4.1.1. Self-Relation during PD
One key factor in interest development is self-relation [
10]. Therefore, we investigated the teachers’ answers to find traces of self-relation to identify value-related patterns.
Qualitative Analysis. We approached the qualitative (i.e., human coding) of the text data in two phases: First, we identified different types of self-relation language. We categorized self-relation indicators under two main categories: low and high. Then, we counted each participant’s frequency of self-relation terms to reach a sense of their self-relation profile. We then grouped our sample into three categories based on the frequencies of self-relation language in their data (
Table 2). We followed the same approach to the other dimensions.
After identifying self-relation quotes for the participants, we investigated the language used to see if there were different patterns. The key difference between participants’ self-relation language was that while generic statements identified low self-relation, high self-relation was indicated by quotes that included specifics. For example, as can be seen in the following quotation, Teacher D suggested: “I am still excited about the program because I know it will offer a great opportunity for the students at my school to be exposed to the other side of GAMING (sic.)”. This quote lacks specifics regarding how and in what ways it would be a great opportunity for the students. Similarly, another teacher, Teacher C, argued: “The biggest thing about programming is understanding the concept of solving a problem. Understanding the flow chart is applicable to Unity and solving issues in life”. It is hard to discern from these quotes, and low-self-relation quotes like this, how and why learning programming would be related to real-life problem solving.
On the other hand, the indicators for high self-relation included more details regarding the “how and why” of such a connection. The teachers’ responses involved more detail connecting these ideas to what they learned. For example, Teacher B described how learning programming was related to problem-solving like Teacher C, but they gave more details on how and connected these ideas to the course:
Considering the focus on product creation for solving real problems, this course would enable students to construct an understanding from evaluating applications and what it produces and identifying solutions. From the perspective of showcasing what a program looks like and what it can do, our students should be able to see and comprehend what elements of coding (computer programming) allow certain things to happen.
High self-relation indicators also included the relation of the PD program to others’ lives in a specific and targeted manner. For example, Teacher B described their relationship to coding and how they were motivated to teach their students: “What has motivated me throughout the course is the fact I am doing something that I once loved to do and moved away from and also the fact I am learning it to teach it to my students”.
In this part of the qualitative analysis, we concluded that teachers who developed self-relation to the PD content included specific arguments regarding the how and why, while the teachers with low self-relation described it in more generic terms.
Frequency of Responses. Approaching the qualitative analyses with a quantitative perspective, we counted the number of occurrences of self-relation-related quotes. We should note that all the teachers received the same prompts while answering the questions on different platforms. In our analyses, we found that while some teachers responded several times with detail, other teachers’ reports were limited. As can be seen in
Table 2, two teachers, Teacher A (four times) and B (six times) made significantly more self-relation connections while answering the prompts compared to the others (C: two times, D: one time, E: none). The low self-relation teachers not only mentioned self-relation less in their responses; their responses were also not specific.
LIWC Analysis. Finally, in addition to human coding, to analyze the self-relation patterns of teachers, we benefited from computational tools: LIWC software. Self-relation is context specific. For example, in our case, our participants were teachers, and self-relation meant words corresponding to their teaching, as noted in the human coding section above. Therefore, based on the human-coded data, we formed a dictionary related to self-relation using frequently used words. This new dictionary included words such as opportunity, enable, teach, and student, words that refer to the ways PD is applied to teachers’ lives and their responsibility as teachers. The LWIC analysis showed that Teachers A (119.26) and B (77.13) used significantly more self-relation-related words in their responses than the others. This finding confirms the human coding analysis: Teachers A and B had the most self-relation responses.
Summary. Self-relation and connection of the activities to one’s life was key indicator that significantly varied between teachers. Looking at the frequency of responses related to value and the detail in their responses, we decided that there were three profiles considering the value through self-relation (
Table 2). We considered Teachers A and B as having high value through self-relation. These teachers frequently talked about how the PD program is related to their or others’ lives and explained why and how. We considered Teachers C and D in the mid-range through the self-relation profile because they mentioned the program’s value much fewer times, and their responses were not as specific. Finally, we did not find self-related responses for Teacher E in human coding. Although LIWC results showed that she mentioned the words in our dictionary a few times, we categorized them in the low-self-relation group.
4.1.2. Knowledge Increase during PD
Knowledge increase is an important component of interest development [
10]. We analyzed the knowledge increase resulting from participation in the PD program as one of the factors related to interest development from a few different perspectives. First, we analyzed the CS knowledge test the teachers completed before and after the PD program. In addition, like self-relation, we examined the teachers’ text data for traces of “knowledge” they gained during our PD through human coding and computational methods.
CS Knowledge Test. After grading the pre and posttest, we categorized teachers into quartiles to determine their relative performance and knowledge levels. To calculate the quartiles, we combined the scores from the pre and posttest and calculated the scores that would correspond to the 0–25th (1), 26th–50th (2), 50th–75th (3), and 75th–100th (4) quartiles. The first quartile included scores between 0–48.6, the second quartile consisted of scores between 48.6–55.7, the third quartile was between 55.8–63.95, and the last quartile consisted of scores between 63.95–100.
The results indicated that Teachers A and B started with the lowest CS knowledge (first quartile), while Teacher C had the highest knowledge (fourth quartile) on the pretest. Teachers D and E were in the second quartile. The results showed that teachers A and B scored the lowest in the pretest, improved the most in the post-test and ended in the third and fourth quartiles, respectively (
Table 3). Compared to Teachers A and B, Teachers C, D, and E gained significantly smaller knowledge based on the CS test results.
Qualitative Analysis. Following the self-relation analysis steps, we first looked for, and identified patterns in text that corresponded to knowledge traces the teachers gained through the PD program. Based on our qualitative results, we noted that low-knowledge indicators were generic, while high-knowledge was indicated by PD-specific text.
We coded knowledge increase indicators that did not target program-specific language as low. For example, Teacher D mentioned, “Things can take longer to figure out than expected. Things can get frustrating when everything does not work correctly”. This description of their learning was in very general terms that could be applied to any other program and, therefore, considered a low knowledge indicator. Cases in the low category also included snippets where there was more detail but were still referring to basic steps. For example, Teacher C indicated they “learned the basic menu screen in Unity. I have also learned how to attach code to my Unity program using Visual Studio. I have learned the basic steps of creating and running code with the game”. As seen from the quotations, they talked about learning in general terms and the basic concepts without going into the mechanisms involved.
It should be noted that data from teachers C and D included some specificity indicating, albeit low, knowledge gains, despite being sporadic and limited. Teacher E, however, did not have elaborative text data to be analyzed for knowledge increase.
The pattern for the high category included PD-specific information that the teachers came across during their engagement in the course modules. For example, Teacher A mentioned:
After the meeting I better understand why we would use an array and switch in our codes. I also learned the importance of mapping out the solution to the challenge in plain English.
Another teacher talked about their experience during learning and specifically talked about a lesson and the knowledge she gained from it. As can be seen from the following quotation, they were involved and invested in the process and learning:
“I now can say I have steady rocket that lands on the launch pad. My rocket body parts were not assembled correctly even though they gave an appearance of being correct. That is because of my axis angles were not correct like I thought. I learned that I did not have my axis angles positioned correctly. I learned how to do that. This was key to me getting my body parts aligned straight. That was indeed my take away in this assignment”
(Teacher B)
Frequency of Responses. In addition to qualitative analyses, we counted the times the teachers explicitly mentioned “knowledge bits” they learned from the PD activities. Corresponding to the previous analyses, Teachers A (ten times) and B (eight times) reported the highest number of knowledge references. Teacher C reported five times what they learned, but the responses were primarily related to the early weeks of the PD program. On the other hand, there were very few referrals to specific knowledge in Teacher D’s (two times) and E’s (none) text data.
LIWC Analysis. In addition to human coding, we benefited from LIWC software to analyze teachers’ knowledge-increase patterns. Since the knowledge language is context-specific, we created our own “knowledge” dictionary. It included words specific to our PD content and words that referred to knowledge gain and learning in our PD (e.g., learn, module, variables, Unity). The results of the LIWC analysis supported the qualitative analysis. They indicated that Teacher A (sum = 594) and B (sum = 468) referred to PD-related knowledge gains more than the other teachers (D = 83, E = 67, C = 215). In addition to our self-made dictionary, we also used the curiosity subcategory from the default LIWC dictionary as it related to knowledge-seeking and interest in new knowledge. The results for the curiosity dictionary also followed a similar pattern: A (sum = 106), B (sum = 70), C (sum = 58), E (sum = 23), and D (sum = 13).
Term Frequency—Inverse Document Frequency. In addition to LIWC, we also analyzed the text data collected during the PD through another computational text analysis method: TF-IDF. This analysis showed the most common words used by each teacher compared to the rest of the text data. As shown in
Figure 2, teachers differed in the most frequent words they used while responding to prompts based on the words we noted to be in the knowledge category. For example, tf-idf results for Teachers A and B included PD-specific knowledge terms and learning-related topics (e.g., rocket, model, module, add, video). Teacher C’s results also included some language related to our PD and software (e.g., chart, function). However, the most frequently used words were not explicitly related to the PD (e.g., overcome, biggest, laptop). Teacher D mostly talked about general issues not specific to the PD process (e.g., energy, frustrating, exposed). The responses of Teacher E were mainly related to the implementation of the PD program (e.g., curriculum, assessment, target) rather than the learning-related topics of the PD.
Summary. Based on our analyses of the participant data (knowledge test, qualitative, frequency of responses, LIWC, tf-idf), we categorized teachers in terms of their knowledge profiles (
Table 4.). Results from different analyses aligned and provided a complete picture of the participants’ knowledge profiles. Teachers A and B were categorized as high due to the frequency of learning-related text they used. Furthermore, according to the computational text analyses, these teachers used the most knowledge-related words specific to their learning experiences during the PD. Both teachers confirmed these findings and showed the biggest knowledge gains in the CS test. We therefore categorized Teachers A and B as “high” in terms of their knowledge during PD.
For Teachers C and D, we found a significantly lower number of texts referring to specific learning through PD, and they showed marginal gains from the PD as indicated by their test score changes. They also were in the fourth and third posttest quartiles. We, therefore, categorized them in the “mid” knowledge category. Finally, Teacher E had the lowest scores in all the measures, placing them in the “low” category.
4.1.3. Affective Profiles
Positive feelings toward a subject are considered essential factors for interest development [
10]. Therefore, reporting excitement about an experience (i.e., the PD program) or a certain content can indicate interest development. To find traces of positive emotion or excitement, we analyzed our data using multiple lenses: qualitative (human coding), frequency (human coding), and LIWC (computational coding).
Qualitative Analysis. Qualitatively investigating the data, we first identified text snippets containing affective words, then categorized them as low and high indicators. Like value through self-relation and knowledge increase, a low indicator of excitement included the display of excitement without any specificity to the content of PD. For example, Teacher C indicated that they were interested in learning without explaining why: “I am still very interested in learning more”.
The pattern for high excitement usually included explaining the reasons (i.e., elaboration) for them to feel excited. For example, Teacher A talked about their feelings toward coding and explained why they had positive feelings: “Adding the code is fun. I am eager to learn more about adding it and seeing changes”. Similarly, they discussed their feelings toward re-teaching these subjects: “I am curious as to how we would teach kids to identify the most important elements of their game”. They also discussed their excitement and attitude toward coding in general:
What has motivated me throughout the course is the fact I am doing something that I once loved to do and moved away from and also the fact I am learning it to teach it to my students. I still feel excited about the project as I did in the beginning. Because I have a background in coding, I knew there would be challenges, and I was and still am ready to embrace them.
Frequency of Responses. When we looked at the frequency of responses related to positive feelings, teachers’ responses showed great variation. Teachers A (eight times) and B (five times) reported positive feelings toward the PD and their learning (i.e., they indicated they are motivated, interested, and excited in different weeks.). In contrast, Teachers C and D talked once about positive feelings, while Teacher E did not have any response related to their positive feelings about the program.
LIWC Analysis. In addition, we benefited from LIWC software to analyze teachers’ affective difference patterns. For the affective difference patterns, we used the Satisfaction, Effort Enjoyment, and Accomplishment subcategories of the Behavioral Activation dictionary [
30]. The creators of the dictionary characterized the satisfaction category with words such as enthuse, love, and satisfied. The effort-enjoyment subcategory was characterized by words like enjoy, energized, and enthusiastic. The accomplishment category included words such as proud, achieve, and goal. The LWIC analysis showed that Teachers A (sum = 84.56) and B (sum = 92.08) had higher satisfaction scores than other teachers. Moreover, Teacher A scored highest in terms of effort enjoyment (sum = 321.38) and accomplishment (sum 63.12). Therefore, LIWC results suggest that Teachers A and B talked more about positive feelings in their responses (
Table 5).
Summary. Our analyses of the teachers’ text data, both using human coding and computational methods, showed that there was variation among teachers. Still, these different types of analyses also confirmed each other’s findings (
Table 6). While some teachers (Teachers A and B) frequently talked about the positive feelings they experienced during the PD and elaborated on how and why they were excited, other teachers did not talk much about the positive feelings, or their responses were more generic. Therefore, we categorized Teachers A and B in the high group. The next group, mid (Teacher C and D), included a lower frequency of positive emotions than the high group. Finally, the low group included participants with the lowest scores from the affective indicator measures.