Next Article in Journal
Building Successful STEM Partnerships in Education: Strategies for Enhancing Collaboration
Previous Article in Journal
The Institutional Evolution of Chinese University Data Governance: An Analytical Framework Based on Historical Institutionalism
Previous Article in Special Issue
Levels of Complexity in Mathematics Teachers’ Knowledge Connections: An Approach Based on MTSK and Piaget’s Schemas
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Stepping Stones: Adopting a Fading Programme Design to Promote Teachers’ Use of Metacognitive Strategies for Mathematical Problem Solving

Department of Social Work, Education and Community Wellbeing, Northumbria University, Newcastle-upon-Tyne NE7 7XA, UK
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Educ. Sci. 2025, 15(7), 892; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15070892
Submission received: 11 June 2025 / Revised: 4 July 2025 / Accepted: 9 July 2025 / Published: 12 July 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Different Approaches in Mathematics Teacher Education)

Abstract

Metacognition and self-regulated learning are widely understood to offer significant benefits for pupils’ mathematical problem solving; however, the existing literature highlights that the under-representation of these concepts in curriculum, policy, and teacher professional development means that their potential for impact remains unfulfilled. This article, therefore, examines the potential value of an innovative fading professional development programme—“Stepping Stones”—in enhancing teachers’ understanding and use of metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving. Adopting a convergent mixed methods design, this pilot evaluation involved Year 2 teachers across five primary schools. The results from both qualitative and quantitative data demonstrate that, as the scaffolding provided by programme materials faded and teachers assumed greater responsibility for session planning, they incorporated metacognitive strategies into their planning and delivery with increased independence. The results also indicate the acceptability of this professional development model, suggesting that, when combined with peer collaboration, the fading design was associated with improvements in knowledge and confidence regarding both metacognition and mathematical problem solving, alongside increased ownership and buy in. The conclusions advocate further examination and implementation of fading models of professional development to promote the understanding and use of metacognition for mathematical problem solving and recommend exploration into different professional development contexts.

1. Introduction

The potential benefits of metacognition are well-established both generally (EEF, 2021b; Lai, 2011; J. Perry et al., 2019) and for mathematical problem solving more specifically (Muncer et al., 2022; Vula et al., 2017). However, metacognition is often neglected in policy and practice (J. Perry et al., 2019), with some research suggesting that teachers’ own understanding of metacognition is lacking, leading to misconceptions and weak implementation (Muijs & Bokhove, 2020). This, combined with a lack of support and guidance for teachers (Jossberger et al., 2010), means that its potential for impact remains unfulfilled.
In exploring ways to promote teachers’ understanding and use of metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving, this article reports the impact of an innovative, fading programme—"Stepping Stones”—as part of a multi-faceted professional development (PD) offer. The article discusses research from a pilot evaluation across five English primary schools in response to one central research question: how did Stepping Stones influence teachers’ understanding and use of metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving? This question is further divided into two sub-questions: (a) did teachers’ deployment of metacognitive strategies vary as the programme faded and (b) how did teachers perceive the acceptability of the fading design?
The article begins with an overview of the existing literature regarding metacognition and its role in mathematical problem solving before discussing implementation in school contexts. We then briefly discuss research around PD for teachers and provide an overview of the Stepping Stones programme. From there, we explain the research design and methodology before the results and discussion, in which we respond to the research question by exploring both teachers’ deployment of metacognitive strategies and their perceptions around the acceptability of the fading programme model.

1.1. Metacognition

Often simplistically defined as “thinking about thinking”, metacognition is widely acknowledged to significantly influence learning outcomes (EEF, 2021b; Lai, 2011; Muijs & Bokhove, 2020; J. Perry et al., 2019; Veenman et al., 2006). First defined by Flavell (Flavell, 1979), metacognition involves planning, monitoring, regulating, and evaluating performance whilst completing a task (Lai, 2011). This can encompass a range of classroom activities, including “comparing different students’ approaches to problem solving and decision making; identifying what is known, what needs to be known, and how to produce that knowledge; or having students think aloud while solving problems” (Costa, 1991).
There is evidence that interventions targeting the development of metacognitive strategies are associated with large effects across age and attainment ranges (J. Perry et al., 2019; V. Perry et al., 2012; Wall, 2008). Furthermore, several authors highlight the link between metacognition and motivation (Dignath & Büttner, 2008; Lai, 2011; Martinez, 2006), indicating that increased examination and reflection upon the learning processes unpinning successful task completion can encourage pupils’ self-belief, leading to increased resilience and perseverance in the face of setbacks (Martinez, 2006).

1.2. Implementation in School Contexts

Despite broad consensus regarding the potential value of metacognition for pupil outcomes, these approaches are underdeveloped in school contexts due to their relative absence in national policies and curricula (J. Perry et al., 2019). Widespread teaching, particularly in primary education contexts, may also have been curtailed by debate surrounding the age at which pupils are capable of metacognition, with some authors suggesting that children under the age of eight have limited capacity for metacognition (Flavell, 1979; Lai, 2011). However, in recent years, this view has been increasingly challenged, with those such as Whitebread et al. (2009) finding both verbal and non-verbal indicators of metacognition in children from the age of three. Whilst this may appear encouraging, some sources indicate that metacognition does not necessarily increase with age (Kuhn, 2000; Lai, 2011) and that some pupil groups, including socio-economically disadvantaged pupils, may be less likely to acquire metacognitive knowledge and skills without explicit teaching (Muijs & Bokhove, 2020). Taken together, these concerns underscore the vital importance of establishing how best to promote the development of metacognition within classroom contexts.
In their systematic review of the literature, Muijs and Bokhove (2020) emphasise the importance of both direct and indirect approaches for promoting the development of pupils’ metacognition, including explicit instruction and teacher modelling, alongside opportunities for guided practice, dialogue, and scaffolded inquiry. Veenman et al. (2004) also highlight the importance of embedding the teaching of metacognition within curriculum content, maintaining that discrete teaching may inhibit pupils’ capacity to transfer metacognitive knowledge and skillfulness between contexts.
References to the value of thinking aloud are also prominent within the literature, whereby teachers model their approach to a task alongside their metacognitive planning, monitoring, and evaluation of their chosen strategies (K. D. Allen & Hancock, 2008; Kistner et al., 2010; Muijs & Bokhove, 2020; Schraw, 1998). When modelling, Muijs and Bokhove (2020) particularly endorse deployment of worked examples, whereby teachers adopt a staged approach, reviewing problems incrementally to articulate thought processes and the rationales for the use of specific cognitive and metacognitive strategies before gradually reducing scaffolding to increase pupil independence. However, some studies highlight that explicit modelling is used relatively infrequently by teachers (Kistner et al., 2010; Schraw, 1998), suggesting that its potential for impact may yet be unrealised.
This echoes wider concerns regarding teacher knowledge of metacognition (Veenman et al., 2006), which are of particular importance given the role of teachers as metacognitive role models (EEF, 2021b; Wall & Hall, 2016). This research suggests that teachers’ deliberate deployment of strategies to promote pupils’ metacognition in turn enhances their own metacognitive awareness, forming a “catalytic relationship” (Wall & Hall, 2016, p.408) whereby the deepening of metacognition of each actor further enhances that of the other. In addition to acting as role models (Wall & Hall, 2016), effective implementation of metacognitive strategies in classroom contexts often requires teachers to first adopt a meta-metacognitive role (Stillman, 2011) through monitoring and scaffolding pupils’ metacognitive processes. During complex problem solving, teachers need to actively orchestrate learners’ metacognition through observing how they plan, monitor, and evaluate their approaches. This assessment is subsequently used to inform adaptations to metacognitive instruction, enabling the gradual transfer of responsibility to pupils themselves (Stillman, 2011).
The role of peer discussion and collaboration more broadly is also significant to the development of metacognition, both generally (Lai, 2011; Schwartz et al., 2009) and for mathematics specifically (Donovan & Bransford, 2005; Hodgen et al., 2018; Kyriacou & Issitt, 2008). For example, Schwartz et al. (2009) suggest that metacognition may be developed through articulating and explaining reasoning. This view is supported, albeit loosely, by the work of several other researchers, including Cross and Paris (1988) and Hennessey (1999), as well as Lai (2011), who describe the benefits of peer interactions in constructing and refining metacognitive knowledge and regulation. Indeed, Schraw (1998) believes that pupils may well provide better role models for metacognition than teachers themselves, arguing that “Frequently, students are better able to model cognitive and metacognitive skills, and provide a powerful rationale for these skills within the student’s zone of proximal development” (p. 118). However, there is a note of caution evident in the work of those such as Stein et al. (2008), who note that facilitating such discussions requires considerable skill.

1.3. Metacognition and Mathematical Problem Solving

Numerous studies cite the importance of metacognition in promoting competence in mathematics (Donovan & Bransford, 2005; Hodgen et al., 2018; Muijs & Bokhove, 2020; Mulholland, 2021). Indeed, Picker and Berry (2000) suggest that supporting pupils’ understanding of the learning process is of particular import given perceptions of success in mathematics as a supernatural power resulting from “the general invisibility to pupils of the mathematical process [whereby] mathematical facility looks more like a power than an ability which anyone has the possibility to learn” (p. 88). This is supported by a considerable body of evidence emphasising the importance of teaching pupils to think through mathematics to deepen understanding (Boaler, 2006; Jansen, 2008; Westwood, 2011; Wright et al., 2008).
Mathematical problem solving is highly prized by both educators and employers; however, several sources raise concerns that the current English curriculum and assessment system fail to adequately support pupils in developing this important skill (Boylan et al., 2023). This has led some to advocate a shift away from rote and procedural forms of learning, towards greater emphasis upon metacognition to promote flexibility and independence (Boylan et al., 2023, 2024). The link between metacognition and improved mathematical problem solving is well-established (Izzati & Mahmudi, 2018; Woodward et al., 2012). Mathematical problem solving involves an initial assessment of the problem and subsequent planning, monitoring, and evaluation of the approach used to address it, therefore aligning closely with the metacognitive process (Foster, 2019; Izzati & Mahmudi, 2018; Stillman, 2011). Similarly, several sources emphasise the importance of encouraging pupils to identify and compare multiple types of mathematical problems and the strategies deployed to approach these (Ofsted, 2021; Woodward et al., 2012; Wright et al., 2008) to build knowledge and confidence in approaching novel and unfamiliar problems.
However, despite their acknowledged importance, evidence suggests that both metacognition and problem solving are neglected in the broader mathematics curriculum (Ofsted, 2023) and teacher PD (Boylan et al., 2023; Department for Education, 2024b; Ofsted, 2023). Ofsted’s (2023) review of 50 English schools reported insufficient opportunities to explain or justify approaches to problem solving, or describe relationships, impeding the development of deeper understanding. Ofsted (2021, 2023) also highlights the need for greater consideration of progression, sequencing problems to enable pupils to engage with increasingly complex examples over time. This under-representation is also reflected in pupil outcomes. For example, when considering international trends in mathematics learning for Year 9 pupils (aged 13–14 years), England’s Department for Education (DfE) (Department for Education, 2024b) suggested that recent improvements in international league tables have been driven by improvements in number, data, and probability and identified relatively weak outcomes for mathematical reasoning, which underpins problem solving. Combined, these studies underscore the need for a more defined and coherent approach to metacognitive development to promote pupils’ mathematical problem solving.

1.4. Teacher Professional Development

The existing research is clear that PD—defined as “structured, facilitated activity for teachers intended to increase their teaching ability” (Sims et al., 2021)—can positively impact pupil outcomes (Cordingley et al., 2015; Higgins et al., 2014; Sancar et al., 2021), with some sources suggesting that teacher quality accounts for up to 40 per cent of variance in learning (The Sutton Trust, 2011). PD is often looked upon favorably by teachers and policymakers (Opfer & Pedder, 2010) and attracts high levels of investment both in England (Department for Education, 2024a) and internationally (Boulay et al., 2018). Despite this, research on what PD is, what it looks like, and how it should be delivered has been relatively limited (Fletcher-Wood & Zuccollo, 2020; Sims et al., 2021). This is of particular importance given that the impacts of individual approaches vary considerably, underscoring the potential value of better distinguishing between more- and less-effective PD models (Sims et al., 2021).
While those such as Asterhan and Lefstein (2024) challenge the assumption that it is possible to identify generally effective features of PD, in recent years, there has been a marked increase in studies attempting to review impact and identify commonalities in what makes PD effective (Fletcher-Wood & Zuccollo, 2020; Sims et al., 2021). However, these efforts have been impeded by the multitude of different definitions and designs deployed within studies, limiting comparison of their relative effectiveness (Sims et al., 2021) and rendering it difficult to reach conclusions about which features of teacher PD matter most (Fletcher-Wood & Zuccollo, 2020).
Various sources emphasise the importance of incorporating multiple complementary mechanisms, or activities, when designing PD (Sims et al., 2021), suggesting that their deployment is more closely related to successful outcomes than factors identified by previous reviews, such as duration of activity (Cordingley et al., 2015). Common mechanisms include sequencing PD to manage cognitive load; providing opportunities to revisit and consolidate prior learning (Cordingley et al., 2015; Sims et al., 2021); motivating teachers through presenting information from credible sources—such as research evidence—(Cain, 2015; Sims et al., 2021); and establishing shared goals (Cordingley et al., 2015; Sims et al., 2021).
The existing literature also advocates the use of modelling and instruction to promote understanding of specific teaching techniques (C. D. Allen & Penuel, 2015; Cordingley et al., 2015; Sims et al., 2021); feedback (Cordingley et al., 2015); collaboration and peer support (C. D. Allen & Penuel, 2015; Miquel & Duran, 2017), and it also emphasises the importance of reflection and repetition of desired behaviours when seeking to embed and sustain change (Sims et al., 2021). Beyond consideration of mechanisms, Cordingley et al. (2015) and Popova et al. (2021) underscore the importance of developing both subject and pedagogic content within PD, emphasising that different subjects require radically different pedagogies (Shulman, 1986; Villegas-Reimers, 2003), thus highlighting the importance of carefully considering content and context, as well as the mechanisms adopted for PD delivery.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. The Current Study

Combining the findings from research into metacognition and teacher PD, we developed a programme and accompanying PD offer to enhance teachers’ understanding and use of metacognitive strategies to develop mathematical problem solving. This programme drew upon earlier work from one member of our research team, employing metacognitive strategies with 9–11-year-old pupils (Mulholland, 2021; Mulholland Shipley, 2016). The refined programme—"Stepping Stones”—was piloted with 6–7-year-old pupils in five primary schools in England (6 × Year 2 classes and 1 × mixed Year 1/2 class) across ten weeks between January and March 2024, drawing upon the findings of V. Perry et al. (2012), who found larger effects for metacognitive interventions delivered in ten or more sessions. Intended to supplement the daily mathematics lesson, sessions lasted between 15 and 20 min in duration and were delivered as a whole-class intervention, three times per week.
Framed within the context of a visual metaphor—stepping stones across a river—the programme prompted teachers and pupils to focus attention on the learning process and to understand and implement metacognitive approaches in mathematics teaching. Programme materials were developed by two members of our research team and included teacher scripts and pupil-facing session slides. Programme sessions were structured around worked examples—complete part-by-part demonstrations of a particular approach (V. Perry et al., 2012)—to encourage pupils to critique approaches to mathematical problem solving whilst reducing demands upon cognitive load (Muijs & Bokhove, 2020; V. Perry et al., 2012; Woodward et al., 2012). Session slides were used to review possible approaches to mathematical problems step by step, representing each step on an individual session slide. The number of steps varied between 3 and 10, depending on the complexity of the mathematical problems. Each step was annotated, drawing upon the work of Atkinson et al. (2000), who suggest that both labelling and segmentation increase the likelihood that pupils will adapt methods rather than persist with previous approaches. Examples of session slides are provided in Figure 1.
As programme delivery progressed, fading worked examples and worked examples featuring clearly marked errors were introduced both to model the use of these formats to participating teachers and to provide greater variety—and responsibility—for pupils, whilst simultaneously deepening understanding (Renkl & Atkinson, 2003). Each Stepping Stones session also incorporated an additional problem with a very similar structure to the worked example. This enabled pupils to immediately apply their learning, with the support of the previously studied example readily available in memory (Atkinson et al., 2000).
Drawing upon research from well-evidenced strategies for promoting metacognitive development, programme materials also incorporated models for metacognitive talk, including thinking aloud and debriefing questions to structure pupils’ reflections regarding the planning, monitoring, and evaluation of learning (Muijs & Bokhove, 2020; Schraw, 1998). These models were communicated to participating teachers through the provision of scripts for individual programme sessions, including suggested content to guide teachers’ speech and actions. For example, when first introducing the mathematical problem around which each session focused, the script provided text to inform teachers’ thinking aloud as follows: “When we have any unfamiliar problem our first job is to make sure we understand what is going on here. What do we know from reading this? What information do we now have?” Scripts also included questions to elicit pupils’ metacognitive thinking, such as “Which representation do you think would be quickest and easiest to draw? Why would that be helpful?” and “Which elements of this problem were most challenging?” All Stepping Stones sessions also concluded with consistent debriefing questions to encourage structured reflection and evaluative thinking. The repetition of these questions was deliberate, intended to encourage familiarity and internalisation of these questions for both pupils and teachers. A copy of the programme slide used at this point within sessions is included in Figure 2.
Programme materials were complemented by an accompanying PD offer to promote the development of professional understanding. This was a fundamental element of the Stepping Stones programme, drawing upon research underscoring the role of teachers’ own metacognitive knowledge in influencing that of their pupils (Branigan & Donaldson, 2020; Stillman, 2011; Wall & Hall, 2016). Teachers were supported in three face-to-face training sessions, one school visit from one member of the research team to observe programme sessions in action and provide targeted feedback and support, and a further optional online check-in. An outline of the PD offer is provided in Figure 3.
In addition to these discrete PD sessions, we intended the programme materials themselves to form part of the comprehensive learning offer, serving as worked examples to model metacognitive strategies in action. Stepping Stones can, therefore, be seen to deploy worked examples in two distinct ways: firstly, as a means of supporting pupils’ understanding of approaches used for specific mathematical problems, and secondly, as a vehicle for teachers’ PD to develop understanding of pedagogic techniques to promote metacognitive thinking.
A central principle of Stepping Stones was the fading approach it adopted. For the initial six weeks of the programme, all session plans and resources were provided for teachers by our research team. This was intended to support teachers’ internalisation of metacognitive teaching strategies, helping these to become embedded within teachers’ instructional repertoires. In the final four weeks of delivery, the scaffolding offered by programme materials gradually faded, with the provision of partially completed session slides and teacher scripts during weeks 7 and 8 of delivery and blank materials during weeks 9 and 10. This fading process prompted teachers to assume increasing responsibility for session planning, and is represented below, in Figure 4. Although faded worked examples are well-established in the literature regarding skills acquisition as a means of managing learners’ cognitive load (Centre for Education Statistics and Evaluation, 2017; Paas et al., 2003), to our knowledge, this is their first application in a teacher PD context. At the outset of this study, our working hypothesis was that the fading design would prompt the gradual transition from the heavily scaffolded use of metacognitive strategies towards teachers’ more independent application as they planned and created their own programme materials in the final four weeks of delivery.
This fading process was facilitated by support from both peers and two members of our research team, providing opportunities to integrate numerous mechanisms associated with effective PD, including guidance and instruction (C. D. Allen & Penuel, 2015; Cordingley et al., 2015; Sims et al., 2021), feedback (Cordingley et al., 2015), collaboration, and peer support (C. D. Allen & Penuel, 2015; Miquel & Duran, 2017). For example, teachers’ increasingly independent application of the previously modelled metacognitive strategies was supported by collaborative planning of programme sessions in pairs or trios, as well as peer feedback. Time for this collaboration was incorporated into PD sessions, acknowledging the many demands upon teacher workload and the associated challenges for professional learning. During the second PD session, teachers worked collaboratively to plan sessions for delivery during weeks 7 and 8 from partially completed plans and programme materials. In the third PD session, teachers wholly planned sessions for delivery during weeks 9 and 10 using blank materials, following the structure and teaching techniques employed in previous weeks.
These opportunities for collaboration and co-construction were an important element of the Stepping Stones PD offer and were intended to provide peer support, scaffolding the development of participating teachers’ knowledge and the use of metacognitive strategies. By planning sessions together from partially completed and then blank materials, teachers were prompted to make shared decisions regarding which strategies to include and how to sequence them, verbalising their internal decision-making processes, receiving feedback, and refining their plans accordingly. These collaborative processes enabled reflection on future action (Schön, 1987) and were also intended to foster increased ownership of materials, drawing upon prior research that suggests a link between ownership and increased application of desired teaching techniques (Miquel & Duran, 2017; Sims et al., 2021) Crucially, collaboration also functioned as a mechanism to deepen understanding and encourage the internalisation of metacognitive strategies as participating teachers considered how and when to adapt these for their individual contexts.

2.2. Research Design

Employing a convergent mixed methods design (Creswell & Creswell, 2018), this study comprised quantitative data to track changes in teachers’ experiences and learning outcomes across the delivery period, including the degree of variance in teachers’ deployment of metacognitive strategies as the programme faded. Concurrently, qualitative data was used to explore teachers’ perceptions concerning the acceptability of the programme and its features. Ethical approval for this work was obtained from the host organisation (application number 10332). The data were originally collected as part of an Education Endowment Foundation (EEF)-funded Innovation project, with two members of the research team responsible for the design, delivery, and management of the project. Upon completion of the project for the EEF, the project report was used to conduct further data analysis for this study, in line with the EEF’s publication screening processes. All shared data was anonymised, prioritising participants’ rights to anonymity and confidentiality.
The research design was underpinned by a critical realist framework (Bhaskar, 1978; Robson, 2002). The selection of this approach was driven by complexities found in studies that report challenges to teachers’ understanding and implementation of metacognitive strategies in the classroom (Dignath & Büttner, 2008; Wilson & Bai, 2010). Metacognition involves the creation of internal mental processes that are not directly observable, such as planning, monitoring, and evaluating one’s own thinking. The adoption of a critical realist approach provided a framework to unpack these “deep” mechanisms to reveal more about teachers’ metacognitive practices. By this means, quantitative data were treated as the “empirical” layer of the study, where frequency counts and associations of metacognitive strategies across the fading programme established a base layer to identify previously unobserved and unknown patterns. Through discovery of these underlying mechanisms, quantitative data revealed an “actual” snapshot of the absence and presence of these strategies with some understanding of their use in shared planning activities. Qualitative data contributed evidence to capture the “real” and offered explanatory insights into the underlying causes of how teachers interpreted the programme design.

2.3. Data Collection and Sample

Using a purposive sampling strategy, seven teachers from five primary schools in Northeast England were recruited via an open expression of interest shared by various mathematics education organisations across the region. The sampled teachers all worked full time and had varying levels of teaching experience. Specifically, one teacher was in their first year of teaching, two teachers had at least three years of experience, and four had been teaching for ten or more years. Programme delivery was free to schools, and each school received a payment of GBP 3000 from the Education Endowment Foundation for taking part.
Impact was evaluated via multiple data collection points and methods. This convergent mixed methods approach permitted in-depth analysis and triangulation of data, centering around understanding teachers’ metacognitive practices and facilitating greater insights into the underlying factors influencing their experiences and engagement. These multiple forms of data are summarised in Table 1.
Pre-, mid-, and post-programme surveys included a series of open (qualitative) and closed (quantitative) questions, such as “How do you currently teach problem solving in Year 2?” and “How confident do you feel about teaching pupils to solve problems (1 = not confident at all; 5 = very confident)” to understand more about teachers’ experiences and reflections. Of the seven teachers sampled in the pilot, n = 5 completed the pre-survey, n = 7 completed the mid-survey, and n = 6 the post-survey. We also collected feedback following each of the three PD sessions, with completion rates of n = 5 following session 1, n = 5 following session 2, and n = 6 following session 3. Teachers were asked to rate their levels of confidence (1 = not confident at all; 5 = extremely confident) and consider the impact upon their learning, including by identifying the most useful elements of the training.
Visits to each participating school were scheduled during the second or third week of teaching. This provided a valuable means of triangulating teachers’ self-reported perceptions and actions with observations of programme delivery undertaken by members of our research team. The visits provided an opportunity to review fidelity of implementation, as well as acceptability to teachers and schools. Structured observations used a five-item checklist to assess the degree to which key programme features were implemented (1 = not implemented; 2 = partially implemented; 3 = fully implemented). As the programme faded (weeks 7–10), a second checklist was used to review teacher-generated materials as teachers became more responsible for, and assumed ownership of, these sessions. This checklist featured nine key strategies, including models for metacognitive talk such as thinking aloud and the use of debriefing questions to structure pupils’ planning, monitoring and evaluation of learning, and the use of worked examples to support pupils to critique approaches to learning. Each review was undertaken by at least one member of the research team. This took approximately 15 min to complete and was supplemented with field notes and brief comments.
Finally, semi-structured interviews were conducted with participating teachers. Two interviews were completed with two pairs of teachers, while three individual interviews were arranged with the remaining teachers. All interviews lasted up to 30 min, with an extended focus on programme design, and they ran concurrently alongside other data collections. As part of quality assessment, an independent external review established the face validity of the pre-, mid-, and post-programme survey questions, checklists, and interview protocol before data collection commenced. This process ensured that key features of the programme remained a central focus of the review.

2.4. Data Analysis

The study concurrently gathered quantitative and qualitative data through a variety of instruments, as outlined in Table 1. The analytical plan of simultaneous data collection allowed us to start building a picture of important programme trends, insights, and reflections gathered from teachers and research team field notes. Figure 5 illustrates the process for each data collection point, outlining how these data were integrated and interpreted.
Following data collection, analysis was conducted separately, drawing upon quantitative and qualitative analytical techniques (see Section 2.5 and Section 2.6). The independently analysed quantitative results and qualitative themes were then systematically merged, allowing for cross-validation, corroboration, and explanation of the findings. An example of this is the quantitative data collected from the pre-, mid-, and post-programme surveys; following analysis, these were integrated with qualitative PD session feedback and semi-structured interview findings to add depth and detail in our reporting.

2.5. Quantitative Data Analysis

Quantitative data from surveys and checklists were prepared and entered into the Social Package for Statistical Sciences (SPSS, version 29). Frequency counts of teachers’ responses and observations to quantitative components of the pre-, mid-, and post-programme surveys, training session feedback surveys, and programme implementation and teacher-generated materials checklists were collated and analysed alongside qualitative data from open response questions. These data were assessed concomitantly, ensuring a parallel and equitable analysis of multiple sources to accurately report teachers’ views and experiences.
Two ordinally coded checklists were used in this study: one for observation of programme implementation (1 = not implemented; 2 = partially implemented; 3 = fully implemented) and another for review of teacher-generated materials (1 = missing; 2 = brief information included; 3 = detailed information included). The review of programme implementation was based on five observations of programme delivery, while nine reviews of teacher-generated materials were recorded in total (review of three sessions for each of weeks 7, 8, and 9). Reviews of teacher-generated materials for sessions 10–12 of week 10 were excluded from the final analysis due to time constraints in the final PD session, which prevented the completion of materials within the allotted time. This is considered in further detail in the Results and Discussion below. Initial analysis included inspection of frequency counts for each checklist item to determine how features of the programme were implemented and how often they were included in teacher-generated materials. For the observation of programme implementation, a total score of 15 would indicate features were “fully implemented”, while for the teacher-generated materials checklist, a total count of 27 would represent “detailed information included” for each feature.
Descriptive statistics (frequency counts and mean ranks) were used to understand patterns and trends from data recorded in the checklists. Further analysis was performed on the teacher-generated materials checklist using the Goodman and Kruskal gamma test (Goodman & Kruskal, 1954) to investigate the association of the nine programme features in teachers’ planning across the nine sessions held between weeks 7 and 9. The test investigated the existence of concordant or discordant pairs from ordinally ranked observation data for each session over the specified time. The test provides an estimate of the strength of association between the variables with sign (e.g., positive ties or negative ties). The coefficient ranges between measures of −1 and 1, where −1 represents a perfect negative association and 1 represents a perfect positive association (Agresti, 2012). Whilst the test is non-parametric by design, there is an assumption that ties have a monotonic relationship, whether positive or negative. Following an inspection of a visual plot, we inspected the ties for each of the nine checklist items on the number of observations (n = 9) (weeks 7–9) and concluded there to be a monotonic relationship.

2.6. Qualitative Data Analysis

We adopted Braun and Clarke’s (2022) six-phase process of reflexive thematic analysis (RTA) to explore patterns across the qualitative datasets. It is important to acknowledge the influence of our subjective opinions, influences, and overlapping identities (designer, deliverer, and researcher) throughout this process. Leaning into—and not discounting—these influences is an important consideration in RTA. Additionally, we ensured there was sufficient time to move freely back and forth between the phases, as is required for the reflexive aspect of thematic analysis.
The two members of our research team most closely involved in the design and delivery of the Stepping Stones programme initiated the familiarisation phase of analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2022). In the subsequent phases, the two remaining members of our research team provided support for further analysis. Having both insider and outsider perspectives throughout this process allowed for thoughtful and complex discussions and analysis, with insider perspectives affording opportunities to bring the data to life, understanding the situations and stories behind the words, whilst outsider perspectives offered fresh viewpoints and perceptions (Nowell et al., 2017). These different perspectives also encouraged us to move fluidly between inductive modes, where analysis and theme development are driven by the data, and deductive modes, where existing theoretical concepts provide a lens through which to read the data (Braun & Clarke, 2022). Our adoption of RTA was, therefore, an intentional epistemological choice, allowing for inductive insight as well as deductive engagement. This flexibility in approach allowed for rich discussion and added further depth to our analysis.
For our familiarisation and initial coding, we used both paper and electronic copies of the data. We also completed this at various locations, collaboratively in rooms at the university campus, as well as from home over Microsoft Teams. We were conscious that this change in modes could potentially affect each of our moods and, by extension, our engagement with the analytical process. Initially, we produced 18 different codes together in the same room. After a two-week break, we reconvened to review the data and began to cluster these codes into four potential broad patterns of meaning, or candidate themes. After further discussion, a review of the codes and the complete dataset, we further refined our candidate themes to identify three broad areas of interest: professional development design, feelings of increased ownership, and the value of collaboration. Throughout phases three, four, and five, we moved between our initial themes, developing and reviewing them and eventually defining, refining, and naming them.
Braun and Clarke (2022) remind us that reflexive thematic analysis could continue indefinitely, but researchers must decide when the analysis has reached a depth that feels both meaningful and useful. In reviewing the qualitative data alongside the quantitative data, we collectively agreed upon our final themes: building knowledge, confidence, and capacity; increased ownership and “buy in”; and the value of collaboration. Figure 6 provides an overview of our analysis process, illustrating how our initial codes developed into candidate themes before being refined into three final themes. In keeping with the theoretical framework of reflexive thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2022), our development of themes reflected a creative process of synthesising meaning, shaped by our engagement with the data and relevant theoretical ideas.
Whilst our sample size limits any claims to generalisability, this is neither unexpected nor was it an aim of this study. Instead, when considering the analysis presented in this paper, we hope to enable readers to consider the transferability of insights presented here (Braun & Clarke, 2022). As such, it is the responsibility of the reader to determine the extent to which our findings may apply to their own contexts.

3. Results

Combined, our results respond to the central research question by considering how Stepping Stones influenced teachers’ understanding and use of metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving. We also explore teachers’ experience of the fading approach through examining two further sub-questions: (a) did teachers’ deployment of metacognitive strategies vary as the programme faded and (b) how did teachers perceive the acceptability of the fading design? Our analysis of the quantitative and qualitative data has identified three themes as follows:
  • Building knowledge and confidence captures teachers’ perceptions and understanding of planning and teaching metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving.
  • Ownership and “buy in” reports upon teachers’ views around the importance of metacognition, the impact the fading design had upon attitudes towards the programme, and the strategies therein.
  • The value of collaboration speaks to the extent to which teachers valued working with each other as well as the research team.
These themes align with the wider literature regarding principles for effective design and delivery of PD more broadly, suggesting that the fading model implemented through Stepping Stones provided a successful means of enhancing teachers’ understanding and use of metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving. These themes will be discussed in turn below, drawing upon data in the form of verbatim quotations from teachers and complemented with descriptive statistics from the quantitative analysis.

3.1. Building Knowledge, Confidence, and Capacity

Teachers suggested that opportunities to engage with underpinning research regarding both metacognition and mathematical problem solving during PD sessions increased their motivation to deliver the Stepping Stones programme.
“sometimes you might try things in your own classroom and you never really know how it’s working […] It’s been really lovely trying something new and talking about the theory behind it to start with because then it gives you a reason why you want to do it as well”.
(Teacher C, final interview)
“I hadn’t even really thought about the fact that we never really teach problem solving. We expect children to be able to do it, but we never actually explicitly teach it. And then I thought especially those pupil premium children, their metacognition isn’t as developed as others and it really got us thinking that it’s definitely something we need to do”.
(Teacher T, final interview)
In addition to motivation, there was a demonstrable increase in teachers’ self-reported knowledge of the evidence base for metacognition following participation in the programme. Pre-programme surveys indicated that five teachers perceived their knowledge as “not familiar at all”, whereas post-programme surveys revealed that six teachers reported increased familiarity of knowledge of metacognitive strategies. Similar patterns were observed in responses concerning teachers’ confidence in teaching problem solving, with post-programme results indicating that four teachers reported feeling “extremely confident” while two teachers felt indifferent or slightly confident.
This was also reflected in the qualitative data. For one teacher in their first year of teaching, programme delivery afforded opportunities to revisit and reinforce knowledge obtained from statutory early career training (Department for Education, 2019). However, for more established teachers, PD and programme content developed an understanding of both metacognition and mathematical problem solving. This resonates with the wider literature, which suggests that these elements of pedagogy are currently under-represented within both educational policy and practice (Boylan et al., 2023; Foster, 2019; Ofsted, 2023; J. Perry et al., 2019).
“I knew the word metacognition and I didn’t know a lot about it before I came on the course. So for me, it was useful in terms of enabling me to know how to teach the children to plan and when to work through a problem and to be able to do that with a little bit of support and then independently”.
(Teacher T, final interview)
“Stepping Stones has enabled me to understand how to explicitly teach problem solving. The course has helped me to understand different approaches to teaching problem solving through worked examples, faded worked examples and mistakes”.
(post-programme survey, Q7a)
Teachers particularly praised the consistent structure of Stepping Stones sessions, emphasising the value of this programme design feature in building familiarity and confidence. For example, teachers reported the benefits of repeatedly applying techniques, such as highlighting key words, to support their discussion when seeking to understand problem solving tasks.
“it’s nice that the programme repeats itself, so we start with the highlighting and then we move on to unpicking the question. I think having that repeated process constantly has been really good for supporting my disadvantaged children who don’t remember as much”.
(Teacher S, final interview)
The accounts suggest that repeated exposure to worked examples of metacognitive strategies embedded throughout the teacher scripts and pupil-facing PowerPoint slides during weeks 1–6 of delivery successfully instilled knowledge of metacognitive strategies and their practical application, enabling teachers to implement these with fidelity to our intended design. This indicates that programme materials built requisite knowledge to enable teachers to serve as metacognitive role models, aiding preparation for meta-metacognition, whereby teachers monitor and scaffold pupils’ metacognitive processes (Stillman, 2011).
Teachers’ perceptions of the importance of programme materials as models are further supported by observations of programme delivery, undertaken by members of the research team during school visits during weeks 2 and 3 of Stepping Stones delivery. The results in Table 2 reveal moderate to high levels of implementation of seven out of nine strategies. Two strategies, “15–20 min session delivered” and “Programme slides used”, were fully implemented (100%) and ranked highest compared to the other strategies. High implementation rates were also observed for “Problem-solving objective shared with the class” and use of “Mixed-attaining trios” to promote peer discussion, both at 93%. In contrast, “Expectations for discussion” and “Strategies for problem-solving” were implemented in 87% of the observations of practice reviewed by the research team, demonstrating a substantial but slightly lower implementation rate. The strategies “River used” and “Alternating worked example” showed the lowest implementation rates, at 60% and 53%, respectively. Fieldnotes recorded by the research team suggest that during the setting visits, most teachers did not use the optional alternating worked examples to extend sessions. However, they reported using them within other sessions. In sum, we interpret these results as an indicator of consistent and strong adherence to these specific strategies throughout delivery.
By internalising the structures and key ideas utilised within weeks 1–6 of the Stepping Stones programme, participating teachers appeared to become less dependent on the scaffolds as the programme progressed. Qualitative responses shared during post-programme interviews indicated that, during later weeks of delivery, increased confidence enabled teachers to move beyond the use of the session scripts to deploy metacognitive strategies with greater flexibility, including devising appropriate metacognitive questions in response to classroom interactions with pupils. This shift was further reinforced during the collaborative planning sessions, in which participants’ suggestions deepened their understanding of how and when to deploy metacognitive strategies effectively.
“the first couple of weeks, I used [the scripts] like gospel and reading them when I was getting the kids to talk. But then I’ll be honest, the last two weeks I haven’t even really looked at them. I’ve looked at them at the start and said alright, it’s a complete problem or a faded problem or a mistake, just so that I know what’s coming next”.
(Teacher C, final interview)
Over time, teachers not only moved away from using teacher scripts but also increasingly tailored metacognitive discussions to respond flexibly to their pupils’ interests and thinking.
“The PowerPoints were brilliant. I didn’t use the teacher script and I did for the first week but I kind of went through the slides obviously before I taught it and I just thought about if I was teaching this what would be my questions because I think my questions would be sometimes different from what was in the teacher’s script and really because the way that the slides are set out and there were lots of opportunities for further discussion. So, me and the children ended up going off on a tangent so I didn’t want to be held to a script, I wanted to teach it the way I want to teach it”.
(Teacher S, final interview)
In the initial weeks of delivery, this suggests that teachers were not just deploying metacognitive strategies themselves but were also learning how to scaffold pupils’ use of these through meta-metacognition (Stillman, 2011). This account suggests that, as confidence built, teachers began to transition from structured modelling to adaptive support based on real-time assessments of their pupils, reflecting the fading of meta-metacognitive scaffolds (Stillman, 2011).
This self-reported decrease in teachers’ reliance upon session scripts is echoed in the quantitative data. Table 3 presents results from observations of the key features of the Stepping Stones programme, undertaken from a review of participant-generated materials conducted by our research team. The results from three of the checklist items indicate a strong, negative, statistically significant association between Learning Intentions (G = −0.857), Teacher Overview (script) (G = −1.00), and Think Aloud (G = −0.750) checklist items and the number of planning sessions. More specifically, the proportion of concordant pairs is 85% (Learning Intentions) and 75% (Think Aloud) lower than the proportion of discordant pairs from our analysis.
Analysis indicates that data returned in response to the checklist item regarding teacher scripts represents a perfect negative pattern. This suggests that in the latter weeks of delivery, as the scaffolding offered by programme materials fades and teachers have built considerable familiarity with pedagogic techniques, there is a shift from the inclusion of “detailed information” towards “brief information”. The change, initiated at the points at which teachers assume greater responsibility for session planning, represents a small shift backwards when comparing a hypothetical shift from “detailed information” to “missing”. Our analysis suggests that although a decrease in the level of detail occurred, the core components of these planning elements remained present, albeit in a less developed form. Although it might initially seem that the reduced scaffolding led to teachers being less detailed in their metacognitive strategies, a closer analysis of both our qualitative and quantitative data reveals the opposite. In fact, their increased confidence in the strategies enabled them to produce less-detailed scripts.
“I think the more we’ve got in the way of it and I’m able to speak from me, the more I know what I’m asking. We don’t need the scripts as much because, well, we’ve done it for 10 weeks”.
(Teacher T, final interview)
In contrast to the three statistically significant checklist items, there were five checklist items that were interpreted as not being statistically significant in accordance with the planning sessions. We interpret these findings as suggesting that teachers’ planning remained consistent, rather than undergoing a fading trend. Specifically, checklist items “Alternating worked example identified as an optional extension”, “teacher overview details key question prompts”, “Worked example uses complete/mistakes/fading as appropriate”, “Visual representations are used to support understanding of key mathematical concepts”, and “Worked example included in the session slides include reference to/exemplification of the problem solving objectives taught during the session” reported no statistical change in within our research team’s review of participant-generated session planning. The consistent presence of detailed information for these items suggests they were universally prioritised in planning, likely due to their perceived fundamental importance or strong existing practice and experience of utilising them during the programme.
During post-programme interviews, teachers also highlighted the value of worked examples in delivering clear models, allowing pupils to revisit a shared reference point when undertaking subsequent problem solving tasks with their peers.
“So they know that we’re gonna get through these steps. We’re gonna learn how to do it, and then we’re gonna have a go. So I think that was really useful for them as well and gave the other ones a great model of how to solve the problem. So even if there weren’t 100% sure they could look go right, well, we’ll do this one. But they were looking back at the steps on the river to have a go at their problem”.
(Teacher L, final interview)
This supports the existing literature regarding the role of worked examples in scaffolding the gradual release of responsibility from teacher to pupils (Webb et al., 2019). Worked examples focus attention upon recognising and articulating approaches used for problem solving (Woodward et al., 2012), effectively managing cognitive load (EEF, 2021a; Sweller et al., 2019) in preparation for pupils’ independent problem solving. Similarly, teachers’ accounts emphasised the importance of worked examples not solely in pupil-facing materials but also to support their own professional learning. For example, teachers echoed participants elsewhere in the literature (J. P. Allen et al., 2015; Sims et al., 2021) in commending the live modelling of teaching techniques during PD, suggesting the importance of this approach in building confidence and shared understanding: “we came away from that first session, though already with like a vision of how it was going to work in the school” (Teacher K, final interview).
It is important to note that teachers also recognised that opportunities for collaborative planning of future programme sessions, “meant very little extra teacher load was needed” (post-programme survey, 11a), with “no extra work to be done and no extra resources to be made” (Teacher L, final interview). Responses indicated that this may have contributed towards perceptions of the acceptability of the programme, suggesting that “there’s nothing better than coming away from training and doing something that you can actually immediately put in” (Teacher L, final interview). This suggests the importance of protecting time for discussion and scaffolded application when encouraging the implementation of new techniques and approaches.
There was also some indication that teachers increasingly applied metacognitive strategies such as thinking aloud, worked examples, and questions to elicit metacognitive thinking beyond the Stepping Stones programme itself, integrating these into their wider classroom practice.
“I feel I am much more confident in using items such as, Think Alouds and worked examples in my practice and I have started using them across my teaching”.
(post-programme survey, 8a)
“I definitely hear myself thinking, but why? ‘Tell me you know that because…’ and those types of stem sentences definitely carry through”.
(Teacher K, final interview)
“it made me think a little bit more about small steps. So I think sometimes I think about doing a worked example, but I don’t think about all the small steps where that’s what the problem solving really did because there was 5–6 steps to it. Sometimes it was going in a lot smaller steps than I would probably think of myself, which is really good because then it affected my practice in a good way. So I use a lot of the strategies that we’ve done in Stepping Stones to help me in my work which is really good.”.
(Teacher C, final interview)
Again, this is in line with findings across the literature relating to PD, advocating the value of repetition in promoting the formation of habits, influencing behaviour change, and leading to sustained integration of new techniques and approaches (Michie et al., 2014; Sims et al., 2021).

3.2. Increased Ownership and “Buy in”

Teachers confirmed the acceptability of the fading design, indicating that this increased feelings of ownership, as well as enhancing understanding of the underpinning rationale for pedagogic intentions within sessions.
“I think it’s really nice to have that ownership of the system to it as well. And then coming back into school and obviously I work with another year group partner, so then me telling them and saying, oh, well, we’ve one this because of this reason and this is how it’s going to work”.
(Teacher C, final interview)
“I think that was really beneficial with the teaching of it and the implementation of it, like understanding where it’s all come from and doing it yourself. Especially writing the script because it gave… It allowed us to realise what we do need in the script and what we can skip out”.
(Teacher L, final interview)
These accounts suggest that collaborative planning empowered teachers, equipping them to offer guidance and explanations to enhance colleagues’ understanding of programme content and delivery. As they assumed greater responsibility for planning sessions within the fading programme design, teachers additionally reported tailoring content according to the specific needs of their pupils. This may have been because three out of the five teachers surveyed thought the average level of understanding of problem solving and reasoning within their current class cohorts was slightly below the expected standard of attainment (pre-programme survey, Q13). Adaptations by teachers included minor changes to “some of the wording so the children can understand a bit better” (mid-programme survey, Q8), supplementing programme materials with additional resources and manipulatives, as well as larger changes to the degree of challenge presented by the mathematical problems themselves.
“I think probably since the people on the course started developing them, they’ve become a bit easier and I don’t know if that’s just because it’s done by the year 2 teachers kind of with their kids in mind.”.
(Teacher C, final interview)
These accounts evidence teachers’ sensitive and nuanced adjustments to programme content, resonating with the work of Thompson and Wiliam (2008), Lendrum and Humphrey (2012), and Sims et al. (2021) regarding the potential value of creating space for small alterations to better meet the needs of pupils and contrasting with previous studies maintaining the importance of maintaining absolute fidelity to original design (O’Donnell, 2008).
Teachers’ positive experiences led some to signal their intention to continue using Stepping Stones beyond the conclusion of the programme. For example, five teachers indicated they would very likely continue using the programme in the future (post-programme survey, Q25). In some cases, teachers also indicated that their experiences of scaffolded planning through the fading programme encouraged them to plan additional weeks of the programme, beyond the end of the formal teaching period.
“I am going to continue this in summer term and plan and prepare my own slides. I am planning on using the programme with next year’s year 2 cohort too”
(post-programme survey, Q25a)
Similarly, participating teachers reported interest from wider colleagues and that, in some cases, colleagues in other year groups were seeking to introduce mathematical problem solving sessions based upon the Stepping Stones model.
“I’ve had an ECT and another staff member observe from our school and the ECT was from year four and she thinks that she would like to roll it out in year four as a quick 10 min. Let’s identify the key facts and things. So I’m in discussions with our SLT at the minute to see whether or not we might be able to do it across the school like say three times a week”.
(Teacher S, final interview)
“I think having a conversation with [Maths Lead] because she’s been really, really excited by it and she’s been thinking about how she can do things like it with Year 6. She’s been having a go”.
(Teacher C, final interview)

3.3. The Value of Collaboration

References to the value of collaboration were common to all teachers. This encompassed the impact of collaboration in developing understanding of Stepping Stones, echoing findings from C. D. Allen and Penuel (2015) regarding the link between collaboration and sense making. Teachers’ accounts also suggested that opportunities for collaboration and co-construction enhanced relationships between teachers and between teachers and the research team, with some indication that this may have been encouraged by the fading design, which incorporated opportunities for authentic and open involvement in collaborative planning and reflection as a core focus of PD sessions.
“The collaboration was so useful because I felt sometimes like I wasn’t as valuable as the other teachers, but only because it was… It was really nice to hear experienced teachers’ feedback on the way they do maths or the way they do something else and it was nice to even make our own and sit with somebody else and say like, ‘I would have done it this way with my class’. But they’ve done it a completely different way and it was really nice to hear that different ways of doing things in maths. I thought that was so useful. When I came back to my headteacher I said, ‘Ohh, it was amazing’”.
(Teacher J, final interview)
“both of you were absolutely brilliant in your delivery and you kind of opened it up to the floor more than enough because sometimes on some courses that you go to you get talked to a lot. And although they ask for questions, they don’t really want me to answer, they want to answer themselves, whereas I think this was very much you really wanted to hear what we wanted to do and really wanted us to get involved”.
(Teacher C, final interview)
Teachers particularly endorsed the decision to undertake collaborative planning sessions in-person, rather than online, supporting the work of McConnell et al. (2013) regarding teachers’ preference for face-to-face formats for PD activities (McConnell et al., 2013; Moore et al., 2023).
“I know we did talk about the planning stage where that might have been better just at school, but it wasn’t. It was lovely to be altogether and share our ideas, so I did think the group discussions and the group meetings were brilliant”.
(Teacher S, final interview)
Accounts from some teachers suggested that this preference for in-person collaboration may stem from the availability of support from the research team.
“Really good opportunity to plan with K present for support”.
(Training session 3 feedback)
“The two sessions we had with K and W really helped break down the planning process”.
(post-survey, 8a)
This connects with the findings of E. Perry and Booth (2024) regarding the importance of the facilitator within professional learning, alongside helping to address the current absence of studies providing examples of facilitated activity when working with groups of teachers identified by Hadar and Brody (2021).
Teachers acknowledged time as both a barrier and an enabler of successful collaboration. Overall, the timing and sequencing of PD sessions were endorsed by teachers, suggesting that this was well-balanced to ensure appropriate opportunities to apply new techniques in practice before gathering together to share feedback and engage with ongoing support both from peers and the research team.
“I think the frequency of seeing each other actually was enough to not need anything else. Because we’ve seen you probably once a month, I think I felt like that if we did have a problem then we knew how to reach you […] There’s a lot of work gone into it, you weren’t just handed it and said go and have a go because it was like the sessions where we caught up fell nicely within a week of us being able to give it a go, get back, feedback. Then the sessions we planned together. They weren’t immediately the week after. So, then that fed into those weeks and then we came back together again”.
(Teacher K, final interview)
However, particularly during the final PD session—when teachers were required to collaboratively plan weeks 9 and 10 of the programme without the scaffolding from partially completed materials—teachers expressed a desire for more time to ensure that all resources were completed.
“you and W were both there supporting and any questions we had you were there to help and support. I think it was just the time, just the amount of time for it, you know. You gave us materials, like all of the things that you use like the puzzles and the problems. […] I don’t think we could have had any more support really. I think it was just the time”.
(Teacher T, final interview)
The need for increased planning time as the programme materials faded and teachers assumed greater responsibility for the preparation of resources was also evident from the research team’s review of teacher-generated materials. This demonstrated that both session slides and teacher scripts for the final week of the programme—week 10—were incomplete due to insufficient time to complete these collaboratively during the final PD session. However, teachers’ responses demonstrated varying perceptions of the reasons why additional time was needed, including the need for greater time to think through the mathematical content of problems and to navigate the online software used to create pupil-facing resources.
“me and my partner didn’t get the second slides done. So we only got the one session done. So I do think a little bit more time to plan both of them in that session would have been useful for me to break it down and think about it myself”
(Teacher J, final interview)
“I think time one is a tricky one because you could have given us longer and some people could have absolutely flown through it. Or you could have gave us less time and some people still would have done it. I think it depends on more the computing skills than the mathematical skills and process, and I think probably the people that got through the problems quicker were the ones that were more confident using the software, they make animations and things like that rather than the actual maths behind it. I think the only issue that I had was the Wi-Fi […] because that was making the collaborative document really slow and cumbersome”.
(Teacher C, final interview)
Whilst it is perhaps important to note that Teacher J was less experienced than other participants, this perhaps reflects—as Teacher C observes—the different pace at which different individuals work and the challenges incurred through attempting to gauge this when working across groups.

4. Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore a fading programme design in the context of teacher PD. This article, therefore, contributes to the literature by providing an account of the original use of gradually fading worked examples to scaffold teachers’ understanding and application of metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving. The use of worked examples in our study was multi-layered, employed both as a pedagogic strategy to support pupils directly within sessions and, concurrently, through the broader Stepping Stones model whereby the programme materials themselves—including teacher scripts and pupil-facing session slides—formed a worked example for teachers to build familiarity with these techniques in action before gradually fading this support over the final weeks of the programme. The data provided a nuanced consideration of how Stepping Stones and the associated PD offer impacted the teachers involved over the duration of delivery, focusing upon a central research question: how did the Stepping Stones programme influence teachers’ understanding and use of metacognitive strategies for mathematical problem solving? Further exploration considered two sub-questions: (a) did teachers’ deployment of metacognitive strategies vary as the programme faded and (b) how did teachers perceive the acceptability of the fading programme design? In this section, we will discuss our results with reference to the wider literature.
Previous research notes the positive impact of metacognitive strategies upon competency for mathematics and mathematical problem solving (Donovan & Bransford, 2005; Hodgen et al., 2018; Muijs & Bokhove, 2020; Mulholland, 2021; Woodward et al., 2012) but also cautions that the existing guidance and PD for teachers is insufficient (Boylan et al., 2023; Department for Education, 2024b), leading to weak implementation (Jossberger et al., 2010; Muijs & Bokhove, 2020). This study addresses these issues by demonstrating that adoption of the fading design inherent within Stepping Stones led to demonstrable gains in teachers’ knowledge, confidence, and application of metacognitive strategies. This was evident across multiple datasets, including teachers’ self-reported perceptions, the research teams’ observations of programme delivery, and our review of participant-generated programme materials as teachers’ assumed greater responsibility for planning Stepping Stones sessions during the final four weeks of delivery. Furthermore, our results suggest that this fading approach was acceptable to teachers, leading to a sense of ownership and buy in, which encouraged teachers’ participation in, and delivery of, Stepping Stones, alongside sensitive adjustments to tailor programme content according to the needs of pupils. We, therefore, advocate further examination of this fading PD model to explore its potential in serving as a template for teacher PD for mathematical problem solving, as well as possible applications in broader contexts.
The fading design deployed within this study was a deliberate choice, designed to support teachers’ knowledge and understanding in reference to the existing understanding of mechanisms for effective teacher PD (Cordingley et al., 2015; Sims et al., 2021). We believe that our results endorse its use in this novel context, resonating with wider research surrounding the use of fading worked examples within cognitive load theory (Sweller et al., 2019) and the design of professional learning in other disciplines, such as health (Fraser et al., 2015). Worked examples are an effective means of scaffolding learning, reducing cognitive load, and supporting skill acquisition (Kirschner et al., 2006; Renkl, 2014; Sweller et al., 2019), and this was the case in our study. Teachers’ accounts indicated that worked examples enhanced understanding of the programme format and structure but also metacognition and how to teach it in relation to problem solving in mathematics.
Stepping Stones first focused upon building requisite knowledge of research around metacognition and mathematical problem solving within the initial PD session for teachers. Teachers’ accounts endorsed this approach, indicating that this increased motivation for participation. These accounts resonate with the existing literature, reinforcing the value of presenting information from credible sources when seeking to influence behaviour change (Sims et al., 2021), as well as underscoring the need for clear communication of research evidence to build necessary understanding to appropriately translate and apply findings into practice (Cain, 2015; Farley-Ripple et al., 2022; Sims et al., 2021). Additionally, teachers’ knowledge was enhanced through engagement with programme materials, which were specifically designed as worked examples, modelling the desired metacognitive strategies within the first six weeks of the programme before fading throughout the final four weeks of delivery.
The success of this approach, evident in teachers’ later application of these same strategies in their own planning, reinforces the existing literature regarding the importance of modelling and rehearsal to develop specific teacher techniques (Sims et al., 2021). As well as this, managing cognitive load effectively through this faded programme ensured that teachers assimilated new knowledge without feeling overwhelmed, enabling them to gradually transition to independent application (Paas & Ayres, 2014; van Merriënboer & Sweller, 2010) and to grow in confidence, even as the support offered to them reduced. Our results also connect with extensive findings regarding the importance of repetition in habit formation and sustaining behaviour change, both specifically for teacher PD (Sims et al., 2021), as well as for behaviour change more generally (Michie et al., 2014).
Teachers’ accounts endorse the emphasis upon collaboration when designing the future programme materials, resonating with findings evident in the wider literature regarding the utility of peer support between teachers in both implementing and sustaining new pedagogical approaches (Miquel & Duran, 2017). Teachers found collaboration—both among themselves and with members of the research team—valuable in deepening their understanding and use of metacognitive strategies employed within the Stepping Stones programme. This is reminiscent of the findings of C. D. Allen and Penuel (2015) on the role of peer collaboration in sense making and echoes broader research emphasising the pivotal role of developing understanding of the underpinning rationale for changes as part of effective PD (Cordingley et al., 2015).
Collaborative planning appeared to play a particularly critical role in the fading model, as teachers transitioned from guided implementation to independent application and adaptation. By co-constructing materials, teachers not only reinforced their understanding but also developed a sense of ownership over the approach, which has been linked to greater application of teaching techniques (Miquel & Duran, 2017; Sims et al., 2021). This finding is of particular interest, as it contrasts with previous studies maintaining the importance of absolute fidelity to the original design (O’Donnell, 2008) to avoid lethal mutations, which may lessen its effectiveness. Instead, our work demonstrates that teachers adhered to core programme principles, whilst also adapting to accommodate contextual factors (Lendrum & Humphrey, 2012; Thompson & Wiliam, 2008). This nuanced adaptation to contextual factors could explain why certain planning items did not significantly change as the programme faded. Essentially, teachers perceived them as being fundamental to their planning and sine qua non as part of their planning repertoires. This study, therefore, highlights the importance of promoting ownership to encourage intelligent adaptation of programme content and delivery whilst maintaining alignment to central design principles.
It is important to acknowledge that, in this study, collaborative planning was curtailed by insufficient time to develop scripts and session slides for the final week of delivery, meaning that these remained incomplete. This highlights the importance of allocating adequate protected time for collaborative activity and reinforces the wider literature, which emphasises that innovation can be inhibited or that PD for teachers can often fail to become successfully embedded due to a lack of time within already extensive workload demands (Cordingley et al., 2015; Creagh et al., 2023; Sims et al., 2021). To address this issue, in future iterations of Stepping Stones, we propose extending both the duration of PD sessions to increase the amount of time allocated for collaborative planning and the length of the programme itself.

4.1. Limitations

Although the results from this pilot study indicate positive impacts for participating teachers, as with all similar studies, they should be interpreted with some caution. Future research should build upon these results through further empirical work to explore the impacts of Stepping Stones across a larger sample of teachers and schools to better understand the learning outcomes resulting from this innovative fading model.
Second, whilst we have taken steps to carefully interpret the multiple sources of data, the analytical procedures run on the quantitative data have some limitations. For example, the Goodman–Kruskal gamma test (Goodman & Kruskal, 1954) looks for associations of tied data and cannot determine causality. Research with larger samples will inevitably have more capacity to run parametric tests with greater power that can detect causality in carefully designed studies. Future research may consider adopting experimental designs with specific controls to determine which metacognitive strategies are preferred amongst teachers.
Third, we recognise the importance of having access to complete data. Due to teacher time constraints, data collected within this project included data from three out of four weeks of the fading phase; therefore, any future programme scaling would benefit from a consistent availability of data to include all weeks of programme delivery. We also contend that the fading design for PD may have wider applications beyond the context of metacognition and mathematical problem solving, and, therefore, advocate further exploration of this to scaffold teachers’ understanding and use of additional pedagogic approaches and in broader subject contexts.

4.2. Conclusions

In exploring the design and implementation of an innovative, fading model of teachers’ PD, this original study aims to enhance our understanding and application of metacognitive strategies in mathematical problem solving. It offers a novel, insightful contribution to the existing body of evidence that is often overlooked and under-represented. Overall, our results demonstrate that, when combined with peer collaboration, our fading design was associated with improved knowledge and confidence for both metacognition and mathematical problem solving, alongside increased ownership and buy in. We, therefore, conclude that wider adoption of Stepping Stones provides a means of encouraging sustained behaviour change, prompting more frequent use of metacognitive strategies with increasing independence. Future research should build upon these results through empirical work to further explore the value of the Stepping Stones model—including in wider curriculum contexts—thereby extending our understanding of effective PD design.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, K.M., W.G.; methodology, K.M., W.G. and C.C.; formal analysis, K.M., C.C., W.G. and D.N.; investigation, K.M. and W.G.; resources, K.M. and W.G.; data curation, K.M. and C.C.; writing—original draft preparation, K.M., W.G., C.C. and D.N.; writing—review and editing, K.M., W.G., C.C. and D.N.; supervision, K.M.; project administration, K.M.; funding acquisition, K.M. and W.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Education Endowment Foundation. The APC was funded by Northumbria University.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Northumbria University (project number 10332, 18 May 2025).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the Education Endowment Foundation (EEF).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders of this project had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Agresti, A. (2012). Categorical data analysis (3rd ed.). Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen, C. D., & Penuel, W. R. (2015). Studying teachers’ sensemaking to investigate teachers’ responses to professional development focused on new standards. Journal of Teacher Education, 66(2), 136–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Allen, J. P., Hafen, C. A., Gregory, A. C., Mikami, A. Y., & Pianta, R. (2015). Enhancing secondary school instruction and student achievement: Replication and extension of the my teaching partner-secondary intervention. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 8(4), 475–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Allen, K. D., & Hancock, T. E. (2008). Reading comprehension improvement with individualized cognitive profiles and metacognition. Literacy Research and Instruction, 47, 124–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Asterhan, C. S. C., & Lefstein, A. (2024). The search for evidence-based features of effective teacher professional development: A critical analysis of the literature. Professional Development in Education, 50(1), 11–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 181–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bhaskar, R. (1978). On the possibility of social scientific knowledge and the limits of naturalism. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 8(1), 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Boaler, J. (2006). How a detracked mathematics approach promoted respect, responsibility, and high achievement. Theory into Practice, 45(1), 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Boulay, A.-M., Bare, J., Benini, L., Berger, M., Lathuillière, M. J., Manzardo, A., Margni, M., Motoshita, M., Núñez, M., Pastor, A. V., Ridoutt, B., Oki, T., Worbe, S., & Pfister, S. (2018). The wulca consensus characterization model for water scarcity footprints: Assessing impacts of water consumption based on available water remaining (aware). The International Journal of Life Cycle Assessment, 23(2), 368–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Boylan, M., Adams, G., & Birkhead, A. (2023). Landscaping mathematics education policy: Landscaping national mathematics education policy. Sheffield Hallam University. [Google Scholar]
  11. Boylan, M., Zhu, H., Jaques, L., Birkhead, A., & Rempe-Gillen, E. (2024). Secondary maths. Education Endowment Foundation (EEF). [Google Scholar]
  12. Branigan, H., & Donaldson, D. (2020). Teachers matter for metacognition: Facilitating metacognition in the primary school through teacher-pupil interactions. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 38, 100718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2022). Thematic analysis: A practical guide. SAGE Publications Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cain, T. (2015). Teachers’ engagement with published research: Addressing the knowledge problem. The Curriculum Journal, 26(3), 488–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Centre for Education Statistics and Evaluation. (2017). Cognitive load theory: Research that teachers really need to understand. Centre for Education Statistics and Evaluation. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cordingley, P., Higgins, S., Greany, T., Buckler, N., Coles-Jordan, D., Crisp, B., Saunders, L., & Coe, R. (2015). Developing great teaching. Teacher Development Trust. [Google Scholar]
  17. Costa, A. L. (1991). The school as a home for the mind: A collection of articles. Corwin. [Google Scholar]
  18. Creagh, S., Greg, T., Nicole, M., Meghan, S., & Hogan, A. (2023). Workload, work intensification and time poverty for teachers and school leaders: A systematic research synthesis. Educational Review, 77(2), 661–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Creswell, J. W., & Creswell, J. D. (2018). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative & mixed methods approaches (5th ed.). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cross, D. R., & Paris, S. G. (1988). Developmental and instructional analyses of children’s metacognition and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(2), 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Department for Education. (2019). Early career framework. Department for Education. [Google Scholar]
  22. Department for Education. (2024a). National professional qualification (NPQ) courses. Available online: https://www.gov.uk/guidance/national-professional-qualification-npq-courses (accessed on 5 June 2025).
  23. Department for Education. (2024b). Trends in international mathematics and science study (TIMSS) 2023: National report for england (Vol. 1). Department for Education. [Google Scholar]
  24. Dignath, C., & Büttner, G. (2008). Components of fostering self-regulated learning among students. A meta-analysis on intervention studies at primary and secondary school level. Metacognition and Learning, 3(3), 231–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Donovan, M. S., & Bransford, J. D. (2005). How students learn—Science in the classroom. National Academy Press. [Google Scholar]
  26. Education Endowment Foundation. (2021a). Cognitive science approaches in the classsroom: Evidence review. Education Endowment Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  27. Education Endowment Foundation. (2021b). Metacognition and self-regulated learning: Guidance report. Education Endowment Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  28. Farley-Ripple, E. N., Tilley, K., Mead, H., Van Horne, S., & Agboh, D. (2022). How is evidence use enacted in schools? A mixed methods multiple case study of “deep-use”schools. Center for Research Use in Education. [Google Scholar]
  29. Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Fletcher-Wood, H., & Zuccollo, J. (2020). The effects of high-quality professional development on teachers and students. Education Policy Institute. [Google Scholar]
  31. Foster, C. (2019). The fundamental problem with teaching problem solving. Mathematics Teaching, 265, 8–10. [Google Scholar]
  32. Fraser, K. L., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2015). Cognitive load theory for the design of medical simulations. Simul Healthc, 10(5), 295–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Goodman, L. A., & Kruskal, W. H. (1954). Measures of association for cross classification. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 49(268), 732–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hadar, L. L., & Brody, D. L. (2021). Interrogating the role of facilitators in promoting learning in teacher educators’ professional communities. Professional Development in Education, 47(4), 599–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hennessey, M. G. (1999). Probing the dimensions of metacognition: Implications for conceptual change teaching-learning. Educational Resources Information Center. [Google Scholar]
  36. Higgins, S., Major, L. E., & Coe, R. (2014). What makes great teaching. Professional Voice, 11(3), 11–16. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hodgen, J., Foster, C., Marks, R., & Brown, M. (2018). Evidence for review of mathematics teaching: Improving mathematics in key stages two and three: Evidence review. Education Endowment Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  38. Izzati, L., & Mahmudi, A. (2018). The influence of metacognition in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 1097, 012107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Jansen, A. (2008). An investigation of relationships between seventh-grade students’ beliefs and their participation during mathematics discussions in two classrooms. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10(1), 68–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Jossberger, H., Brand-Gruwel, S., Boshuizen, H., & Wiel, M. (2010). The challenge of self-directed and self-regulated learning in vocational education: A theoretical analysis and synthesis of requirements. Journal of Vocational Education & Training, 62, 415–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kirschner, P. A., John, S., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kistner, S., Rakoczy, K., Otto, B., Dignath-van Ewijk, C., Büttner, G., & Klieme, E. (2010). Promotion of self-regulated learning in classrooms: Investigating frequency, quality, and consequences for student performance. Metacognition and Learning, 5(2), 157–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(5), 178–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Kyriacou, C., & Issitt, J. (2008). What characterises effective teacher-initiated teacher-pupil dialogue to promote conceptual understanding in mathematics lessons in England in key stages 2 and 3: A systematic review. EPPI-Centre. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lai, E. R. (2011). Critical thinking: A literature review. Pearson’s Research Reports, 6(1), 40–41. [Google Scholar]
  46. Lendrum, A., & Humphrey, N. (2012). The importance of studying the implementation of interventions in school settings. Oxford Review of Education, 38(5), 635–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Martinez, M. E. (2006). What is metacognition? Phi Delta Kappan, 87(9), 696–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. McConnell, T., Parker, J., Eberhardt, J., Koehler, M., & Lundeberg, M. (2013). Virtual professional learning communities: Teachers’ perceptions of virtual versus face-to-face professional development. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 22, 267–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Michie, S., Atkins, L., & West, R. (2014). The behaviour change wheel: A guide to designing interventions. Silverback Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  50. Miquel, E., & Duran, D. (2017). Peer learning network: Implementing and sustaining cooperative learning by teacher collaboration. Journal of Education for Teaching, 43(3), 349–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Moore, B., Legleiter, E., Owens, K., Packard, B., & Wright, J. (2023). Evaluating the effectiveness of an online professional development for rural middle-school science teachers. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 35, 386–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Muijs, D., & Bokhove, C. (2020). Metacognition and self-regulation: Evidence review. Education Endowment Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  53. Mulholland, K. (2021). Using pupil views to uncover evidence of children’s metacognition in mathematics. International Journal of Student Voice, 8, 1–35. [Google Scholar]
  54. Mulholland Shipley, K. (2016). “I think when I work with other people I can let go of all of my ideas and tell them out loud”: The impact of a thinking skills approach upon pupils’ experiences of maths. Newcastle University. [Google Scholar]
  55. Muncer, G., Higham, P. A., Gosling, C. J., Cortese, S., Wood-Downie, H., & Hadwin, J. A. (2022). A meta-analysis investigating the association between metacognition and math performance in adolescence. Educational Psychology Review, 34(1), 301–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Nowell, L. S., Norris, J. M., White, D. E., & Moules, N. J. (2017). Thematic analysis:Striving to meet the trustworthiness criteria. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 16(1), 1609406917733847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. O’Donnell, C. L. (2008). Defining, conceptualizing, and measuring fidelity of implementation and its relationship to outcomes in k–12 curriculum intervention research. Review of Educational Research, 78(1), 33–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ofsted. (2021). Research review series: Mathematics. Ofsted. [Google Scholar]
  59. Ofsted. (2023). Coordinating mathematical success: The mathematics subject report. Ofsted. [Google Scholar]
  60. Opfer, V. D., & Pedder, D. (2010). Access to continuous professional development by teachers in England. The Curriculum Journal, 21(4), 453–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Paas, F., Alexander, R., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Paas, F., & Ayres, P. (2014). Cognitive load theory: A broader view on the role of memory in learning and education. Educational Psychology Review, 26(2), 191–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Perry, E., & Booth, J. (2024). The practices of professional development facilitators. Professional Development in Education, 50(1), 144–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Perry, J., Lundie, D., & Golder, G. (2019). Metacognition in schools: What does the literature suggest about the effectiveness of teaching metacognition in schools? Educational Review, 71(4), 483–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Perry, V., Albeg, L., & Tung, C. (2012). Meta-analysis of single-case design research on self-regulatory interventions for academic performance. Journal of Behavioral Education, 21(3), 217–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Picker, S. H., & Berry, J. S. (2000). Investigating pupils’ images of mathematicians. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 43(1), 65–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Popova, A., Evans, D. K., Breeding, M. E., & Arancibia, V. (2021). Teacher professional development around the world: The gap between evidence and practice. The World Bank Research Observer, 37(1), 107–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Renkl, A. (2014). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38(1), 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skill acquisition: A cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist, 38, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Robson, C. (2002). Real world research: A resource for social scientists and practitioner-researchers. Blackwell Publishers Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  71. Sancar, R., Atal, D., & Deryakulu, D. (2021). A new framework for teachers’ professional development. Teaching and Teacher Education, 101, 103305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Schön, D. A. (1987). Educating the reflective practitioner: Toward a new design for teaching and learning in the professions. Jossey-Bass. [Google Scholar]
  73. Schraw, G. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. Instructional Science, 26(1), 113–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Schwartz, D. L., Chase, C., Chin, D. B., Oppezzo, M., Kwong, H., Okita, S., Biswas, G., Roscoe, R., Jeong, H., & Wagster, J. (2009). Interactive metacognition: Monitoring and regulating a teachable agent. In Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 340–358). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
  75. Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching. Educational Researcher, 15(2), 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Sims, S., Fletcher-Wood, H., O’Mara-Eves, A., Cottingham, S., Stansfield, C., Van Herwegen, J., & Anders, J. (2021). What are the characteristics of effective teacher professional development? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Education Endowment Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  77. Stein, M. K., Engle, R. A., Smith, M. S., & Hughes, E. K. (2008). Orchestrating productive mathematical discussions: Five practices for helping teachers move beyond show and tell. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10(4), 313–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Stillman, G. (2011). Applying metacognitive knowledge and strategies in applications and modelling tasks at secondary school. In G. Kaiser, W. Blum, R. B. Ferri, & G. Stillman (Eds.), Trends in teaching and learning of mathematical modelling (pp. 165–180). Springer. [Google Scholar]
  79. Sweller, J., Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 261–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. The Sutton Trust. (2011). Improving the impact of teachers on pupil achievement in the UK—Interim findings. The Sutton Trust. [Google Scholar]
  81. Thompson, M., & Wiliam, D. (2008). Tight but loose: A conceptual framework for scaling up school reforms (pp. 1–44). Educational Testing Service. [Google Scholar]
  82. van Merriënboer, J. J., & Sweller, J. (2010). Cognitive load theory in health professional education: Design principles and strategies. Medical Education, 44(1), 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Veenman, M. V. J., Wilhelm, P., & Beishuizen, J. J. (2004). The relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Instruction, 14(1), 89–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Villegas-Reimers, E. (2003). Teacher professional development: An international review of the literature. International Institute for Educational Planning. [Google Scholar]
  86. Vula, E., Avdyli, R., Berisha, V., Saqipi, B., & Elezi, S. (2017). The impact of metacognitive strategies and self-regulating processes of solving math word problems. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 10, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Wall, K. (2008). Understanding metacognition through the use of pupil views templates: Pupil views of learning to learn. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3(1), 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Wall, K., & Hall, E. (2016). Teachers as metacognitive role models. European Journal of Teacher Education, 39(4), 403–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Webb, S., Massey, D., Goggans, M., & Flajole, K. (2019). Thirty-five years of the gradual release of responsibility: Scaffolding toward complex and responsive teaching. The Reading Teacher, 73(1), 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Westwood, P. (2011). The problem with problems: Potential difficulties in implementing problem-based learning as the core method in primary school mathematics. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 16(1), 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Whitebread, D., Coltman, P., Jameson, H., & Lander, R. (2009). Play, cognition and self-regulation: What exactly are children learning when they learn through play? Educational and Child Psychology, 26, 40–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Wilson, N., & Bai, H. (2010). The relationships and impact of teachers’ metacognitive knowledge and pedagogical understandings of metacognition. Metacognition and Learning, 5, 269–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Woodward, J., Beckmann, S., Driscoll, M., Franke, M., Herzig, P., Jitendra, A., Koedinger, K. R., & Ogbuehi, P. (2012). Improving mathematical problem solving in grades 4 through 8: A practice guide. U.S. Department of Education. [Google Scholar]
  94. Wright, D., Taverner, S., & Flitney, G. (2008). Thinking through mathematics. Chris Kington. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Annotated Stepping Stones programme slides.
Figure 1. Annotated Stepping Stones programme slides.
Education 15 00892 g001
Figure 2. Structured reflection.
Figure 2. Structured reflection.
Education 15 00892 g002
Figure 3. Programme for professional development.
Figure 3. Programme for professional development.
Education 15 00892 g003
Figure 4. The fading process.
Figure 4. The fading process.
Education 15 00892 g004
Figure 5. Analytical procedures.
Figure 5. Analytical procedures.
Education 15 00892 g005
Figure 6. Phases two to five, from initial codes to final themes.
Figure 6. Phases two to five, from initial codes to final themes.
Education 15 00892 g006
Table 1. Data collection.
Table 1. Data collection.
DataFurther Information
Pre-, mid-, and post-programme surveysTeachers’ self-reported views and understanding of metacognition, problem solving, and wider learning in mathematics, as well as their feelings, confidence, and opinions.
Training session feedback survey Feedback regarding teachers’ perceptions of their learning and experiences following each of the three PD sessions.
Observations during school visitObservation notes from visits to each school during the second or third weeks of delivery. Visits were made by one member of our research team to observe a Stepping Stones session and a short (20–30 min) discussion regarding implementation. Observations were structured using a checklist of key programme features, as well as a series of questions to guide an informal discussion with teachers.
Review of teacher-generated materialsAnalysis of programme materials (pupil-facing session slides and teacher scripts) produced by teachers during the fading stages of the programme (weeks 7–10). Analysis was structured using a checklist of key programme features and was undertaken by members of the research team.
InterviewsSemi-structured interviews, conducted within two weeks following the conclusion of the programme. These were approximately 30 min in duration and took place via Microsoft Teams. The Microsoft Teams transcription function was used to generate a written record of these discussions. An audio recording was also taken and used to cleanse transcripts to produce an accurate record of conversations.
Table 2. Frequency count and ranks of programme implementation checklist items.
Table 2. Frequency count and ranks of programme implementation checklist items.
Checklist StrategyTotal Count and Implementation %Mean Rank
15–20 min session delivered, outside of the daily maths lesson15 (100%) 8.50
Programme slides used15 (100%)8.50
Problem solving objective shared with the class14 (93%)6.50
Mixed-attaining trios are in place for discussion14 (93%)6.50
Expectations for discussion are clearly communicated to children13 (87%)4.50
Strategies for problem solving—in addition to mathematical content alone—are a key focus of the session13 (87%)4.50
Teacher overview used to inform questioning and discussion within the session11 (73%)3.00
“River” used9 (60%)2.00
Alternating worked example used8 (53%)1.00
Table 3. Goodman and Kruskal gamma test results of the teacher-generated materials checklist.
Table 3. Goodman and Kruskal gamma test results of the teacher-generated materials checklist.
Checklist ItemTotal Frequency Count
(Weeks 7–9)
MissingBrief Information IncludedDetailed Information
Included
Γp
Learning intentions, including both National Curriculum and problem solving objectives250 (0%)2 (22.2%)7 (77.8%)−0.8570.040 *
Teacher overview, including script220 (0%)5 (55.6%)4 (44.4%)−1.000.01 **
Teacher overview includes the use of Think Aloud 211 (11.1%)4 (44.4%)4 (44.4%)−0.7500.01 **
Alternating worked example identified as an optional extension261 (11.1%)0 (0%)8 (89.9%)0.0001.00
Teacher overview details key question prompts260 (0%)1 (11.1%)8 (89.9%)−1.000.225
Worked example uses complete/mistakes/fading as appropriate260 (0%)1 (11.1%)8 (89.9%)−1.000.225
Visual representations are used to support the understanding of key mathematical concepts260 (0%)1 (11.1%)8 (89.9%)−1.000.225
Worked example included in session slides includes reference to/exemplification of the problem solving objectives taught during the session260 (0%)1 (11.1%)8 (89.9%)−1.000.225
* p < 0.05 ** p < 0.01.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mulholland, K.; Gray, W.; Counihan, C.; Nichol, D. Stepping Stones: Adopting a Fading Programme Design to Promote Teachers’ Use of Metacognitive Strategies for Mathematical Problem Solving. Educ. Sci. 2025, 15, 892. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15070892

AMA Style

Mulholland K, Gray W, Counihan C, Nichol D. Stepping Stones: Adopting a Fading Programme Design to Promote Teachers’ Use of Metacognitive Strategies for Mathematical Problem Solving. Education Sciences. 2025; 15(7):892. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15070892

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mulholland, Kirstin, William Gray, Christopher Counihan, and David Nichol. 2025. "Stepping Stones: Adopting a Fading Programme Design to Promote Teachers’ Use of Metacognitive Strategies for Mathematical Problem Solving" Education Sciences 15, no. 7: 892. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15070892

APA Style

Mulholland, K., Gray, W., Counihan, C., & Nichol, D. (2025). Stepping Stones: Adopting a Fading Programme Design to Promote Teachers’ Use of Metacognitive Strategies for Mathematical Problem Solving. Education Sciences, 15(7), 892. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15070892

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop