davis, S.L., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 525–530. [Google Scholar]
  31. Sanchez-Fuentes, M.M.; Santos-Iglesias, P.; Byers, E.S.; Sierra, J.C. Validation of the interpersonal exchange model of sexual satisfaction questionnaire in a Spanish sample. J. Sex. Res. 2015, 52, 1028–1041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Stat Methodol. 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Cramér, H. Mathematical Methods of Statistics; Princeton Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1946. [Google Scholar]
  34. Rea, L.M.; Parker, R.A. Designing and Conducting Survey Research; Jossey-Boss: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  35. Cruz, R.V.; Caringal-Go, J.F. Filipinos behind closed doors: Nonsexual and sexual constructs as predictors of sexual and relationship satisfaction in Filipino individuals. Sex. Cult. 2021, 25, 807–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Haning, R.V.; O’Keefe, S.L.; Randall, E.J.; Kommor, M.J.; Baker, E.; Wilson, R. Intimacy, orgasm likelihood, and conflict predict sexual satisfaction in heterosexual male and female respondents. J. Sex. Marital. Ther. 2007, 33, 93–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Totenhagen, C.J.; Butler, E.A.; Ridley, C.A. Daily stress, closeness, and satisfaction in gay and lesbian couples. Pers. Relatsh. 2012, 19, 219–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Deenen, A.A.; Gijs, L.; Van Naerssen, A.X. Intimacy and sexuality in gay male couples. Arch. Sex. Behav. 1994, 23, 421–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Yoo, H.; Bartle-Haring, S.; Day, R.D.; Gangamma, R. Couple communication, emotional and sexual intimacy, and relationship satisfaction. J. Sex. Marital Ther. 2014, 40, 275–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Abele, A.E.; Hauke, N.; Peters, K.; Louvet, E.; Szymkow, A.; Duan, Y.P. Facets of the Fundamental Content Dimensions: Agency with Competence and Assertiveness—Communion with Warmth and Morality. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Sczesny, S.; Nater, C.; Eagly, A.H. Agency and Communion. Their implications for gender stereotypes and gender identities. In Agency and Communion in Social Psychology; Abele, A., Wojciszke, B., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 103–116. [Google Scholar]
  42. United Nations. Sexual and Reproductive Rights; High Commissioner for Human Rights, United Nations: Cairo, Egypt, 1994; Available online: https://hchr.org.mx/historias-destacadas/derechos-sexuales-y-reproductivos-2/#:~:text=%3E%20Decidir%20libremente%20respecto%20de%20la,y%20reproductiva%3B%20interrupci%C3%B3n%20del%20embarazo (accessed on 12 July 2023).
  43. United Nations. Programme of Action; United Nations Population Fund: New York, NY, USA, 2014; Available online: https://www.unfpa.org/es/publications/programa-de-accion-de-la-conferencia-internacional-sobre-la-poblacion-y-el-desarrollo (accessed on 12 July 2023).
  44. World Association for Sexual Health. Declaration of Sexual Rights; Advisory Council and General of WAS: Singapore, 2015; Original Proclamation Valencia, Spain, 1997; Available online: https://worldsexualhealth.net/wp-content/uploads/2021/09/declaracion_derechos_sexuales_sep03_2014_b.pdf (accessed on 12 July 2023).
  45. World Association for Sexual Health. Declaration of Sexual Pleasure; WAS: Rome, Italy, 2021; Available online: https://worldsexualhealth.net/wp-content/uploads/2021/09/WAS-DECLARACIO%CC%81NN-PLACER-SEXUAL-2021-.pdf (accessed on 12 July 2023).
  46. Frederick, D.A.; Gillespie, B.J.; Lever, J.; Berardi, V.; Garcia, J.R. Debunking lesbian bed death: Using coarsened exact matching to compare sexual practices and satisfaction of lesbian and heterosexual women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2021, 50, 3601–3619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Sierra, J.C.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; Arcos-Romero, A.I.; Calvillo, C.; Granados, R. Study of sexual concordance in men and women with different typologies of adherence to the sexual double standard. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2022, 22, 100297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rubinsky, V. Sexual compliance in understudied relationships. Commun. Stud. 2020, 71, 879–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Impett, E.A.; Peplau, L.A. Sexual compliance: Gender, motivational, and relationship perspectives. J. Sex. Res. 2003, 40, 87–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; Sierra, J.C. Typologies of sexual double standard adherence in Spanish population. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Leg. Context 2021, 13, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Danube, C.L.; Norris, J.; Stappenbeck, C.A.; Davis, K.C.; George, W.H.; Zawacki, T.; Morrison, D.M.; Abdallah, D.A. Partner type, sexual double standard endorsement, and ambivalence predict abdication and unprotected sex intentions in a community sample of young women. J. Sex. Res. 2016, 53, 601–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Kim, Y.; Lee, E.; Lee, H. Sexual double standard, dating violence recognition, and sexual assertiveness among university students in South Korea. Asian Nurs. Res. 2019, 13, 47–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Álvarez-Muelas, A.; Mateo, V.; Sierra, J.C. Comparison of the components of the Interpersonal Exchange Model of Sexual Satisfaction between different typologies of adherence to the sexual double standard. Rev. Iberoroam. Psychol. Salud 2023, 14, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Muñoz-García, L.E.; Gómez-Berrocal, C.; Sierra, J.C. Evaluating the subjective orgasm experience through sexual context, gender, and sexual orientation. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2023, 52, 1479–1491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Bancroft, J.; Janssen, E.; Strong, D.; Carnes, L.; Vukadinovic, Z.; Long, S. Sexual risk-taking in gay men: The relevance of sexual arousability, mood, and sensation seeking. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2003, 32, 555–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Sales, J.M.; Smearman, E.L.; Brody, G.H.; Milhausen, R.; Philibert, R.A.; DiClemente, R.J. Factors associated with sexual arousal, sexual sensation seeking and sexual satisfaction among female African American adolescents. Sex. Health 2013, 10, 512–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Burri, A. Sexual sensation seeking, sexual compulsivity, and gender identity and its relationship with sexual functioning in a population sample of men and women. J. Sex. Med. 2017, 14, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gil-Llario, M.D.; Morell-Mengual, V.; Giménez-García, C.; Salmerón-Sánchez, P.; Ballester-Arnal, R. Sexual sensation seeking: A validated scale for Spanish gay, lesbian and bisexual people. AIDS Behav. 2018, 22, 3525–3534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Thorpe, S.; Ware, S.; Tanner, A.E.; Kugler, K.C.; Guastaferro, K.; Milroy, J.J.; Wyrick, D.L. Sexual sensation seeking, hookups, and alcohol consumption among first-year college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2021, 69, 897–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. McCoul, M.D.; Haslam, N. Predicting high risk sexual behaviour in heterosexual and homosexual men: The roles of impulsivity and sensation seeking. Pers Individ. Dif. 2001, 31, 1303–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. The participants’ sociodemographic characteristics (N = 1996).
Table 1. The participants’ sociodemographic characteristics (N = 1996).
VariablesHeterosexual Homosexual
Men
n = 538
Women
n = 550
Men
n = 504
Women
n = 404
Age rank (years)18–7418–74 18–5818–58
M (SD)M (SD)tM (SD)M (SD)t
Age (years)34.52 (12.78)31.10 (11.85)4.57 ***31.05 (9.35)28.67 (8.40)3.98 ***
Number of sexual partners6.04 (9.56)3.80 (4.90)4.87 ***39.18 (82.97)7.45 (9.44)7.64 ***
First sexual relation (years)17.69 (2.86)17.92 (2.94)−1.3017.14 (3.99)17.22 (2.99)−0.33
Level of education
No studies
n (%)
8 (1.5)
n (%)
4 (0.7)
χ2n (%)
0
n (%)
0
χ2
Primary education63 (11.8)48 (8.7)12.36 ***7 (1.4)8 (2)0.71
Secondary education181 (34)152 (27.6)107 (21.2)90 (22.2)
Tertiary education281 (52.7)346 (62.9)390 (77.4)306 (75.7)
Note. M: mean; SD: standard deviation. *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Comparison of percentages between heterosexual men, women, gay men, and lesbians on sexual rewards, costs, and rewards and costs.
Table 2. Comparison of percentages between heterosexual men, women, gay men, and lesbians on sexual rewards, costs, and rewards and costs.
Sexual ExchangesHeterosexual Men (A)Heterosexual Women (B)Gay Men (C)Lesbians (D)X2pV
  • Level of affection you and your partner express during sexual activities
Reward96.6% B 1, C92.2%91.9%93.5%15.430.0170.063
Cost1.9%4.4%4%2.2%
Rewards and Costs1.5%3.3%4.2% A4.2% A
2.
Degree of emotional intimacy (feeling close, sharing feelings)
Reward92.5%90%90.2%94.3%14.630.0230.061
Cost4.4%6.6% D4.2%2.8%
Rewards and Costs3.1%3.3%5.6%3%
3.
Extent to which you and your partner communicate about sex
Reward75.9%82.1% A, C75.8%79.3%15.310.0180.063
Cost17.5%15.1%16.3%15.5%
Rewards and Costs6.6% B2.9%7.9% B5.2%
4.
Variety in sexual activities, locations, times
Reward57%62.4%59.8%59.5%5.130.5260.037
Cost30.5%25%29.3%28.9%
Rewards and Costs12.5%12.6%10.8%11.6%
5.
Extent to which you and your partner use sex toys
Reward43.8%39.8%41.6%54.5% A, B, C27.91<0.0010.092
Cost50% D54.4% D53.4% D37.7%
Rewards and Costs6.3%5.8%5%7.9%
6.
Sexual activities you and your partner engage in to arouse each other
Reward76.4%77.6%74.9%79.3%5.380.4960.037
Cost15%14.1%15.9%11%
Rewards and Costs8.6%8.3%9.2%9.8%
7.
How often you experience orgasm (climax)
Reward90.3% B, C, D78.7%85.2% C82.5%28.62<0.0010.086
Cost5.7%13.2% A, C8.5%10% A
Rewards and Costs4%8.1% A6.3%7.5%
8.
How often your partner experiences orgasm (climax)
Reward80.7%93.2% A, C, D83.6%87.3% A38.66<0.0010.100
Cost12.4% B, D4%9.9% B7.5% B
Rewards and Costs6.9% B2.8%6.5% B5.3%
9.
Extent to which you and your partner engage in intimate activities (e.g., talking, cuddling) after sex
Reward83.9%81.3%82.7%92.8% A, B, C27.25<0.0010.084
Cost11.1% D12.6% D12% D4.5%
Rewards and Costs5%6.1%5.2%2.7%
10.
Frequency of sexual activities
Reward57.8%63.9%58.3%57.3%11.970.0620.056
Cost28.7%23%31.2%28.5%
Rewards and Costs13.6%13.1%10.5%14.1%
11.
How much privacy you and your partner have for sex
Reward80.1%79%85.3%84.4%11.580.0720.055
Cost14.9%15.8%9.9%10.8%
Rewards and Costs5%5.3%4.8%4.8%
12.
Oral sex: extent to which your partner stimulates you
Reward70.4%72%76%76.3%9.430.1510.050
Cost23.7%21.2%17.1%17.2%
Rewards and Costs6%6.8%6.9%6.6%
13.
Oral sex: extent to which you stimulate your partner
Reward73.1%72.9%80.9% A, B77.2%13.890.0310.061
Cost19.3% C20.8% C13.1%16.3%
Rewards and Costs7.6%6.4%6%6.5%
14.
Physical sensations from touching, caressing, hugging
Reward90.5%94.4% A93.5%94.7% A13.340.0380.059
Cost6% D3.9%3.9%25%
Rewards and Costs3.5%1.7%2.7%3.3%
15.
Feelings of physical discomfort or pain during/after sex
Reward47.6% B, D36.6%40.1%33.4%17.510.0080.076
Cost49.2%58% A55.4%60.9% A
Rewards and Costs3.2%5.4%4.5%5.6%
16.
How much fun you and your partner experience during sexual interactions
Reward92.9% C91.1%87.5%95.2% B, C27.62<0.0010.085
Cost5.1% D5.6% D5.8% D1.8%
Rewards and Costs2%3.3%6.7% A, B, D3%
17.
Who initiates sexual activities
Reward67%75.2% A, C67.1%72.2%16.030.0140.068
Cost22.5% B, D14.9%21.1% B, D15%
Rewards and Costs10.5%9.9%11.8%12.8%
18.
Extent to which you feel stressed/relaxed during sexual activities
Reward87.3% C83.4%78%82%18.270.0060.071
Cost8.6%12.1%15.4% A10.7%
Rewards and Costs4.1%4.5%6.6%7.3%
19.
Extent to which you and your partner express enjoyment about your sexual interactions
Reward91.2%92.1%89%93.5%6.890.3300.042
Cost4.7%4.9%5.9%3.5%
Rewards and Costs4.1%3%5.1%3%
20.
Extent to which you and your partner communicate your sexual likes and dislikes to each other
Reward78.2%80.4% C74.3%85.1% A, C16.830.0100.066
Cost15.4% D13.7%17.8% D9.6%
Rewards and Costs6.4%5.9%7.9%5.3%
21.
Ability/inability to conceive a child
Reward74.4% C, D68% C, D31.3%43.9% C199.98<0.0010.259
Cost19.6%28.1% A60.6% A, B, D49.4% A, B
Rewards and Costs5.9%3.9%8.1%6.7%
22.
Extent to which you and your partner engage in role-playing or act out fantasies
Reward55.9%56%52.6%55.7%9.580.1430.052
Cost38.1%37.7%37%38%
Rewards and Costs6%6.3%10.5%6.3%
23.
How you feel about yourself during/after engaging in sexual activities with your partner
Reward93.6% C92.4%89%95% C19.830.0030.072
Cost4.1%4.5%4.5%2.8%
Rewards and Costs2.3%3%6.5% A, B, D2.3%
24.
Extent to which your partner shows consideration for your wants/needs/feelings
Reward82% C84.6% C75.8%89.4% A, B, C33.68<0.0010.093
Cost12.5% D9.7% D16.1% B, D5.5%
Rewards and Costs5.5%5.7%8.1%5%
25.
How your partner treats you (verbally and physically) when you have sex
Reward91.5%90.1%88.4%93%9.280.1580.049
Cost5.5%7.3%7.1%3.8%
Rewards and Costs3%2.7%4.5%3.3%
26.
Having sex when you’re not in the mood
Reward38.5% B24.9%33.3% B34.7% B21.410.0020.080
Cost56%68.8% A61.4%61.6%
Rewards and Costs5.5%6.3%5.3%3.7%
27.
Having sex when your partner is not in the mood
Reward30.8%32.1%24.4%31.4%10.500.1050.057
Cost62.7%62.1%70.9%63.8%
Rewards and Costs6.5%5.8%4.7%4.8%
28.
Extent to which you let your guard down with your partner
Reward93.1%90.4%89.6%93.7%14.060.0290.061
Cost5.3%7.6%6.3%3.8%
Rewards and Costs1.6%1.9%4.1%2.5%
29.
Extent to which your partner lets their guard down with you
Reward87.7%90.5%87.9%94.4% A, C16.280.0120.066
Cost9.3% D7.3% D8% D3.8%
Rewards and Costs3%2.2%4.1%1.8%
30.
Method of protection (from sexually transmitted infections and/or pregnancy) used by you and your partner
Reward77.4% C, D80.8% C, D60.9% D43.5%178.43<0.0010.233
Cost17.3%13.8%32.7% A, B53.3% A, B, C
Rewards and Costs5.3%5.3%6.4%3.3%
31.
Extent to which you and your partner discuss and use protection (from sexually transmitted diseases and/or pregnancy)
Reward79.3% C, D81.4% C, D68.3% D45.9%144.41<0.0010.210
Cost16.6%16.3%27.1% A, B50.5% A, B, C
Rewards and Costs4.1%2.3%4.7%3.6%
32.
How comfortable you and your partner are with each other
Reward94.6%94.6%94.2%96.7%8.180.2250.046
Cost3.9%3.1%3%2.8%
Rewards and Costs1.5%2.2%2.8%0.5%
33.
Extent to which/way in which your partner influences you to engage in sexual activity
Reward76.7%76.4%75.8%83.1%12.170.0580.058
Cost17.9%18%16.4%13.5%
Rewards and Costs5.4%5.6%7.8% D3.3%
34.
Extent to which you and your partner argue after engaging in sexual activity
Reward51.5%42.5%48.5%45.5%11.190.0830.062
Cost45.5%55.3%47%51%
Rewards and Costs3%2.2%4.4%3.6%
35.
Extent to which you and your partner are/are not sexually exclusive (i.e., have sex only with each other)
Reward87.4% C94.4% A, C71.6%94.4% A, C143.25<0.0010.196
Cost7.7% B, D4.5%19.8% A, B, D3.1%
Rewards and Costs4.9% B1.2%8.5% A, B, D2.5%
36.
How much time you and your partner spend engaging in sexual activities
Reward60.8%69.6% A62.6%67.5%12.320.0550.057
Cost27.4%20.9%27.7%23.1%
Rewards and Costs11.7%9.6%9.7%9.4%
37.
How easy it is for you to have an orgasm (climax)
Reward89.7% B, C, D75.5%81.9% B, D73.7%50.11<0.0010.114
Cost7.6%19.2% A, C12.8% A20.3% A, C
Rewards and Costs2.7%5.4%5.3%6.1%
38.
How easy it is for your partner to have an orgasm (climax)
Reward76%90.8% A, C, D79.4%83% A44.73<0.0010.109
Cost17.1% B5.2%14.1% B12.7% B
Rewards and Costs6.9%4%6.5%4.3%
39.
Extent to which your sexual relationship with your partner reflects or breaks down stereotypical gender roles (the way women and men are expected to behave sexually)
Reward76.4% C71.8%66.3%78.8% C36.24<0.0010.105
Cost20.2% D23.3% D23.4% D13.5%
Rewards and Costs3.3%4.9%10.3% A, B7.7% A
40.
How your partner responds to your initiation of sexual activity
Reward73.4%90.3% A, C, D77.5%83.6% A, C58.07<0.0010.123
Cost20.3% B, D6%16.4% B, D10.8% B
Rewards and Costs6.3%3.7%6.1%5.5%
41.
Being naked in front of your partner
Reward95.1% B, C, D85.6%91.3% C89.6%29.323<0.0010.087
Cost3.8%10.9% A, C5.7%7.1%
Rewards and Costs1.2%3.4%3%3.3%
42.
Your partner being naked in front of you
Reward89.2%93.4%91.7%92.4%6.990.3210.043
Cost8.8%5%6.5%5.8%
Rewards and Costs2%1.5%1.8%1.8%
43.
Extent to which your partner talks to other people about your sex life
Reward56.3% B44.1%49.9%48.4%18.510.0050.075
Cost39.9%50.5% A42.5%45.2%
Rewards and Costs3.8%5.5%7.7%6.4%
44.
Extent to which you and your partner read/watch sexually explicit material (e.g., erotic stories, pornographic videos)
Reward43.2%41.1%48.8%44.9%10.190.1170.056
Cost49.4%53.9%44.1%47.2%
Rewards and Costs7.4%5%7.2%7.9%
45.
Pleasing/trying to please your partner sexually
Reward91.8% C92.5% C87.1%92.7% C14.960.0210.062
Cost4.9%5%6.7%4.1%
Rewards and Costs3.3%2.5%6.3% B3.3%
46.
Extent to which sexual interactions with your partner make you feel secure in the relationship
Reward90.5%89.5%88.1%89.7%2.510.8670.026
Cost5.9%6.6%7.2%5.5%
Rewards and Costs3.6%3.9%4.7%4.8%
47.
Extent to which you get sexually aroused
Reward95% B, C90.3%87.1%92.2% C21.710.0010.075
Cost3.1%6.5% A7.4% A5%
Rewards and Costs1.9%3.2%5.4% A2.8%
48.
Amount of spontaneity in your sex life
Reward68.7%69.9%69%74.8%7.640.2660.045
Cost22.1%20.5%22.8%16%
Rewards and Costs9.2%9.6%8.3%9.2%
49.
Extent of control you feel during/after sexual activity
Reward89.3%87%88%89.4%4.040.6710.034
Cost8.5%9.8%8.8%6.8%
Rewards and Costs2.3%3.2%3.2%3.8%
50.
Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you dislike but your partner enjoys
Reward57.4% B43.5%50.7%55.7% B26.69<0.0010.094
Cost36.7%48.9% A43.8%42.5%
Rewards and Costs5.8% D7.6% D5.5% D1.8%
51.
Extent to which you engage in sexual activities that you enjoy but your partner dislikes
Reward50.6% C44.2%40.8%47.9%16.910.0100.074
Cost42.4%49.7%47.9%45.9%
Rewards and Costs7%6.1%11.3%6.2%
52.
Worry that you or your partner will obtain a sexually transmitted infection from each other
Reward57.9% B, C, D45.7%48.5%42%31.09<0.0010.102
Cost38.7%51.9% A44.7%53.8% A, C
Rewards and Costs3.4%2.4%6.8% B4.3%
53.
How confident you feel in terms of your ability to please your partner sexually
Reward88.3%88.8%83.9%88.1%10.330.1110.052
Cost5.7%7.1%9.6%7.6%
Rewards and Costs6.1%4%6.5%4.3%
54.
Extent to which you and your partner engage in anal sex/anal play
Reward47.4%51.5%81.6% A, B, D52%157.24<0.0010.214
Cost45.7% C43.4% C13.2%40.8% C
Rewards and Costs7%5.1%5.3%7.2%
55.
Your partner’s ability to please you sexually
Reward88.7% C88.5% C81.4%87.6% C19.210.0040.071
Cost7%5.6%11.9% A, B, D7.4%
Rewards and Costs4.3%6%6.7%5.1%
56.
Extent to which you think your partner is physically attracted to/sexually desires you
Reward87.1%89.3%84.9%88.3%9.900.1290.051
Cost8.8%6.9%8.4%5.7%
Rewards and Costs4.1%3.8%6.6%6%
57.
Extent to which you are physically attracted to/sexually desire your partner
Reward95.2% B, C90.9% C84.9%92.5% C36.27<0.0010.097
Cost2.7%5.5%7.3% A4.3%
Rewards and Costs2.1%3.6%7.7% A, B, D3.3%
58.
Extent to which you and your partner are sexually compatible (i.e., well-matched in terms of your sexual likes/dislikes)
Reward85.9% C86.9% C79.4%89.6% C24.24<0.0010.079
Cost7.1%6.6%8.1%5%
Rewards and Costs7.1%6.5%12.5% A, B, D5.5%
1 The subscript letter denotes significant differences between groups, with higher scores for the group, which is represented by the subscript letter.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Calvillo, C.; Sierra, J.C.; Sánchez-Fuentes, M.d.M.; Granados, R. Sexual Rewards and Costs in Heterosexual and Gay Hispanic Adults. Healthcare 2024, 12, 170. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020170

AMA Style

Calvillo C, Sierra JC, Sánchez-Fuentes MdM, Granados R. Sexual Rewards and Costs in Heterosexual and Gay Hispanic Adults. Healthcare. 2024; 12(2):170. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020170

Chicago/Turabian Style

Calvillo, Cristobal, Juan Carlos Sierra, María del Mar Sánchez-Fuentes, and Reina Granados. 2024. "Sexual Rewards and Costs in Heterosexual and Gay Hispanic Adults" Healthcare 12, no. 2: 170. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12020170

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop