Cognitive Reappraisal: The Bridge between Cognitive Load and Emotion
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Cognitive Load Theory: Working Memory Limitations Affect Learning
Types of Cognitive Load
3. Emotions Affect Learning and Cognition
3.1. Emotion and Working Memory
3.2. Influence of Emotion Regulation on Cognition and Behavior
4. Emotion Impacts Cognitive Load
4.1. Cognitive Load via Emotional Task-Irrelevant Information
4.2. Emotion, Motivation, and Cognition
4.3. Emotion, Regulation, and Reappraisal
5. Discussion
6. Implications
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Hawthorne, B.S.; Vella-Brodrick, D.A.; Hattie, J. Well-being as a cognitive load reducing agent: A review of the literature. Front. Educ. 2019, 4, 121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalyuga, S. Evolutionary perspective on human cognitive architecture in cognitive load theory: A dynamic, emerging principle approach. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2023, 35, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; van Merriënboer, J.J.; Paas, F. Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 31, 261–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klepsch, M.; Seufert, T. Making an effort versus experiencing load. Front. Educ. 2021, 6, 645284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plass, J.L.; Kalyuga, S. Four ways of considering emotion in cognitive load theory. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 31, 339–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, J. Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2001, 130, 436–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gray, J.R. Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2004, 13, 46–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, J.R.; Braver, T.S.; Raichle, M.E. Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2002, 99, 4115–4120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kensinger, E.A.; Corkin, S. Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. Emotion 2003, 3, 378–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldon, D.F.; Callan, G.; Juth, S.; Jeong, S. Cognitive load as motivational cost. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 31, 319–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moreno, R. Cognitive load theory: More food for thought. Instr. Sci. 2010, 38, 135–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seufert, T. The interplay between self-regulation in learning and cognitive load. Educ. Res. Rev. 2018, 24, 116–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerjets, P.; Scheiter, K.; Cierniak, G. The scientific value of cognitive load theory: A research agenda based on the structuralist view of theories. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 21, 43–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ginns, P.; Leppink, J. Special issue on cognitive load theory. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 31, 255–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Repovs, G.; Baddeley, A. The multi-component model of working memory: Explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience 2006, 139, 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, H.; Yang, S.; Isen, A.M. Positive affect improves working memory: Implications for controlled cognitive processing. Cogn. Emot. 2013, 27, 474–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmeichel, B.J.; Volokhov, R.N.; Demaree, H.A. Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 1526–1540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adams, E.J.; Nguyen, A.T.; Cowan, N. Theories of working memory: Differences in definition, degree of modularity, role of attention, and purpose. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 340–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chassy, P.; Lahaye, R.; Gobet, F. Templates but not emotions facilitate the information flow between long-term and working memory: A Sternberg study with chess experts. J. Expert./March 2023, 6. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, N. Metatheory of storage capacity limits. Behav. Brain Sci. 2001, 24, 154–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, S. A critical analysis of the theoretical construction and empirical measurement of cognitive load. In Cognitive Load Measurement and Application: A Theoretical Framework for Meaningful Research and Practice; Zheng, R.Z., Ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 29–44. [Google Scholar]
- Kane, M.J.; Engle, R.W. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2002, 9, 637–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norris, D.; Kalm, K. Chunking and data compression in verbal short-term memory. Cognition 2021, 208, 104534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thalmann, M.; Souza, A.S.; Oberauer, K. How does chunking help working memory? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2019, 45, 37–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J. Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instr. Sci. 2004, 32, 9–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.G. Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. J. Educ. Psychol. 1992, 84, 429–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.; Renkl, A.; Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 38, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, O.; Castro-Alonso, J.C.; Paas, F.; Sweller, J. Extending cognitive load theory to incorporate working memory resource depletion: Evidence from the spacing effect. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 30, 483–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ayres, P. Subjective measures of cognitive load: What can they reliability measure? In Cognitive Load Measurement and Application: A Theoretical Framework for Meaningful Research and Practice; Zheng, R.Z., Ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 9–28. [Google Scholar]
- Kalyuga, S.; Plass, J.L. Cognitive load as a local characteristic of cognitive processes: Implications for measurement approaches. In Cognitive Load Measurement and Application: A Theoretical Framework for Meaningful Research and Practice; Zheng, R.Z., Ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 59–74. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura, A.; Barbaranelli, C.; Caprara, G.V.; Pastorelli, C. Multifaceted impact of self-efficacy beliefs on academic functioning. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 1206–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.; Tuovinen, J.E.; van Merriënboer, J.J.; Darabi, A.A. A motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance: Optimizing learner involvement in instruction. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 2005, 53, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pintrich, P.R. Implications of psychological research on student learning and college teaching for teacher education. In Handbook of Research on Teacher Education; Houston, W.R., Ed.; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 1990; pp. 826–857. [Google Scholar]
- Schunk, D.H.; Pintrich, P.R.; Meece, J.L. Motivation in Education: Theory, Research and Applications, 3rd ed.; Merrill: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Wigfield, A.; Eccles, J.S. Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 25, 68–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldon, D.F.; Franco, J.; Chao, J.; Peugh, J.; Maahs-Fladung, C. Self-efficacy change associated with a cognitive load-based intervention in an undergraduate biology course. Learn. Instr. 2018, 56, 64–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldon, D.F.; Brockbank, R.; Litson, K. Direct effects of cognitive load on self-efficacy during instruction. J. Educ. Psychol. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, X. Example-based learning: Effects of different types of examples on student performance, cognitive load and self-efficacy in a statistical learning task. Interact. Learn. Environ. 2017, 25, 283–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, X. Personal communication. October 2018, 12, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Kalyuga, S. Cognitive load theory: How many types of load does it really need? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 23, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J. Element interactivity and intrinsic 2011, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 22, 123–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Merriënboer, J.J.; Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 17, 147–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, O.; Paas, F.; Sweller, J. A cognitive load theory approach to defining and measuring task complexity through element interactivity. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2023, 35, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kester, L.; Paas, F.; van Merrienboer, J.J.G. Instructional control of cognitive load in the design of complex learning environments. In Cognitive Load Theory; Plass, J.L., Moreno, R., Brünken, R., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010; pp. 109–130. [Google Scholar]
- Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learn. Instr. 1994, 4, 295–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; Chandler, P. Why some material is difficult to learn. Cogn. Instr. 1994, 12, 185–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klepsch, M.; Seufert, T. Understanding instructional design effects by differentiated measurement of intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Instr. Sci. 2020, 48, 45–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R.; Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. Academic emotions and student engagement. In Handbook of Research on Student Engagement; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2012; pp. 259–282. [Google Scholar]
- Prinz, J. Emotions: How many are there? In The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Cognitive Science, 1st ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012; pp. 183–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scherer, K.R.; Moors, A. The emotion process: Event appraisal and component differentiation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2019, 70, 719–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhang, L.; Liu, S.; Liu, X.; Zhang, B.; An, X.; Ming, D. Emotional arousal and valence jointly modulate the auditory response: A 40-Hz ASSR study. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2021, 29, 1150–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schweizer, S.; Hampshire, A.; Dalgleish, T. Extending brain-training to the affective domain: Increasing cognitive and affective executive control through emotional working memory training. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e24372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pham, M.T. Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2007, 11, 155–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R.; Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. Introduction to emotions in education. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education; Routledge: London, UK, 2014; pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Plass, J.L.; Kaplan, U. Emotional design in digital media for learning. In Emotions, Technology, Design, and Learning; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016; pp. 131–161. [Google Scholar]
- Russell, J.A. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strongman, K.T. The Psychology of Emotion, 3rd ed.; Wiley: Chichester, UK, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Lazarus, R.S. Emotion and Adaptation; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Pekrun, R.; Perry, R.P. Control-value theory of achievement emotions. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education; Routledge: London, UK, 2014; pp. 120–141. [Google Scholar]
- Huang, X.; Li, S.; Wang, T.; Lajoie, S.P. The effects of emotion regulation and students’ perceived challenges on emotion synchrony in collaborative learning. Res. Square 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frijda, N.H. The Laws of Emotion; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- von Scheve, C.; Ismer, S. Towards a theory of collective emotions. Emot. Rev. 2013, 5, 406–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Järvenoja, H.; Järvelä, S.; Törmänen, T.; Näykki, P.; Malmberg, J.; Kurki, K.; Mykkänen, A.; Isohätälä, J. Capturing motivation and emotion regulation during a learning process. Frontline Learn. Res. 2018, 6, 85–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inq. 2015, 26, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R. The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 18, 315–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Covington, M.V. Making the Grade: A Self-Worth Perspective on Motivation and School Reform; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Martin, A.J.; Marsh, H.W. Academic resilience and its psychological and educational correlates: A construct validity approach. Psychol. Sch. 2006, 43, 267–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarason, I.G.; Sarason, B.R. Test anxiety. In Handbook of Social and Evaluation Anxiety; Leitenberg, H., Ed.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990; pp. 475–495. [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger, C.D. Assessment of state and trait anxiety: Conceptual and methodological issues. South. Psychol. 1985, 2, 6–16. [Google Scholar]
- Tobias, S. Test anxiety: Interference, defective skills, and cognitive capacity. Educ. Psychol. 1985, 20, 135–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zohar, D. An additive model of test anxiety: Role of exam-specific expectations. J. Educ. Psychol. 1998, 90, 330–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, A.J.; Marsh, H.W. Academic buoyancy: Towards an understanding of students’ everyday academic resilience. J. Sch. Psychol. 2008, 46, 53–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aldao, A.; Nolen-Hoeksema, S.; Schweizer, S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 30, 217–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blanchette, I.; Richards, A. The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 24, 561–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bodenhausen, G.V. Emotions, arousal, and stereotype-based discrimination: A heuristic model of affect and stereotyping. In Affect, Cognition, and Stereotyping; Mackie, D.M., Hamilton, D.L., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1993; pp. 13–37. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J.B.; Pham, M.T.; Andrade, E.B. The nature and role of affect in consumer judgment and decision making. In Handbook of Consumer Psychology; Haugtvedt, C.P., Herr, P.M., Kardes, F.R., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson, B.L. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden- and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 2001, 56, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Frenzel, A.C.; Barchfeld, P.; Perry, R.P. Measuring emotions in students’ learning and performance: The Achievement Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ). Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 36, 36–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R.; Stephens, E.J. Achievement emotions: A control-value approach. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2010, 4, 238–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zeidner, M. The origins and development of test anxiety. In Test Anxiety: The State of the Art; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1998; pp. 145–170. [Google Scholar]
- Parrott, W.G.; Spackman, M.P. Emotion and memory. Handb. Emot. 2000, 2, 476–490. [Google Scholar]
- Gendolla, G.H.E. On the impact of mood on behavior: An integrative theory and a review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2000, 4, 378–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siemer, M. Mood-specific effects on appraisal and emotion judgements. Cogn. Emot. 2001, 15, 453–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harley, J.M.; Pekrun, R.; Taxer, J.L.; Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation in achievement situations: An integrated model. Educ. Psychol. 2019, 54, 106–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koole, S.L. Does emotion regulation help or hurt self-regulation. Psychol. Self-Regul. Cogn. Affect. Motiv. Process. 2009, 11, 217–231. [Google Scholar]
- Koole, S.L. The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 23, 4–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bargh, J.A.; Williams, L.E. The case for nonconscious emotion regulation. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation; Gross, J.J., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 429–445. [Google Scholar]
- Koole, S.L.; Kuhl, J. Dealing with unwanted feelings: The role of affect regulation in volitional action control. In Handbook of Motivation Science; Shah, J., Gardner, W., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Mauss, I.B.; Bunge, S.A.; Gross, J.J. Automatic emotion regulation. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2007, 1, 146–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ochsner, K.N.; Gross, J.J. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 242–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ochsner, K.N.; Gross, J.J. Cognitive emotion regulation: Insights from social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 17, 153–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gross, J.J. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 224–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Porges, S.W. The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 2007, 74, 116–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rothermund, K.; Voss, A.; Wentura, D. Counter-regulation in affective attentional bias: A basic mechanism that warrants flexibility in motivation and emotion. Emotion 2008, 8, 34–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 2002, 39, 281–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schweizer, S.; Grahn, J.; Hampshire, A.; Mobbs, D.; Dalgleish, T. Training the emotional brain: Improving affective control through emotional working memory training. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 5301–5311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Inzlicht, M.; Schmeichel, B.J.; Macrae, C.N. Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Suri, G.; Whittaker, K.; Gross, J.J. Launching reappraisal: It’s less common than you might think. Emotion 2015, 15, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barger, S.D.; Bachen, E.A.; Marsland, A.L.; Manuck, S.B. Repressive coping and blood measures of disease risk: Lipids and endocrine and immunological responses to a laboratory stressor 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 30, 1619–1638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Butler, E.A.; Egloff, B.; Wilhelm, F.W.; Smith, N.C.; Erickson, E.A.; Gross, J.J. The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion 2003, 3, 48–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- John, O.P.; Gross, J.J. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. J. Personal. 2004, 72, 1301–1334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Myers, L.B.; Burns, J.W.; Derakshan, N.; Elfant, E.; Eysenck, M.W.; Phipps, S. Current issues in repressive coping and health. In Emotion Regulation: Conceptual and Clinical Issues; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2008; pp. 69–86. [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger, D.A. The construct validity of the repressive coping style. In Repression and Dissociation; Singer, J.L., Ed.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1990; pp. 337–386. [Google Scholar]
- Geraerts, E.; Merckelbach, H.; Jelicic, M.; Smeets, E. Long term consequences of suppression of intrusive thoughts and repressive coping. Behav. Res. Ther. 2006, 44, 1451–1460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayes, J.P.; Morey, R.A.; Petty, C.M.; Seth, S.; Smoski, M.J.; McCarthy, G.; LaBar, K.S. Staying cool when things get hot: Emotion regulation modulates neural mechanisms of memory encoding. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 4, 230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johns, M.J.; Inzlicht, M.; Schmader, T. Stereotype threat and executive resource depletion: Examining the influence of emotion regulation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2008, 137, 691–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lane, R.D.; Sechrest, L.; Riedel, R.; Shapiro, D.E.; Kaszniak, A.W. Pervasive emotion recognition deficit common to alexithymia and the repressive coping style. Psychosom. Med. 2000, 62, 492–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richards, J.M.; Gross, J.J. Personality and emotional memory: How regulating emotion impairs memory for emotional events. J. Res. Personal. 2006, 40, 631–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McRae, K. Cognitive emotion regulation: A review of theory and scientific findings. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 10, 119–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folkman, S. Positive psychological states and coping with severe stress. Soc. Sci. Med. 1997, 45, 1207–1221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Folkman, S. The case for positive emotions in the stress process. Anxiety Stress Coping 2008, 21, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Derakshan, N.; Eysenck, M.W. Working-memory capacity in high trait-anxious and repressor groups. Cogn. Emot. 1998, 12, 697–713. [Google Scholar]
- Richards, A.; French, C.C.; Keogh, E.; Carter, C. Test anxiety, inferential reasoning and working memory load. Anxiety. Stress Coping 2000, 13, 87–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mani, A.; Mullainathan, S.; Shafir, E.; Zhao, J. Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science 2013, 341, 976–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mullainathan, S.; Shafir, E. Scarcity: Why Having Too Little Means So Much; Times Books: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Schulz, J.; Miyamoto, A.; Nückles, M. Through the Lens of Scarcity. How Perceived Resource Scarcity Shapes Students Mindsets and Learning Experience (Poster JURE/EARLI 2023 Thessaloniki, Greece). Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/373643680_Through_the_lens_of_scarcity_How_perceived_resource_scarcity_shapes_students_mindsets_and_learning_experience_Poster_JureEarli_2023_Thessaloniki (accessed on 6 August 2024).
- Eysenck, M.W.; Derakshan, N.; Santos, R.; Calvo, M.G. Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion 2007, 7, 336–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, A.J.; Marsh, H.W.; Debus, R.L. Self-handicapping and defensive pessimism: Exploring a model of predictors and outcomes from a self-protection perspective. J. Educ. Psychol. 2001, 93, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarason, I.G. Stress, anxiety, and cognitive interference: Reactions to tests. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 46, 929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarason, I.G. Test anxiety, cognitive interference, and performance. In Aptitude, Learning, and Instruction; Routledge: London, UK, 2021; pp. 131–142. [Google Scholar]
- Ashcraft, M.H.; Kirk, E.P. The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2001, 130, 224–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beilock, S.L.; Carr, T.H. When high-powered people fail: Working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 101–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klein, K.; Boals, A. The relationship of life event stress and working memory capacity. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2001, 15, 565–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmader, T.; Johns, M. Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 440–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bar-Haim, Y.; Lamy, D.; Pergamin, L.; Bakermans- Kranenburg, M.J.; van IJzendoorn, M.H. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 2007, 133, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, R.W. Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 11, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moran, T.P. Anxiety and working memory capacity: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 142, 831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Derakshan, N.; Eysenck, M.W. Anxiety, processing efficiency, and cognitive performance: New developments from attentional control theory. Eur. Psychol. 2009, 14, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eysenck, M.W.; Derakshan, N. New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 50, 955–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stout, D.M.; Rokke, P.D. Components of working memory predict symptoms of distress. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 24, 1293–1303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hembree, R. Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety. Rev. Educ. Res. 1988, 58, 47–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seibert, P.S.; Ellis, H.C. Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance. Mem. Cogn. 1991, 19, 507–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brockbank, R.B.; Feldon, D.F. Conceptualizing cognitive load as motivational cost: A mixed methods perspective. Forthcoming unpublished manuscript.
- Pessoa, L. How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 160–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osaka, M.; Yaoi, K.; Minamoto, T.; Osaka, N. When do negative and positive emotions modulate working memory performance? Sci. Rep. 2013, 3, 1375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, A.J. Using Load Reduction Instruction (LRI) to Boost Motivation and Engagement; British Psychological Society: Leicester, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Y.E.; Yu, S.L.; Wolters, C.A.; Anderman, E.M. Self-regulatory processes within and between diverse goals: The multiple goals regulation framework. Educ. Psychol. 2023, 58, 70–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Y.E.; Zepeda, C.D.; Martin, R.S.; Butler, A.C. Situating cost perceptions: How general cost and motivational regulation predict specific momentary cost dimensions. Educ. Psychol. 2023, 43, 855–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mueller, C.J.; Classe, F.; Stürmer, B.; Kuchinke, L.; Stelzel, C. Neurocognitive Effects of Self-Determined Choice and Emotional Arousal on Time Estimation. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2021, 17, 161–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prencipe, A.; Kesek, A.; Cohen, J.; Lamm, C.; Lewis, M.D.; Zelazo, P.D. Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011, 108, 621–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chevalier, N.; Huber, K.L.; Wiebe, S.A.; Espy, K.A. Qualitative change in executive control during childhood and adulthood. Cognition 2013, 128, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Flake, J.K.; Barron, K.E.; Hulleman, C.; McCoach, B.D.; Welsh, M.E. Measuring cost: The forgotten component of expectancy-value theory. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 41, 232–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seligman, M.E.; Ernst, R.M.; Gillham, J.; Reivich, K.; Linkins, M. Positive education: Positive psychology and classroom interventions. Oxf. Rev. Educ. 2009, 35, 293–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brand, S.; Reimer, T.; Opwis, K. How do we learn in a negative mood? Effects of a negative mood on transfer and learning. Learn. Instr. 2007, 17, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Mello, S.; Graesser, A. Dynamics of affective states during complex learning. Learn. Instr. 2012, 22, 145–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarason, I.G.; Sarason, B.R. Teaching cognitive and social skills to high school students. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1981, 49, 908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kellermann, T.S.; Sternkopf, M.A.; Schneider, F.; Habel, U.; Turetsky, B.I.; Zilles, K.; Eickhoff, S.B. Modulating the processing of emotional stimuli by cognitive demand. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2012, 7, 263–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, C.R.; Pashler, H. Enhanced memory for negatively emotionally charged pictures without selective rumination. Emotion 2005, 5, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Gog, T.; Kester, L.; Paas, F. Effects of concurrent monitoring on cognitive load and performance as a function of task complexity. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2011, 25, 584–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldon, D.F. Cognitive load and classroom teaching: The double-edged sword of automaticity. Educ. Psychol. 2007, 42, 123–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joormann, J.; Levens, S.M.; Gotlib, I.H. Sticky thoughts: Depression and rumination are associated with difficulties manipulating emotional material in working memory. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 979–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schweizer, S.; Dalgleish, T. Emotional working memory capacity in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Behav. Res. Ther. 2011, 49, 498–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Young, J.Q.; Friedman, K.A.; Sewell, J.L.; Thakker, K.; John, M.; van Merrienboer, J.J.; O’Sullivan, P.S. Influence of emotion on cognitive load experienced by trainees while performing patient handoffs. Acad. Med. 2021, 96, S221–S222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, M.D. Cognition-emotion feedback and the self-organization of developmental paths. Hum. Dev. 1995, 38, 71–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, M.D. Interacting time scales in personality (and cognitive) development: Intentions, emotions, and emergent forms. In Microdevelopment: Transition Processes in Development and Learning; Granott, N., Parziale, J., Eds.; Cambridge University: Cambridge, UK, 2002; pp. 183–212. [Google Scholar]
Type | Definition | Context |
---|---|---|
Cognitive Reappraisal | Reframing or reassessing understandings or beliefs that underly emotional responses to make them more adaptive. | -Generally considered adaptive. -Associated with positive overall well-being. -Most effective ER strategy leading to cognitive change. -Most robustly studied ER strategy from a neuroimaging perspective. |
Expressive Suppression | Inhibiting or blocking emotional responses expressively or behaviorally. | -Generally considered maladaptive. -Associated with poorer health and overall well-being, anxiety, and depression. -Most frequently examined ER strategy, often via observation and self-report. |
Emotional Impacts on Cognitive Load | Description |
---|---|
Emotion as Extraneous Cognitive Load | Emotion competes for limited working memory resources, imposing extraneous cognitive load as task-irrelevant information. |
Emotion Impacts Motivation | Emotion influences cognitive load by affecting motivation and resulting mental effort investment. |
Emotion Affects Cognitive Processes | Emotion affects the cognitive processes of encoding, storage and retrieval of information, thus impacting working memory. |
Emotion as Intrinsic Cognitive Load | Emotion affects intrinsic cognitive load when emotion regulation is an integral part of learning outcomes & objectives. |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Brockbank, R.B.; Feldon, D.F. Cognitive Reappraisal: The Bridge between Cognitive Load and Emotion. Educ. Sci. 2024, 14, 870. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14080870
Brockbank RB, Feldon DF. Cognitive Reappraisal: The Bridge between Cognitive Load and Emotion. Education Sciences. 2024; 14(8):870. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14080870
Chicago/Turabian StyleBrockbank, Rebecca B., and David F. Feldon. 2024. "Cognitive Reappraisal: The Bridge between Cognitive Load and Emotion" Education Sciences 14, no. 8: 870. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14080870
APA StyleBrockbank, R. B., & Feldon, D. F. (2024). Cognitive Reappraisal: The Bridge between Cognitive Load and Emotion. Education Sciences, 14(8), 870. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14080870