Supporting Pre-School Children’s Number Learning Through Embodied Representation
Abstract
1. Introduction
1.1. Early Number Sense
1.2. Embodied Representation in Early Mathematics Learning
1.2.1. Finger-Based Number Representations
1.2.2. Developing Representational Drawing
1.2.3. Conceptual Body Movement
1.3. Pedagogy and Mathematics Education in Early Childhood
1.4. Research Question
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Data Sources and Analysis
2.2.1. Teacher Data
2.2.2. Child Data
2.2.3. Data Aggregation and Pattern Searching
3. Results
3.1. The Pre-School Teacher’s Adoption of Embodied Learning Approaches
“Some of the things we sort of implemented early on and we planned into the environment, for example, the number track outside. We did that quite early on in the study and it’s kind of evolved in terms of we were making it at the start, now the children make it, they use it in different ways.”
“We have a beanbag throwing game that’s on the guide, but we also put some skittles some days where the children can knock the skittles down or where the children jump or do hopscotch. And now actually the children use that as their own play, whereas we would sort of introduce it before, now I just noticed the children go there by themselves and do their own thing with it which is great.”
“When children were just playing, touching them [the numbers], ordering them and then we introduced the blindfolds and made it sort of into a game where they would pop the blindfold on and try and recognize that number and something else that’s slightly different but on the same track. We made these little plates that have got these sort of raised, the slightly raised numbers and then the amount of objects.”
Generalisation Statement
3.2. Finger Numbers and Dexterity
“…we play the bunny finger numbers and things like that and so I can see how all of that helps them with this because they’ll see three and where at the start, they would sort of be going one two three and then put it down. Now they see the fruit come up and immediately they get their fingers ready”.
“I had two children who were absent for a while and here she was really struggling to work out how many fingers to get. And one boy had been away, and you can see he was really having to sort of use his other hand, sort of force his fingers down and out of the way. And I just thought it was really interesting because neither of those have been there for many of the finger activities.”
Generalisation
3.3. Drawing Development and Representing Number
“I found after doing all the air shapes together and then we drew them on the board, they could then really create some good shapes with their Play -Doh. And something else that we kind of do a lot as the pencil tracing. I think that lends itself quite well to both shape work and number writing. We’ve just got some little cards they sort of write over with a whiteboard pen and then they do it in pencil in their little special book. So yeah those are some of the things I’ve been sort of really focusing on.”
Generalisation
4. Discussion
4.1. Purposeful Embodied Representation Can Be Readily Incorporated in Play-Based Learning Approaches That Account for the Intentionality of Both Teachers and Children
4.2. Relational Triads Describe the Interconnectedness of Motor Development, Mathematical Representation and Number Sense Development
4.2.1. The Finger-Number Triad
4.2.2. The Drawing-Number Triad
4.2.3. The Movement-Number Triad
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Asakawa, A., & Sugimura, S. (2014). Developmental trajectory in the relationship between calculation skill and finger dexterity: A longitudinal study. Japanese Psychological Research, 57(2), 189–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Australian Government Department of Education [AGDE]. (2022). Belonging, being and becoming: The early years learning framework for Australia (V2.0). Australian Government Department of Education for the Ministerial Council. Available online: https://www.acecqa.gov.au/sites/default/files/2023-01/EYLF-2022-V2.0.pdf (accessed on 15 July 2025).
- Baroody, A. J., Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2019). Teaching and learning mathematics in early childhood programs. In C. Brown, M. B. McMullen, & N. File (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood care and education (pp. 329–353). Wiley Blackwell Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & MacDonald, B. L. (2019). Subitizing: The neglected quantifier. In A. Norton, & M. W. Alibali (Eds.), Constructing number. Research in mathematics education. Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clements, D. H., Sarama, J. H., & Liu, X. H. (2008). Development of a measure of early mathematics achievement using the Rasch model: The research-based early math assessment. Educational Psychology, 28(4), 457–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Csíkos, C., Szitányi, J., & Kelemen, R. (2012). The effects of using drawings in developing young children’s mathematical word problem solving: A design experiment with third-grade Hungarian students. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 81, 47–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dackermann, T., Fischer, U., Nuerk, H. C., Cress, U., & Moeller, K. (2017). Applying embodied cognition: From useful interventions and their theoretical underpinnings to practical applications. ZDM Mathematics Education, 49, 545–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Devlin, B. L., Jordan, N. C., & Klein, A. (2022). Predicting mathematics achievement from subdomains of early number competence: Differences by grade and achievement level. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 217, 105354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dowker, A. (2008). Individual differences in numerical abilities in preschoolers. Developmental Science, 11, 650–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Edens, K., & Potter, E. (2008). How students “unpack” the structure of a word problem: Graphic representations and problem solving. School Science and Mathematics, 108(5), 184–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edwards, S. (2017). Play-based learning and intentional teaching: Forever different? Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 42(2), 4–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischer, U., Suggate, S. P., Schmirl, J., & Stoeger, H. (2017). Counting on fine motor skills: Links between preschool finger dexterity and numerical skills. Developmental Science, 21, e12623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischer, U., Suggate, S. P., & Stoeger, H. (2020). The implicit contribution of fine motor skills to mathematical insight in early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ginsburg, H. P. (2016). Helping early childhood educators to understand and assess young children’s mathematical minds. ZDM Mathematics Education, 48(1), 941–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gracia-Bafalluy, M., & Noël, M. P. (2008). Does finger training increase young children’s numerical performance? Cortex, 44(4), 368–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hall, E. (2009). Mixed messages: The role and value of drawing in early education. International Journal of Early Years Education, 17(3), 179–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holgersson, I., Barendregt, W., Emanuelsson, J., Ottosson, T., Rietz, E., & Lindström, B. (2016). Fingu—A game to support children’s development of arithmetic competence: Theory, design and empirical research. In P. Moyer-Packenham (Ed.), International perspectives on teaching and learning mathematics with virtual manipulatives. Mathematics education in the digital era (Vol. 7). Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jordan, N., Devlin, B., & Botello, M. (2022). Core foundations of early mathematics: Refining the number sense framework. Current Opinion in Behavioral Science, 46, 101181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knaus, M. (2017). Supporting early mathematics learning in early childhood settings. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 42(3), 4–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhnke, P., Beaupain, M. C., Arola, J., Kiefer, M., & Hartwigsen, G. (2023). Meta-analytic evidence for a novel hierarchical model of conceptual processing. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 144, 104994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacDonald, A. (2013). Using children’s representations to investigate meaning-making in mathematics. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 38(2), 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacDonald, A. (2025). When and how do educators facilitate mathematical learning opportunities for babies and toddlers? International Journal of Early Childhood, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macrine, S. L., & Fugate, J. M. (2022). Embodied cognition and its educational significance. In S. Macrine, & J. Fugate (Eds.), Movement matters: How embodied cognition informs teaching and learning (pp. 13–24). MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Malanchini, M., Tosto, M. G., Garfield, V., Dirik, A., Czerwik, A., Arden, R., Malykh, S., & Kovas, Y. (2016). Preschool drawing and school mathematics: The nature of the association. Child Development, 87(3), 929–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mathias, B., & von Kriegstein, K. (2023). Enriched learning: Behavior, brain and computation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 27(1), 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mavilidi, M. F., Okely, A., Chandler, P., Domazet, S. L., & Paas, F. (2018). Immediate and delayed effects of integrating physical activity into preschool children’s learning of numeracy skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 502–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mavilidi, M. F., Ouwehand, K., Schmidt, M., Pesce, C., Tomporowski, P. D., Okely, A., & Paas, F. (2022). Embodiment as a pedagogical tool to enhance learning. In S. A. Stolz (Ed.), The body, embodiment, and education (pp. 183–203). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- McCluskey, C., Kilderry, A., Mulligan, J., & Kinnear, V. (2023). The role of movement in young children’s spatial experiences: A review of early childhood mathematics education research. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 35(2), 287–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nguyen, T., Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., Clements, D. H., Sarama, J. S., Wolfe, C., & Spitler, M. E. (2016). Which preschool mathematics competencies are most predictive of fifth grade achievement? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 550–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ollivier, F., Noël, Y., Legrand, A., & Bonneton-Botté, N. (2020). A teacher-implemented intervention program to promote finger use in numerical tasks. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 35(3), 589–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pape, S., & Tchoshanov, M. (2001). The role of representation(s) in developing mathematical understanding. Theory Into Practice, 40(2), 118–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penner-Wilger, M., & Anderson, M. L. (2013). The relation between finger gnosis and mathematical ability: Why redeployment of neural circuits best explains the finding. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poland, M., & van Oers, B. (2007). Effects of schematising on mathematical development. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 15(2), 269–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef][Green Version]
- Pyle, A., & Danniels, E. (2017). A continuum of play-based learning: The role of the teacher in play-based pedagogy and the fear of hijacking play. Early Education and Development, 28(3), 274–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2009). Early childhood mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for young children. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Sella, F., & Lucangeli, D. (2020). The knowledge of the preceding number reveals a mature understanding of the number sequence. Cognition, 194, 104104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stake, R. E. (2003). Case studies. In N. Denzin, & Y. Lincon (Eds.), Strategies of qualitative inquiry (pp. 134–164). Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Suggate, S., Stoeger, H., & Fischer, U. (2017). Finger-based numerical skills link fine motor skills to numerical development in preschoolers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 124(6), 1085–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). You can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. NeuroImage, 59, 3139–3148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Way, J. (2018). Two birds flew away: The ‘jumble’ of drawing skills for representing subtraction pre-school to year 1. In J. Hunter, P. Perger, & L. Darragh (Eds.), Making waves, opening spaces (Proceedings of the 41st annual conference of the mathematics education research group of Australasia) (pp. 98–101). MERGA. [Google Scholar]
- Way, J., & Cartwright, K. (2024, July 7–14). Finger counting patterns in year 1 children [Paper presentation]. 15th International Congress on Mathematics Education (ICME-15), Sydney, Australia. [Google Scholar]
- Way, J., & Ginns, P. (2024). Embodied learning in early mathematics education: Translating research into principles to inform teaching. Education Sciences, 14, 696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, S. (2007). Graphic-narrative play: Young children’s authoring through drawing and telling. International Journal of Education and the Arts, 56(8), 1–28. [Google Scholar]
- Yukhymenko, M. A., Brown, S. W., Lawless, K. A., Brodowinska, K., & Mullin, G. (2014). Thematic analysis of teacher instructional practices and student responses in middle school classrooms with problem-based learning environment. Global Education Review, 1(3), 93–109. [Google Scholar]
Episode | Purpose and/or Main Activity |
---|---|
Episode 1 Face to Face (5 h) | A whole day introduction to the PL programme, embodied learning and the resources available to them. |
Implementation phase 1 | Begin implementation of embodied learning approaches. |
Episode 2 Online live (2 h) | Deepen understanding of embodied learning practices, focus on relationships between embodied modes and mathematical representations. |
Implementation phase 2 | Continue implementation. |
Episode 3 Online live (1 h) | Guided reflection, feedback and planning support. |
Implementation phase 3 | Continue implementation. |
Episode 4 Online live (2 h) | Deepen understanding of embodied learning practices with focus on student responses. |
Implementation phase 4 | Continue implementation. |
Episode 5 Online live (1 h) | Guided reflection, feedback and planning support. |
Student Name (Pseudonym) | Age at Time 1 Years—Months |
---|---|
Sally | 4—8 |
Ben | 4—4 |
Adam | 4—7 |
Theo | 4—4 |
Will | 4—5 |
Ann | 4—6 |
Mia | 4—7 |
Lily | 3—11 |
Yousef | 5—3 |
Embodied Activity | Frequency |
---|---|
Finger numbers Teacher and children use fingers to show numbers (e.g., hold up three fingers and say ‘three’). | 18 |
Finger counting Start on left hand with closed fist and raising thumb, palm facing self. | 13 |
Finger counting to 5 using left hand, starting with closed fist and raising thumb (palm facing self). Teaching note: When modelling to class, turn your back and hold your hands high so the class sees the same perspective as their own hands. | 1 |
Finger counting to 10 continuing from left hand across to ‘little finger’ of right hand. | 4 |
Fingertips Touch the matching quantity of fingertips to dots on a card (subitising small quantities in various arrangements). Note: you could also use the free app ‘Fingu 1’ for iPad. | 4 |
Hold up (Bunny Ears) Children hold up a specified number of fingers out of sight (e.g., eyes closed, over head or under table) | 4 |
Finger stories Use fingers to model rhymes and simple stories depicting increasing or decreasing the number in a group, etc., (e.g., Model ‘five little ducks’ by starting with five raised fingers and lowering a finger each time a little duck goes missing). | 3 |
Touch tracing 1-digit Child uses their pointing finger to trace around numerals (0 to 9), preferably raised or textured. When confident, trace with eyes closed. | 4 |
Mark-marking to represent a quantity in one-to-one correspondence for quantities that are visible, or not visible. (a) Visible, e.g., make a mark for each bear in the basket. (b) Not visible, e.g., clap three times, and make three marks with chalk. Also, reverse the process, e.g., make three marks, then clap three times. | 1 |
Pencil tracing 1 Children trace over lines (straight, curved, zig-zag, curly, etc.) or basic shapes with a pencil or crayon. Invite the children to copy the lines or shapes using the pencil. | 4 |
Air shapes Teacher uses a pre-planned gesture that air-traces the outline of the shape every time the shape’s name is spoken (e.g., every time you say, ‘sit in a circle’, trace a circle in the air). Encourage the children to also make shape gestures in connection with shape names and descriptive words such as, curved, straight, pointy, wiggly, small, big. | 5 |
Finger tracing Touch-tracing and air-tracing various lines and shapes with the pointing finger. Encourage children to close their eyes and retrace the figure from memory. (Incl. rice tray and sensory bags) | 9 |
Finger drawing in shallow sand trays to explore lines and shapes. This could take the form of free form drawing or be representational drawing in response to a stimulus such as a story or in response to music). Children might like to close their eyes while they draw. | 10 |
Arrow drawing Touch-tracing, pencil tracing and air-tracing arrows (including bent turn -right and -left arrows). Then invite the children to draw arrows themselves on paper, or with pavement chalk outside. | 1 |
Number track counting Children walk or step or jump along a number track forwards and backwards without turning around. | 7 |
Throw a bean bag Throw a beanbag onto a number track, then step or jump on the numerals in sequence to fetch it, while saying the number names. Note: You could also use a hopscotch-type structure or a ‘number snake’ for this activity. | 4 |
Child | Time 1 | Time 2 |
---|---|---|
Sally | 10 | 25 |
Ben | - * | 100 |
Adam | 12 | 12 |
Theo | 11 | 43 |
Will | 10 | 29 |
Ann | 29 | 49 |
Mia | 12 | 29 |
Yousef | NR ** | 12 |
Name | Time | Dexterity | Subitized 3 Fingers | Finger Count to 10 with 1-to-1 | REMA Task |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sally | 1 | Independently moves fingers into position | Yes | Yes | 1–5 subitizes |
2 | Fluid, confident finger movement | Yes | Yes | 1–5 subitize for 1–2 counts by ones for 3–5 | |
Ben | 1 | Cannot independently move fingers into position | No—counted | No 1-to-1 beyond 1 | 1–5 subitizes |
2 | Fluid, confident finger movement | Yes | Yes | 1–5 subitizes | |
Adam | 1 | Independently moves fingers into position | No—counted | Only to 5 | None correct counts by ones |
2 | Independently moves fingers into position | No—counted | Yes | 1 subitizes (others incorrect) | |
Theo | 1 | Cannot independently moves fingers into position | No—counted | No 1-to-1 beyond 1 | 1, 2 correct counts by ones |
2 | Independently moves fingers into desired (mostly) | Yes | Some 1 to 1 | 1–5 subitizes then check counts by ones 3–5 | |
Will | 1 | Independently moves fingers into position (mostly) | Yes | Yes | 1–5 counts by ones |
2 | Fluid, confident finger movement | Yes | Yes | (missed 1) 2–5 subitizes | |
Ann | 1 | Independently moves fingers into position (mostly) | Yes | Yes | 1–5 subitizes eyes closed to match then checks |
2 | Independently moves fingers into position (mostly) | Yes | Yes | 1–5 subitizes | |
Mia | 1 | Independently moves fingers into position | No—counted | Yes | 1–5 subitizes then check counts by ones 2–5 |
2 | Independently moves fingers into position | Yes | Yes | 1–5 subitizes then check counts by ones 3–5 | |
Lily | 1 | Cannot independently moves fingers into position | Yes | Only to 5 | (No data) |
2 | Struggles to move fingers into position | Yes | Yes | ||
Yousef | 1 | Struggles to move fingers into position | Yes | Confused but self-corrected | None correct |
2 | Independently moves fingers into d position | Yes | Confused | 1–5 subitizes then check counts by ones 3–5 |
Time 1 Grip | Time 1 Drawing | Time 2 Grip | Time 2 Drawing | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Theo | “I can’t draw birds but I could draw somethings else” | “I’ve got 5. When finished S: That’s the ‘tail’ and a bird, a beak and I draw eyes.” | ||
Will | “This is the roof and these two birds” | “The line is the fence and some birds” | ||
Lily | “four, eight, two” | “I draw the fence.” |
Time 1 | Time 2 | |
---|---|---|
Adam | “5 birds flew away. … 1 bird sat on the fence … 1 bird flew away. No 5 birds” | “5 birds on the fence … 2 flew away … 4 still on the fence” |
Mia | “I draw 5 birds on a fence” | “5 birds on a fence. 2 flew away” |
Yousef | “A drew the tree with leaves and a bird flying away” | “I drewed 5 birds and that’s the mumma bird, she flied off…” |
Ann | “So they’re on the wall (points to birds) and 2 flew away (points to 4th and 5th bird and flicks finger up the page) so there’ll be 3 more left” | “So they’re on the wall (points to birds) and 2 flew away (slides finger up arrows) so there’ll be 3 more left” |
Ben | “7 birds (pointed to 7) and 2 flew away (pointed to 2).” | (Wrote 3B first) “5 minus the 2 equals to 3” |
Name | Time | Drawing Category | Make Group or 5 | Start with 7 Make 12 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sally | 1 | B. Beginning representation. Something from story | Yes counts by ones | Yes counts from one |
2 | C1. Noticing number—drew one group | Yes counts by ones | Yes counts from one | |
Ben | 1 | C1. Did not draw—wrote numerals. Noticing number—correct size one group | Yes puts 3, some in some out to get 5 | No counts on but gets 15 |
2 | D. Did not draw—numerals only. Wrote correct Equation 5 − 2 = 3 | Yes counts by ones | Yes counts on from 7 | |
Adam | 1 | A1. No recognisable representation of objects | No, by twos, dumps random amount | Did not attempt |
2 | C2. Noticing number—Two groups drawn and one or both is of the correct | No, by twos dumps random amount | Did not attempt | |
Theo | 1 | A1. no recognizable representation of objects | Yes counts by ones | No counts by ones the added items only |
2 | C1. Noticing number—drew one group | Yes counts by ones | No trial and error count by ones (8) | |
Will | 1 | C1. Only used strokes but intentionally represented number in one group | Yes counts by ones | Yes counts from one |
2 | C1. Noticing number—drew one group (drew birds) | Yes counts by ones | Yes counts on from 7 | |
Ann | 1 | D. Complete partitioning—Three correct sized groups. Crossed out two birds | Yes counts by ones | No counts by ones all items (15) |
2 | D. Complete partitioning-Three correct sized groups. Used arrows | Yes counts by ones | Yes counts on from 7 | |
Mia | 1 | C1. Noticing number—drew one group | Yes counts by ones | No counts by ones added items only |
2 | C2. Noticing number—Two groups drawn | Yes counts by ones | Yes counts from one | |
Lily | 1 | A1. Not representing—drew squiggles and said they were numbers | No data | No data |
2 | B. Something recognisable from the story—a fence | No data | No data | |
Yousef | 1 | B. Something recognisable from the story—a bird | No, by ones dumps random amount | No counts from one with error (16) |
2 | C2. Noticing number—two groups | Yes counts by ones | No counts from one with errors |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Way, J.; Cartwright, K. Supporting Pre-School Children’s Number Learning Through Embodied Representation. Educ. Sci. 2025, 15, 1170. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15091170
Way J, Cartwright K. Supporting Pre-School Children’s Number Learning Through Embodied Representation. Education Sciences. 2025; 15(9):1170. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15091170
Chicago/Turabian StyleWay, Jennifer, and Katherin Cartwright. 2025. "Supporting Pre-School Children’s Number Learning Through Embodied Representation" Education Sciences 15, no. 9: 1170. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15091170
APA StyleWay, J., & Cartwright, K. (2025). Supporting Pre-School Children’s Number Learning Through Embodied Representation. Education Sciences, 15(9), 1170. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15091170