Enhancing Form–Meaning Connections in the Language Teaching of Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from Two Teaching Interventions
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Passives in Typical and Atypical Language Acquisition
1.2. Teaching the Passive in Children with DLD
1.3. FonF Grammar Teaching Techniques: The Case of Processing Instruction and Dictogloss
1.3.1. PI
1.3.2. Dictogloss
1.3.3. (FonF) Teaching Interventions in Greek
1.4. Goals and Research Questions of the Current Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
- (a)
- Children should have been officially diagnosed with DLD by developmental-behavioural pediatricians, and by qualified speech and language therapists in the absence of autism, intellectual disability, deafness, etc. (Bishop et al., 2017);
- (b)
- They should have been monolingual native speakers of Greek;
- (c)
- They should have been between the third and fifth grade of primary school (8–11 years old), given that Greek passives are acquired towards the end of the second grade of primary school in typically developing children (Paspali et al., 2024a).
2.2. Materials and Procedure
2.2.1. Processing Instruction Protocol
2.2.2. Dictogloss Protocol
2.2.3. Evaluation
2.2.4. Data Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Control Group
3.2. PI and Dictogloss Groups
3.2.1. Comprehension Task
3.2.2. Production Task
3.2.3. SR Task—Accuracy Scores
3.2.4. SR Task—Structural Scores
4. Discussion
Pedagogical Implications and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
PI | Processing Instruction |
CI(s) | Confidence Interval(s) |
SD | Standard Deviation |
1 | We would like to note that this is not an acquisition study; this is a teaching intervention study. Measuring overall deficits in language and cognitive abilities in children with DLD (compared to typically developing children) is beyond its scope. The crucial measurement, in terms of the aims and research questions of the study, was children’s performance on passives before the teaching intervention, which was measured in the pre-test. Furthermore, 14 out of the 23 children in the present study had been tested in a series of (non-)standardized language and cognitive measures approximately 18 months before the data collection of the present study. These children exhibited significantly lower vocabulary, grammar, and working memory skills compared to their age-matched monolingual peers but similar non-verbal intelligence (Paspali et al., 2024b). Crucially, no differences were detected in the accuracy scores of the pre-test between those 14 children and the rest of the children in the present study (all p-values > 0.05). |
2 | The sentences used in the pre- and (delayed) post-tests were carefully constructed to be equivalent in terms of syntactic complexity, vocabulary, and length. As already mentioned in the manuscript, all sentences were created in advance and then randomly assigned to the three tests to avoid repetition of the same items and minimize item-specific bias across time points. Furthermore, generalized linear mixed-effects models (GLMMs) with sentence (item) included as a random effect were employed, which statistically accounts for any residual variation in sentence difficulty. This ensures that observed differences in accuracy across the three time points reflect actual changes in performance rather than differences in the difficulty of the test items. |
3 | One reviewer asks whether any inter-rater reliability procedures were implemented among teachers delivering the interventions. We would like to clarify that the teachers were not involved in scoring or evaluating the children’s performance. Their role was limited to delivering the intervention using one of the two protocols with identical instructions (which were provided to them both orally and in written format) across the participants after the training they received. Thus, inter-rater reliability procedures were not applicable/relevant at the intervention stage. Notice that children are not evaluated on the teaching materials during the teaching intervention. They are evaluated on the testing materials. Hence, the crucial part of the teaching intervention, is not how accurately the child will respond but to learn through the materials and correct their errors together with the instructor, which further promotes noticing (Swain, 2000) of the target structure. As already noted in the manuscript, the testing sessions were conducted by the researcher, not the teachers. However, we acknowledge that variability in implementation could still affect internal validity. Thus, several standardization procedures were implemented. As part of their four-hour training session and detailed written guidelines (see Materials and Procedure), each teacher completed two pilot sessions: one with another teacher and one with a typically developing child (aged 9–10) acting in the student role. These sessions were conducted under observation by the researcher, who provided immediate feedback and further clarification where necessary. Furthermore, all implementation sessions were recorded (see Materials and Procedure) and no additional guidance or feedback was required, as all sessions were delivered according to the protocol. To address any residual concerns about inter-teacher variability, an additional analysis was conducted by including teacher as a fixed effect in the primary mixed-effects model for each task, treating the teacher as a categorical factor. Incorporating the teacher as a fixed effect did not improve model fit (as indicated by ANOVA model comparisons with and without the teacher factor; p > 0.05 across all tasks). The model with the teacher also revealed no significant main or interaction effects for the teacher, suggesting that differences in instructional delivery did not confound the treatment effects. Thus, the above collectively provides evidence that any observed differences in learning outcomes can be attributed to the experimental conditions rather than to variability in instructor implementation. |
References
- Adani, F., Stegenwallner-Schütz, M., Haendler, Y., & Zukowski, A. (2016). Elicited production of relative clauses in German: Evidence from typically developing children and children with specific language impairment. First Language, 36(3), 203–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Agathopoulou, E., & Papadopoulou, D. (2014). Synchrones Prosengiseis sti didaskalia tis grammatikis tis ellinikis os defteris skepsis (Modern approaches to teaching Greek grammar as a second language). In E. Katsarou, & M. Liakopoulou (Eds.), Themata Didaskalias Kai Agogis Sto Polypolitismiko Scholeio (Teaching and education issues in the multicultural school environment). University of Macedonia. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/288182796_Synchrones_prosengiseis_ste_didaskalia_tes_grammatikes_tes_ellenikes_os_deuteres_glossas (accessed on 2 February 2025). (In Greek)
- Alexiadou, A., Anagnostopoulou, E., & Schäfer, F. (2015). External arguments in transitivity alternations. OUP Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- Andreou, M., Peristeri, E., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2022). Reference maintenance in the narratives of Albanian–Greek and Russian–Greek children with Developmental Language Disorder: A study on crosslinguistic effects. First Language, 42(2), 292–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armon-Lotem, S., Haman, E., Jensen de López, K., Smoczynska, M., Yatsushiro, K., Szczerbinski, M., van Hout, A., Dabašinskienė, I., Gavarró, A., Hobbs, E., Kamandulytė-Merfeldienė, L., Katsos, N., Kunnari, S., Nitsiou, C., Sundahl Olsen, L., Parramon, X., Sauerland, U., Torn-Leesik, R., & van der Lely, H. (2015). A large-scale cross-linguistic investigation of the acquisition of passive. Language Acquisition, 23(1), 27–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bachourou, T., Stavrakaki, S., Koukoulioti, V., & Talli, I. (2024). Cognitive vs. linguistic training in children with developmental language disorder: Exploring their effectiveness on verbal short-term memory and verbal working memory. Brain Sciences, 14(6), 580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balthazar, C. H., Ebbels, S., & Zwitserlood, R. (2020). Explicit grammatical intervention for Developmental Language Disorder: Three approaches. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 51(2), 226–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balthazar, C. H., & Scott, C. M. (2018). Targeting complex sentences in older school children with specific language impairment: Results from an early-phase treatment study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 61(3), 713–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barcroft, J., Mauzé, E., Sommers, M., Spehar, B., & Tye-Murray, N. (2025). Perception versus comprehension of bound morphemes in children who are deaf and hard of hearing: The pivotal role of form-meaning mapping. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 68(3), 1024–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron, L. S., & Arbel, Y. (2022). An implicit–explicit framework for intervention methods in Developmental Language Disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 31(4), 1557–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, D. V. M., Adams, C. V., & Rosen, S. (2006). Resistance of grammatical impairment to computerized comprehension training in children with specific and non-specific language impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(1), 19–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., & Greenhalgh, T. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 1068–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Camarata, S. M., & Nelson, K. E. (1992). Treatment efficiency as a function of target selection in the remediation of child language disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 6(3), 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chahboun, S., Wahl, H. T., Langner, J., & Vaags, A. (2024). Developmental language disorders and special educational needs: Consideration of inclusion in the Norwegian school context. Frontiers in Education, 9, 1436298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cleave, P. L., Becker, S. D., Curran, M. K., Van Horne, A. J. O., & Fey, M. E. (2015). The efficacy of recasts in language intervention: A systematic review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 24(2), 237–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delage, H., Stanford, E., Garnier, P., Oriol, E., & Morin, E. (2025). The efficiency of an explicit approach to improve complex syntax in French-speaking children with developmental language disorder: A pilot study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 41(1), 66–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dosi, I., & Gavriilidou, Z. (2020). The role of cognitive abilities in the development of definitions by children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 49(5), 761–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Du, J., Durrleman, S., He, X., & Yu, H. (2024). The repetition of passives in Mandarin-speaking children with Developmental Language Disorder and Autistic children with language impairment. Lingua, 310, 103793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ebbels, S., & van der Lely, H. (2001). Meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding for children with severe persistent SLI. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(s1), 345–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ebbels, S. H., Gadd, M., Nicoll, H., Hughes, L., Dawson, N., Burke, C., Calder, S. D., & Frizelle, P. (2024). The effectiveness of individualized morphosyntactic target identification and explicit intervention using the SHAPE CODING system for children with developmental language disorder and the impact of within-session dosage. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 55(3), 803–837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, R., Loewen, S., & Erlam, R. (2006). Implicit and explicit corrective feedback and the acquisition of L2 grammar. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28(2), 339–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finestack, L. H. (2018). Evaluation of an explicit intervention to teach novel grammatical forms to children with Developmental Language Disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(8), 2062–2075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Finestack, L. H., & Fey, M. E. (2009). Evaluation of a deductive procedure to teach grammatical inflections to children with language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 18(3), 289–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fotiadou, G., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2010). The acquisition of transitivity alternations in Greek: Does frequency count? Lingua, 120(11), 2605–2626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillam, R. B., & Hoffman, L. M. (2003). Information processing in children with specific language impairment. In L. Verhoeven, & H. van Bolkom (Eds.), Classification of developmental language disorders (pp. 149–170). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gorman, M., & Ellis, R. (2019). The relative effects of metalinguistic explanation and direct written corrective feedback on children’s grammatical accuracy in new writing. Language Teaching for Young Learners, 1(1), 57–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, J. F., & Doherty, S. (2018). Native and nonnative processing of active and passive sentences. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(4), 853–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, J. F., Malovrh, P. A., Doherty, S., & Nichols, A. (2022). A self-paced reading (SPR) study of the effects of processing instruction on the L2 processing of active and passive sentences. Language Teaching Research, 26(6), 1133–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lenth, R. (2025). emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means (R package version 1.10.7-100001). Available online: https://rvlenth.github.io/emmeans/ (accessed on 3 February 2025).
- Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Leonard, L. B., Wong, A. M.-Y., Deevy, P., Stokes, S. F., & Fletcher, P. (2006). The production of passives by children with specific language impairment: Acquiring English or Cantonese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(2), 267–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Long, M. (1991). Focus on form. In Foreign language research in cross-cultural perspective: Studies in bilingualism (pp. 39–52). John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lum, J. A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 48(9), 1138–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marinis, T., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2015). 5. Sentence Repetition. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 95–122). Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marinis, T., & Saddy, D. (2013). Parsing the passive: Comparing children with specific language impairment to sequential bilingual children. Language Acquisition, 20(2), 155–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marshall, C., Marinis, T., & van der Lely, H. (2007). Passive verb morphology: The effect of phonotactics on passive comprehension in typically developing and Grammatical-SLI children. Lingua, 117(8), 1434–1447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montgomery, J. W. (2002). Understanding the language difficulties of children with specific language impairments. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(1), 77–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montgomery, J. W., & Evans, J. L. (2009). Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 269–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montgomery, J. W., & Gillam, R. B. (2024). Introduction to the forum: Intervention with children with developmental language disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 33(2), 527–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nerantzini, M., Mastropavlou, M., Christou, T., Lekakou, M., & Zakopoulou, V. (2022). Processing voice morphology and argument structure by Greek Beginning Readers and children with Reading Difficulties. Applied Psycholinguistics, 43(1), 125–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neumann, K., Kauschke, C., Fox-Boyer, A., Lüke, C., Sallat, S., & Kiese-Himmel, C. (2024). Interventions for developmental language delay and disorders. Deutsches Ärzteblatt International, 121, 155–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olioumtsevits, K., Papadopoulou, D., & Marinis, T. (2023). Second language grammar learning in refugee children. Pedagogical Linguistics, 4(1), 50–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ouli, P. A., & Konta, E. (2025). The effectiveness of processing instruction and production-based instruction on the acquisition of the past tense in Greek by child heritage learners: A preliminary study. Ampersand, 14, 100207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Owen Van Horne, A. J., Curran, M., Larson, C., & Fey, M. E. (2018). Effects of a complexity-based approach on generalization of past tense–ed and related morphemes. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(3S), 681–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papadopoulou, D., Rothweiler, M., Tsimpli, I. M., Chilla, S., Fox-Boyer, A., Katsika, K., Mastropavlou, M., Mylonaki, A., & Stahl, N. (2009). Motion verbs in Greek and German: Evidence from typically developing and SLI children. In A. Tsangalidis (Ed.), Selected papers from the 18th ISTAL (pp. 289–299). Monochromia. [Google Scholar]
- Papadopoulou, D., Zmijanjac, K., & Agathopoulou, E. (2010). Apoktisi tis symfonias ousiastikou-epithetou stin elliniki os G2: Sygritiki meleti dyo didaktikon paremvaseon (Acquisition of noun-adjective agreement in Greek as a second language: A comparative study of two instructional interventions). Studies in Greek Linguistics, 30, 487–502. Available online: https://ins.web.auth.gr/images/MEG_PLIRI/MEG_30_487_502.pdf (accessed on 2 February 2025). (In Greek).
- Paspali, A., Marinis, T., & Alexiadou, A. (2024a). When morphology is not enough: The acquisition of voice in monolingual Greek children and bilingual children with Greek as a heritage language. Language Acquisition, 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paspali, A., Papadopoulou, D., Marinis, T., Triantafylla, A., Kyriakidou, R., & Alexiadou, A. (2024b). Sentence repetition in Greek children with developmental language disorder [Manuscript submitted for publication, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, University of Konstanz, Humboldt University of Berlin & Leibniz-Centre General Linguistics].
- Paspali, A., Papadopoulou, D., Marinis, T., Varlokosta, S., & Alexiadou, A. (2024c, September 27). Non-active voice in Greek children with developmental language disorder [Oral presentation]. 12th European Congress of Speech and Language Therapy, Bruges, Belgium. [Google Scholar]
- Plante, E., Mettler, H. M., Tucci, A., & Vance, R. (2019). Maximizing Treatment Efficiency in Developmental Language Disorder: Positive Effects in Half the Time. American journal of speech-language pathology, 28(3), 1233–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poll, G. H., Betz, S. K., & Miller, C. A. (2010). Identification of clinical markers of specific language impairment in adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 414–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Qin, J. (2008). The effect of processing instruction and dictogloss tasks on acquisition of the English passive voice. Language Teaching Research, 12(1), 61–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 4 January 2023).
- Rice, M. L., & Blossom, M. (2013). What do children with specific language impairment do with multiple forms of DO? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(1), 222–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Santamaria, K. D., Muñoz, M. L., Atkins, J. L., Hobbs, D. R., & O’Donald, K. (2013). A preliminary investigation into the application of processing instruction as therapy for aphasia in Spanish speakers. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(4), 338–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shintani, N. (2015). The effectiveness of processing instruction and production-based instruction on L2 grammar acquisition: A meta-analysis. Applied Linguistics, 36(3), 306–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simard, D. (2009). Differential effects of textual enhancement formats on intake. System, 37(1), 124–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sousa, L. P. d. Q., Tiraboschi, F. F., Lago, N. A. d., & Figueiredo, F. J. Q. d. (2019). Collaborative English language learning: Some reflections from interactions between pairs. Trabalhos Em Linguística Aplicada, 58(1), 259–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In S. Gass, & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 235–253). Newbury House. [Google Scholar]
- Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through collaborative dialogue. In J. P. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning (pp. 97–114). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Talli, I., & Stavrakaki, S. (2020). Short-term memory, working memory and linguistic abilities in bilingual children with Developmental Language Disorder. First Language, 40(4), 437–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thornbury, S. (1997). Reformulation and reconstruction: Tasks that promote “noticing”. ELT Journal, 51(4), 326–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsimpli, I. M., Peristeri, E., & Andreou, M. (2016). Narrative production in monolingual and bilingual children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 195–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41(3), 399–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van der Lely, H. K. J. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs. adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua, 98(4), 243–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- VanPatten, B. (1996). Input processing and grammar instruction: Theory and research. Ablex. [Google Scholar]
- VanPatten, B. (2002). Processing instruction: An update. Language Learning, 52(4), 755–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- VanPatten, B., & Cadierno, T. (1993). Explicit instruction and input processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 225–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wada, R., Gillam, S. L., & Gillam, R. B. (2020). The use of structural priming and focused recasts to facilitate the production of subject- and object-focused relative clauses by school-age children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(4), 1883–1895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willis, D., & Willis, J. (2007). Doing task-based teaching. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wong, W. (2024). Thirty years of processing instruction and structured input. In W. Wong, & J. Barcroft (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and input processing. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Zwitserlood, R., Wijnen, F., van Weerdenburg, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). “MetaTaal”: Enhancing complex syntax in children with specific language impairment—A metalinguistic and multimodal approach. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 50(3), 273–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Week 1 | Week 2 | Week 3 | Week 5 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Group | ||||
Control group | pre-test | - | post-test | - |
PI | pre-test | PI teaching protocol 2 sessions | PI teaching protocol 2 sessions + post-test | delayed post-test |
Dictogloss | pre-test | D teaching protocol 2 sessions | D teaching protocol 2 sessions + post-test | delayed post-test |
Pre-Test | Post-Test | Delayed Post-Test | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Task | Group | |||
Comprehension task | Control group | 0.46 (0.51) | 0.42 (0.50) | 0.29 (0.46) |
Dictogloss | 0.61 (0.49) | 0.88 (0.33) | 0.76 (0.43) | |
PI | 0.54 (0.50) | 0.76 (0.43) | 0.65 (0.48) | |
Production task | Control group | 0.38 (0.50) | 0.25 (0.44) | 0.33 (0.48) |
Dictogloss | 0.48 (0.50) | 0.94 (0.24) | 0.86 (0.35) | |
PI | 0.50 (0.50) | 0.81 (0.40) | 0.78 (0.42) | |
SR—accuracy scores | Control group | 0.25 (0.44) | 0.12 (0.34) | 0.21 (0.42) |
Dictogloss | 0.24 (0.43) | 0.29 (0.46) | 0.19 (0.39) | |
PI | 0.21 (0.41) | 0.46 (0.50) | 0.46 (0.50) | |
SR—structural scores | Control group | 0.38 (0.50) | 0.38 (0.50) | 0.33 (0.48) |
Dictogloss | 0.59 (0.50) | 0.79 (0.41) | 0.74 (0.44) | |
PI | 0.61 (0.49) | 0.69 (0.47) | 0.69 (0.48) |
Estimate | Std. Error | z | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 1.212 | 0.314 | 3.86 | <0.001 |
Test 1 | 0.831 | 0.218 | 3.81 | <0.001 |
Test 2 | −0.008 | 0.121 | −0.07 | 0.946 |
Group | 0.862 | 0.572 | 1.51 | 0.132 |
Test 1: Group | 0.410 | 0.308 | 1.33 | 0.183 |
Test 2: Group | 0.011 | 0.167 | 0.07 | 0.948 |
Estimate | Std. Error | z | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 1.319 | 0.217 | 6.07 | <0.001 |
Test 1 | 1.186 | 0.212 | 5.59 | <0.001 |
Test 2 | 0.193 | 0.117 | 1.66 | 0.097 |
Group | 0.627 | 0.369 | 1.70 | 0.089 |
Test 1: Group | 0.724 | 0.325 | 2.23 | 0.026 |
Test 2: Group | 0.016 | 0.179 | 0.09 | 0.928 |
Estimate | Std. Error | z | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | −1.041 | 0.270 | −3.86 | <0.001 |
Test 1 | 0.446 | 0.140 | 3.18 | <0.001 |
Test 2 | 0.041 | 0.081 | 0.51 | 0.613 |
Group | −0.676 | 0.536 | −1.26 | 0.207 |
Test 1: Group | −0.590 | 0.279 | −2.12 | 0.035 |
Test 2: Group | −0.412 | 0.162 | −2.55 | 0.011 |
Estimate | Std. Error | z | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.918 | 0.236 | 3.90 | <0.001 |
Test 1 | 0.373 | 0.149 | 2.51 | 0.012 |
Test 2 | 0.051 | 0.086 | 0.59 | 0.556 |
Group | 0.335 | 0.452 | 0.74 | 0.460 |
Test 1: Group | 0.363 | 0.261 | 1.39 | 0.165 |
Test 2: Group | 0.061 | 0.151 | 0.40 | 0.688 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Paspali, A. Enhancing Form–Meaning Connections in the Language Teaching of Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from Two Teaching Interventions. Educ. Sci. 2025, 15, 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15050618
Paspali A. Enhancing Form–Meaning Connections in the Language Teaching of Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from Two Teaching Interventions. Education Sciences. 2025; 15(5):618. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15050618
Chicago/Turabian StylePaspali, Anastasia. 2025. "Enhancing Form–Meaning Connections in the Language Teaching of Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from Two Teaching Interventions" Education Sciences 15, no. 5: 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15050618
APA StylePaspali, A. (2025). Enhancing Form–Meaning Connections in the Language Teaching of Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from Two Teaching Interventions. Education Sciences, 15(5), 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15050618