1. Introduction
Studies on meaningful learning experiences of students in higher education have taken variant dimensions over the last decades. A good number of psychologists and sociologists have dug deep into students’ reflections of themselves as they learn [
1,
2,
3]. An outcome of this insight into students’ learning is the identification of perceived self-efficacy as a good predictor of desirable learning outcomes [
4]. Perceived self-efficacy, according to Bandura [
5], refers to “beliefs in one’s capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to manage prospective situations” (p. 2). These internal convictions put an individual in a better situation to approach a presented task and behave in a particular way. An individual will tend to engage in tasks for which they have perceived self-competence and try to avoid the ones with less perceived self-competence. Self-efficacy is a determinant factor that positively correlates with the amount of effort expended on a task, perseverance when faced with impediments, and resilience during challenging situations [
1].
There has been a long-time debate among educationists on what are appropriate ways of assessing self-efficacy with some contending for the general perspective while others opting for the domain/situation specific perspective (e.g., [
6,
7]). The domain-specific perspective has influenced the conceptualization of self-efficacy around many fields. For example, mathematics self-efficacy has long been conceptualized as “a situational or problem-specific assessment of an individual’s confidence in her or his fully perform or accomplish a particular” [
2]. In a similar manner, engineering self-efficacy has been defined as a “person’s belief that he or she can successfully navigate the engineering curriculum and eventually become a practicing engineer” [
8]. Self-efficacy among engineering students has been investigated from conceptualization through developing measuring instruments to correlation with other variables like performance, anxiety, and performance [
9,
10]. In the same way it has been investigated in mathematics and other science-based courses.
Despite studies on mathematics self-efficacy and performance being sparse, especially in higher education (HE), the available empirical evidence has established a remarkable relationship between mathematics self-efficacy and academic performance, with the former being a strong predictor of the latter [
11,
12,
13,
14]. For example, Peters [
15] reported a quantitative empirical study on the relationship between self-efficacy and mathematics achievement including other constructs among 326 undergraduate students. Employing multi-level analysis, it was found that mathematics self-efficacy differed across genders, with boys taking the lead, and positively correlated with achievement. More recently, Roick and Ringeisen [
16] found, in their longitudinal study, that mathematics self-efficacy exerted a great influence on performance and played a mediating role between learning strategies and mathematics achievement. Similar corroborative results can also be found in the quantitative study reported in [
17].
A good number of educators have empirically shown and emphatically argued that the best way to achieve a higher predictive power of mathematics self-efficacy on academic performance of students is through task-specific measures (e.g., [
14]). Surprisingly, an extensive search of the literature revealed a lack of instruments for measuring students’ self-efficacy on year-one calculus tasks. This is despite the fact that calculus has been a compulsory part of most year-one Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) curricula of many universities in the world. The current study therefore aimed at developing a measure for assessing students’ self-efficacy on year-one calculus tasks with high psychometric properties. Furthermore, in order to enhance the predictive validity of the developed instrument its relationship with approaches to learning was also investigated.
2. Literature review
It is Albert Bandura who is considered the first psychologist in the history of clinical, social, and counseling psychology to have introduced the word “self-efficacy” (see, [
18]) to refer to “the conviction that one can successfully execute the behavior required to produce the outcomes” [
19]. However, some authors have contended that the “outcome expectancy” concept, which was extensively investigated prior to 1977, is equivalent to self-efficacy in theory, logic, and operationalization [
20,
21]. In Bandura’s rebuttal of this criticism, he elicited the conceptual differences between outcome and self-efficacy expectancies while maintaining that the kinds of outcomes people expect are strongly influenced by self-efficacy expectancies (see, [
22]). An overview of some of these controversies including arguments, counterarguments, disparities, and agreements can be found in the literature (e.g., [
23,
24]).
The basic tenet of the self-efficacy theory is that all psychological and behavioral changes occur as a result of modifications in the sense of efficacy or personal mastery of an individual [
19,
25]. In the words of Bandura [
19], “people process, weigh, and integrate diverse sources of information concerning their capability, and they regulate their choice behavior and effort expenditure accordingly” (p. 212). In addition, Bandura’s theory posits that the explanation and prediction of psychological changes can be achieved through appraisal of the self-efficacy expectations of an individual. In other words, the mastery or coping expectancy of an individual is a function of outcome expectancy—the credence that a given behavior will or will not result to a given outcome—and self-efficacy expectancy—“the belief that the person is or is not capable of performing the requisite” [
23].
Furthermore, the applications of Bandura’s theory as suitable frameworks of conceptualization are numerous in cardiac rehabilitation studies [
26], educational research, clinical nursing, music and educational practices [
27,
28,
29,
30]. In a study involving undergraduate students taking a biomechanics course in the United States, Wallace and Kernozek [
31] demonstrated how the self-efficacy theory can be used by instructors to improve students’ learning experience and lower their anxiety towards the course. Moreover, Sheu et al. [
32] reported a meta-analysis study on the contributions of self-efficacy theory in learning science, mathematics, engineering, and technology. The foregoing discussion points to the wide acceptance of Bandura’s self-efficacy theory not only among psychologists but also the educational community at large.
The different conceptualizations of self-efficacy involving general and domain-specific perspectives have recurring implications on the measurement of the construct. A look into the literature reveals that mathematics self-efficacy has been measured with instruments tailored towards general assessment (e.g., [
16]), sources of efficacy (e.g., [
33]), task-specific efficacy (e.g., [
34]), and adaptations from other instruments or which are self-developed (e.g., [
35]). These instruments have their strengths and weaknesses. A brief account of each type of instruments is presented in the forthcoming paragraphs accompanied by the justification for a desired approach in the current study.
General assessment instruments have been developed to measure students’ self-reported ratings of their capabilities to perform in mathematical situations. Chan and Yen Abdullah [
36] developed a 14-item mathematics self-efficacy questionnaire (MSEQ) in which respondents appraised their ability on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (usually). MSEQ had four sub-structures comprised of three items each measuring general mathematics self-efficacy and “efficacy in future” coupled with four items each measuring self-efficacy in class and in assignments. Evidence of validity was provided, and internal consistency of the items was investigated with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.94, which showed high reliability. A similar result was also reported in an omnibus survey instrument developed by Wang and Lee [
37], in which mathematics self-efficacy was a subcategory. These kinds of omnibus instruments have been reported to be problematic in their predictive relevance [
38].
Other closely related instruments to mathematics general assessment types are the adapted mathematics subcategory items from other instruments. For example, in a longitudinal study involving 3014 students, You, Dang and Lim [
39] developed a mathematics self-efficacy measure by adapting items from the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ) developed by Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, and McKeachie [
40]. Furthermore, in an attempt to operationalize mathematics self-efficacy, Y.-L. Wang et al. [
35] developed an instrument which was an adaptation of the science learning self-efficacy questionnaire developed in [
41] by substituting mathematics for science in the original instrument. Some authors have independently developed measures for mathematics self-efficacy in which the sources of their items are not disclosed. For example, Skaalvik, Federici, and Klassen [
42] developed a 4-item mathematics self-efficacy Norwegian measure as part of a survey instrument without any disclosure of the sources of their items. These instruments were not too different from the general academic self-efficacy measures in terms of their predictive power of performance [
38].
Based on Bandura’s [
3,
5] theorized sources of self-efficacy—
mastery experience,
vicarious experience, verbal/social persuasions, physiological or affective states—some educationists have developed and investigated some measures [
33,
43,
44]. In a quantitative empirical three-phase study, Usher and Pajares [
33] developed a measure and investigated the sources of mathematics self-efficacy. The study started in Phase One with an 84-item measure and ended in Phase Three with a revised 24-item instrument. The final version contained six items in each of the mastery experience, vicarious experience, social persuasions, and physiological state subcategories with 0.88, 0.84, 0.88, and 0.87 Cronbach’s alpha coefficients as pieces of evidence of item internal consistency, respectively. The study confirmed the hypothesized mastery experience of Bandura [
5] as the strongest predictor of learning outcome [
33]. Other studies have also reported corroborative empirical evidence to confirm the hypothesized sources of mathematics self-efficacy using Usher and Pajares’ [
33] instrument with either wording or language adaptations [
45,
46].
With the exception of sources of self-efficacy measures, the most effective approach in terms of achieving high predictive power of learning outcome is to assess mathematics self-efficacy through a task-specific measure [
47]. The basic idea in developing a mathematics task-specific instrument is to conceptualize self-efficacy on predefined mathematical task(s) and tailor the instrument items towards the respondent’s self-capability to complete the tasks. An example of early instruments developed using this approach was the 52-item mathematics self-efficacy scale (MSES) by Betz and Hackett [
34] to measure self-efficacy among mathematics college students. In the administration of this instrument, the respondents had to rate their confidence in successfully completing 18-item mathematics tasks; solving 18-item math related problems; and achieving at least a “B” grade in a 16-item college mathematics related course like calculus, statistics, etc. Evidence of reliability was provided with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of 0.90, 0.93, and 0.92 on each subscale as well as 0.96 on the 52-item scale [
34]. MSES has been investigated, revised, and validated with items adapted to university mathematics tasks/problems as well as its rating reduced from a 10-point to five-point Likert scale [
14,
48].
A task-specific mathematics self-efficacy instrument was also utilized by the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) in their 2012 international survey across 65 countries as reported in [
49]. The eight-item instrument measured students’ self-reported level of confidence in completing some mathematical tasks without solving the problems. The rating involved a five-point Likert scale ranging from “not at all confident” to ”very confident”’ in which students were asked, for example, “how confident would they feel about solving an equation like 2(
x + 3) = (
x + 3) (
x – 3)”? Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.83 was provided as evidence of reliability [
49].
5. Conclusions
Despite the abundant empirical evidence of the high predictive power of task-specific mathematics self-efficacy in the literature, an instrument for its measure is still lacking [
4,
50]. The current study was framed within a quantitative research methodology to develop a concise measure of calculus self-efficacy with high psychometric properties among year-one university students. Bandura’s self-efficacy theory provided a theoretical framework for the conceptualization and operationalization of items on the developed calculus self-efficacy inventory (CSEI). This theory posits that all psychological and behavioral changes occur as a result of modifications in the sense of efficacy or personal mastery of an individual [
19,
25]. On this basis, the accompanied guidelines and recommendations of this theory [
50] were followed in constructing the CSEI items.
The initial instrument contained 15 items, in which 234 respondents rated their confidence in solving year-one calculus tasks on a 100-point rating scale. The results of the factor analysis using MRFA for factor extraction, promin rotation, and parallel analysis for retaining factors revealed a one-factor solution of the model. The final 13-item inventory was unidimensional with all eigenvalues greater than 0.42, an average communality of 0.74, and a 62.55% variance of the items being accounted for by the latent factor, i.e., calculus self-efficacy. These results can be interpreted as evidence of construct validity in measuring students’ internal confidence in successfully solving some calculus tasks. The CSEI has the following advantages over the mathematics self-efficacy scale (MSES) developed by Betz and Hackett [
34] and its revisions (e.g., [
48]): Its concise length, task specificity, higher factor loadings, and communality.
Furthermore, the reliability coefficient of the CSEI was found to be 0.91 using the ordinal coefficient alpha with the formula described in [
70]. This coefficient portrays evidence of high internal consistency of items in the inventory [
63]. This reliability coefficient is higher than the coefficient of the mathematics task subscale of the MSES reported in [
2,
34], and it is within the ranges of the revised MSES reported in [
14,
48]. There are some misconceptions on the appropriate use of the ordinal coefficient alpha for estimating scale reliability as can be found in [
73]. These misconceptions are acknowledged. However, the examples of the types of items provided in Chalmer’s own article are enough to justify the use of the ordinal coefficient alpha in the current study.
The results of the current study also provided an insight into the correlation between approaches to learning and calculus self-efficacy. The significant positive correlation between the deep approach and self-efficacy as well the significant negative correlation between the surface approach and self-efficacy are indications of the predictive validity of the CSEI. This finding also confirms the hypothesis of Bandura’s self-efficacy theory [
4,
6] as well as corroborates the mediating role played by self-efficacy between learning strategies and performance reported in [
16]. It is a crucial to remark that the causal effect between calculus self-efficacy and approaches to learning is not claimed with this finding. Rather, the results have only established a relationship between these constructs that can be explored further in future studies. The final 13-item instrument is available in English and Norwegian upon request from the corresponding author. This inventory is therefore recommended to university teachers in order to assess students’ confidence in successfully solving calculus tasks.