Next Article in Journal
Online Mathematics Education during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Didactic Strategies, Educational Resources, and Educational Contexts
Previous Article in Journal
Quality of Life in Deafblind People and Its Effect on the Processes of Educational Adaptation and Social Inclusion in Canary Islands, Spain
Previous Article in Special Issue
SAGA-Supporting Social-Emotional Development in Early Childhood Education: The Development of a Mentalizing-Based Intervention
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Relationship between Children’s Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Big Five, Big Two and Big One Personality Traits

by
Èlia López-Cassà
1,*,
Núria Pérez-Escoda
2 and
Alberto Alegre
3
1
Department of Didactics and Educational Organization, University of Barcelona, 08007 Barcelona, Spain
2
Department of Research Methods and Diagnosis in Education, University of Barcelona, 08007 Barcelona, Spain
3
Department of Early Childhood and Elementary Education, East Stroudsburg University, East Stroudsburg, PA 18301, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Educ. Sci. 2022, 12(7), 491; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12070491
Submission received: 2 April 2022 / Revised: 23 June 2022 / Accepted: 12 July 2022 / Published: 16 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Social and Emotional Development in Early Childhood Education)

Abstract

:
The irrefutable repercussions of personality and socio-emotional development on children’s learning and psychological well-being justify the relevance for the educational context of delving into the relationship between those two constructs. Therefore, the research presented in this article investigates the link between trait EI and the B5, B2, and B1 (or GFP) personality traits in children between 9 and 13 years of age. We used the Spanish adaptation of the BFQ-NA (Big Five Personality Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents) and the CDE_9-13 (Emotional Development Questionnaire for primary education) with a sample of 259 primary school students. The results showed correlations between the two Big personality factors (B2) and the Big One personality factor (B1) with trait EI. However, the relationship between trait emotional intelligence and the Big Five personality model (B5) was not very high; only two of the five personality traits significantly predicted trait EI. Thus, our results differ from studies conducted with adults, but instead, it is similar to studies conducted with children. Finally, this study reinforces the thesis that trait EI can be considered a synonym of the GFP (General Factor Personality). Consequently, it implies designing and implementing learning and socioemotional development programs during the school years to promote adaptability and social efficacy.

1. Introduction

Societies of the 21st century face significant challenges, among which is the improvement of education to provide people with the knowledge, skills, and tools they need throughout their lives [1]. For this reason, we are witnessing profound educational changes aimed at comprehensive development in skills, educational quality, responding to social demands, and giving visibility to emotional education as a subject in compulsory education.
Recently, the OECD [2] has presented an international report stating that the benefits of developing socio-emotional skills in children go beyond cognitive development and academic results; additionally, they are essential drivers of health and labor market prospects [2].
In this sense, programs designed to develop emotional competencies have shown positive results [3,4,5]. Numerous studies confirm that educational programs that include emotional education positively impact attitudes towards oneself and others, improve classroom climate, assertiveness, resilience, reduce disruptive behavior and increase academic performance, and other relevant aspects of quality education [3,6,7,8]. Moreover, they contribute to improving psychological well-being [9,10,11] while decreasing anxiety rates [6,12] and mental health problems [13].
Hence, several authors have defended that emotional education is considered as a primordial aspect of the integral development and well-being of students [14,15,16,17]. Emotional education is one of the life skills that should be emphasized, as it aims to provide students with a series of necessary tools that enable them to deal effectively and efficiently with the challenges, tasks, and situations that occur in their lives [17].
Emotional intelligence, therefore, plays a vital role in achieving success in most human activities [18], including those related to education [19]. In addition to emotional intelligence, personality plays a fundamental role in human behavior. Childhood personality is a proven predictor of many essential outcomes in adulthood [20,21]. Personality can be described as a set of stable characteristics and tendencies predisposing the person to behave in a certain way in various life situations [22].
The debate on these constructs has continued throughout the 21st century, where various theoretical frameworks have been developed on personality and similarly on emotional intelligence [23].
Therefore, knowing and analyzing the influence of Emotional Intelligence (EI) on children’s personalities and how these variables affect their education is essential.
The study of personality has been conceptualized by two models, the Giant Tree model [24] and the Big Five model of personality [25]. Both taxonomies postulate that major personality traits are determinants of an individual’s behavior, although the Big Five is the most widely accepted model of personality in the different fields of study. This model holds that we can study personality through five major factors: Openness, Responsiveness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism that account for most individual differences [26]. However, following the work of Digman [27] and De Young et al. [28], these traits were grouped into two more prominent personality factors (B2): Alpha/Stability and Beta/Plasticity. The former would include agreeableness, responsibility, and neuroticism, while the latter would encompass openness and extroversion. This grouping has been used in several studies [29,30] and the same model has been applied across cultures, life stages (children, adolescents, and adults), and languages [31,32].
In contrast, other studies have revealed the existence of a general factor of personality (GFP) which is based on the idea that it is a dominant higher-order factor (Big one, B1) that binds and integrates all other personality components [33,34,35]. According to this theory, GFP would represent an evolutionarily adaptive trait with an inherited basis, although influenced by upbringing, socialization, and education [35,36,37].
Research on the five factors and personality, in general, has focused mainly on individual differences in adulthood but has not been as thoroughly examined within younger populations. However, an interesting research study conducted by Tackett et al. [21] provides the first examination of the hierarchical structure of childhood personality in five different countries and ages. Their results showed that the five-factor model was notable from early childhood, but as children grow, they experience changes in each personality dimension. Those deviations continue until adulthood. Thus, the need to study the development of children’s personality traits has been increasingly recognized to understand how traits develop and change across the lifespan [38,39,40].
Emotional intelligence (EI) is a construct that first appeared in an article published by Salovey and Mayer [41]. Since then, a wide variety of models of emotional intelligence have proliferated, which can be grouped into two broad categories. One refers to the ability model, and the other to the trait model. The difference between the two tends to be clearly delineated as they are seen as distinct constructs [6].
The ability model considers EI as the ability to process emotional information [42], while Trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) relates to stable personality traits [43]. These authors defined EI as: “A set of emotional components and self-perceived abilities representing the personality construct” [43] (p. 479). The construct formulated by Petrides provides a clear operationalization of the emotion-related aspects of personality and lies outside the taxonomy of cognitive abilities and within the taxonomy of personality [44].
Petrides et al. [45] provided a comprehensive overview of the fields of application of trait EI. Although much research remains to be done, there is evidence of the importance of Trait EI in adults [46], children [47,48], and adolescents [49,50]. Research on Trait EI in children, the stage we will address in this paper, indicates that Trait EI is a significant predictor of health, well-being, social relationships, academic performance, and adaptive behaviors [51,52,53].
Some research argues that trait EI can be considered a lower-order personality trait [54,55], whereas others suggest that trait EI would be an indicator of the General Personality Factor (GPF) [56,57].
Meta-analysis studies have shown relatively high associations between GPF and trait EI [35,58,59,60] and have provided interesting findings confirming that all five personality traits (B5) correlate with trait EI in adult samples [58,60]. However, the GFP-trait EI association in children and adolescents appears to differ from that in adults due to the structures and development of personality during childhood [61]. Furthermore, the lower-order structure of youth traits appears to shift with age [40,62]. Recent research points out that several personality traits show distinctive developmental trends in children and youths. The mean levels of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and openness decrease from late childhood to middle adolescence. Therefore, there is a decrease in personality maturity [61,63,64].
Given the above, we need to clarify these contributions and probe deeply into the relationship between trait EI and personality. This study uses a sample of Spanish children aged 9 to 13 years and a new measure of trait EI developed by Pérez-Escoda et al. [65].
Specifically, our research has three objectives:
  • To analyze the relationship between trait emotional intelligence and the Big Five (B5) model of personality.
  • To assess the correlation between trait EI and the two Big Five (B2) factors.
  • To test the relationship between trait EI and the Big One (B1) personality factor.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Procedures

The students answered the different questionnaires online with the presence and support of their teacher during a class session. The duration of the tests was approximately 45 min.
All students participated voluntarily, and the schools collected the corresponding consents signed by the children’s families.

2.2. Participants

A total of 259 Spanish elementary students (49.4% female) completed the sample for this study. The participants ranged in age from 9 to 13 years old (M = 9.98, SD = 2.44) and in their level of studies from fourth to sixth grade of primary education.

2.3. Ethics Statement

This study was conducted following the recommendations of the Bioethics Committee of the University of Barcelona and subject to the ethical standards established by the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its subsequent modifications.
The research group signed a research agreement with each school guaranteeing the confidentiality of the results.

2.4. Measurement Instruments

Personality. The BFQ- NA (Big Five Personality Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents) [66], in its Spanish adaptation by Del Barrio et al. [67], was applied to measure the Big Five personality model in children. It consists of 65 items arranged on a five-point Likert-type scale, with values between one point “Completely false for me” and five points “Completely true for me”. Soto et al. [40] reported the psychometric characteristics of the BFQ-NA, obtaining a Cronbach’s alpha between 0.78 and 0.88 while confirming the five-factor structure corresponding to the Big Five through confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha of the subscales was Conscientiousness α = 0.85, Agreeableness α = 0.78 Neuroticism α = 0.84, Extraversion α = 0.76, Openness α = 0.84; Total scale α = 0.88.
Trait emotional intelligence. Trait emotional intelligence was measured with the Emotional Development Questionnaire for Primary Education CDE_9-13 [65]. This self-report questionnaire consists of 41 items with an 11-point Likert-type response (0 = very seldom or never, 10 = almost always), distributed in five dimensions of the pentagonal model of the Psychopedagogical Guidance Research Group of the University of Barcelona (GROP) [68]: emotional awareness, emotional regulation, emotional autonomy, social competence, and life competencies and well-being, as well as a general dimension. In this study, the reliability of the scales measured with Cronbach’s α was 0.80, 0.75, 0.62, 0.72, and 0.72, respectively. The reliability of the full scale was 0.91.

3. Results

3.1. The Location of Trait EI in the B5 Factor Space

We found moderate to high correlations between trait EI and the B5 (see Table 1). Trait EI correlated the highest with Agreeableness (r = 0.70) and the lowest with Extraversion (r = 40). The mean inter-correlation between the global trait EI and the B5 was r = 0.55.
Multiple regression analysis with trait EI as the criterion variable and B5 as predictors showed that Agreeableness and Neuroticism significantly predicted trait EI (see Table 2, Section 1), accounting for 57.1% of its variance. All VIF values were below five, showing no problematic multicollinearity issues.
We ran a principal axis factoring exploratory analysis of the 65 BFQ-NA items and the 41 CDE_9-13 items with no rotation. We obtained five factors instead of six and did not find a trait EI factor. Instead, the 41 trait EI items loaded in all five factors mixed with many BFQ-NA items. The results obtained indicated that different dimensions of emotional intelligence loaded in different factors in conjunction with different personality traits (see Table A1 in Appendix A).

3.2. Trait EI and the B2

We investigated the relationship of trait EI with the B2. We computed an Alpha/Stability coefficient adding conscientiousness to agreeableness and neuroticism. We also calculated a Beta/Plasticity variable adding Openness to Extraversion. Both coefficients correlated positively (r = 0.73, p < 0.01).
As observed in Table 1, the correlation between trait EI and Alpha/Stability (r = 0.46) was lower than that between trait EI and Beta/Plasticity (r = 0.56). The B2 explained a substantial 31% of the variance in Trait EI, with Beta/Plasticity as the strongest and only significant predictor (Table 2, Section 2).
Therefore, these results indicated that all five dimensions of emotional intelligence correlated more strongly with Plasticity than Stability.

3.3. Trait EI and the Big One

We retained one unique unrotated general component (GFP) to identify the general personality factor. The correlation between trait EI and the GFP was r = 0.69.
Following Van der Linden et al. [55], we also performed a hierarchical regression analysis including trait EI as the criterion, the General Personality Factor (GFP) as predictor entered in step 1, and the individual B5 scales (O, C, E, A, and N) as predictors entered in step 2. The GFP in step 1 explained a substantial 48% of the variance in trait EI (R²adj:47.5, F (1,116) = 102.08, p < 0.001). Concerning the total unique variance of the B5 scale scores in step 2, they explained an additional 7.9% of trait EI variance (R²adj:55.4, F (6,111) = 24.91, p < 0.001).
We combined the five BFQ-NA scales with the five CDE_9-13 factors and submitted them to a single-factor PCA using Oblimin (delta = 0) rotation (see Table 3). The analysis of the eigenvalues and the scree plot indicated the existence of only one factor. The loadings of the B5 on this factor ranged from 0.53 to 0.85, while those of trait EI ranged from 0.69 to 0.80. The trait EI dimensions that loaded highest were Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. Still, Emotional Awareness, Emotional Regulation, Social Competence, and Life and Well-Being Competencies loaded higher than Neuroticism, Openness, and Extraversion.

4. Discussion

We had three main objectives in the present study. The first was the analysis of the relationship between trait emotional intelligence (EI) and the Big Five (B5) model of personality. The second objective was to assess the correlation between trait EI and the two Big Five factors (B2). The last one was to test the relationship between trait EI and the Big One personality factor (B1). This study used a sample of Spanish children aged 9 to 13 years and a new measure of trait EI developed by Pérez-Escoda et al. [65].
Regarding the relationship between trait EI and the B5 model of personality, our findings showed that only two of the five personality traits, Agreeableness and Neuroticism, significantly predicted trait EI. This result is different from the obtained in other studies in adults, where all B5 scales significantly predicted trait EI [35,58,59,60]. There may be different reasons for this discrepancy. On the one hand, it may be due to differences between the personality structures of adults and young people [64]. According to research on children and adolescents, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, stability, and openness decrease from childhood to adolescence [63,64]. Therefore, it is possible that trait EI behaves as a distinct personality dimension for adults but does not behave in the same way for children. If that were the case, it would mean that adults and children have not only an age difference but also some qualitative differences in personality and emotional development.
On the other hand, we used a different theoretical framework of trait emotional intelligence and a different measurement instrument. We relied on Bisquerra and Pérez-Escoda’s [68] theoretical framework and used a measure of trait EI that offers five dimensions: emotional awareness, emotional regulation, social competence, emotional autonomy, and competence for life and well-being [65]. Other research studies were based on Petrides and Furnham’s definition of emotional intelligence [43]. They used other measures of trait EI such as the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire—Child Form (TEIQue-CF) [69], Trait Emotional Intelligence Adolescent Questionnaire (TEIQue-ASF) [70], or the Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ) [71], all of which contemplate different components of emotional intelligence. The different measurement tools and trait EI dimensions may explain why the results diverge.
The second objective was to investigate the relationship of trait EI with the B2. The correlations were confirmed. The results indicated that all five dimensions of emotional intelligence correlated more strongly with Plasticity than Stability. This result is different from the one obtained by Alegre et al. [60], in which Emotional Regulation, Life and Well-being competencies, and Autonomy are related more strongly to Stability than to Plasticity. In contrast, Emotional Awareness and Social Competence correlated more strongly to Plasticity.
Our last objective was to test the relationship between trait EI and the Big One personality factor (B1). The correlation between this GFP and trait EI was strong. This correlation was similar to other studies with adults [37,58,59,60] and children [61]. In our study, the trait EI dimensions with the highest loadings were Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. In addition, Emotional Awareness, Emotional Regulation, Social Competence, and Life and Well-Being competencies loaded higher than Neuroticism, Openness, and Extraversion. Whereas, in the study of Kamamoto et al. [61], Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness were positively associated, and Neuroticism was negatively associated with trait EI scores, consistent with the directions of the factor loadings of the GFP. Our research reveals that GFP was significantly associated with trait EI, similarly to other studies with children [61,63]. Therefore, our study reinforces the thesis that trait EI can be considered the same as GFP or, as Rushton and Irwing [34] (p. 146) put it, the culmination of the GFP. Research on trait EI revealed that children with higher scores show more adaptive coping styles and prosocial behaviors [52,72]. Therefore, GPF can be considered an equivalent of trait EI that conceptually overlaps with EI [35,59], likewise as a social efficacy factor [37,73,74]. In addition, a meta-analysis study developed by Roberts et al. [75] points out how personality traits can be modified through intervention. Therefore, it makes sense to develop social-emotional programs in schools at different stages of schooling.
Our study has some limitations that are important to note. First, we used questionnaires to obtain all the data. Also, the same respondents answered all the questionnaires. Monomethod and mono response studies tend to find artificially inflated correlations. In this study, the correlations are robust, and we believe that this eliminates the possibility of false correlations.

5. Conclusions

The novelty of our research, with respect to other studies, is that it was conducted with a sample of Spanish children between 9 and 13 years of age and used a different definition and measurement of trait EI known as the Emotional Development Questionnaire for Primary Education (CDE_9-13) [65].
Our results provide some support for the convergent validity of the B5 concerning trait EI. However, they do not support the discriminant validity between the B5 and trait EI. On the other hand, the correlations between trait EI and the B2 were confirmed in our study. Finally, our results confirm that the trait EI and the GFP are the same constructs. Since, by definition, people with high trait EI possess the ability to adapt and thrive in our society, this advantage also applies to the GFP. These conclusions are essential for the field of education as it implies and supports the need to introduce social and emotional development programs from the earliest years of schooling to promote adaptability and social efficacy.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, È.L.-C., N.P.-E. and A.A.; methodology, N.P.-E. and A.A; validation, N.P.-E. and A.A.; investigation, È.L.-C., N.P.-E. and A.A.; data curation, È.L.-C.; writing—original draft preparation, È.L.-C., N.P.-E. and A.A; writing—review and editing, È.L.-C., N.P.-E. and A.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Research Groups 2021 Grant from the Faculty of Education of the University of Barcelona.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki in 1964 and its later amendments. It followed the recommendations of the Bioethics Commission of the University of Barcelona.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available on request due to privacy and ethical restrictions.

Acknowledgments

The authors are particularly grateful to all schools, children, and their families for their participation in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Combined Factor analysis of trait EI and B5 items.
Table A1. Combined Factor analysis of trait EI and B5 items.
Components
12345
ITEM90.716
ITEM30.659
BFQ38−0.658
BFQ32−0.657
ITEM410.653
BFQ11−0.623
BFQ7−0.616
ITEM320.613
BFQ28−0.587
BFQ45−0.581
BFQ34−0.578
ITEM270.577
ITEM240.571
ITEM100.563
BFQ22−0.563 0.365
BFQ62−0.557 0.360 −0.334
ITEM380.556 0.305
BFQ52−0.556 0.346
ITEM370.550
ITEM220.549
BFQ65−0.540
ITEM250.536 0.319
ITEM60.534
ITEM330.524 0.348
BFQ63−0.523 0.399
BFQ490.5150.422
ITEM280.508 0.343
ITEM400.502
ITEM230.499 0.337
ITEM5498
BFQ30−0.497 0.392 −0.375
ITEM10.494
BFQ12−0.490 0.328 −0.347
BFQ48−0.487 0.344
BFQ3−0.487
ITEM180.484 0.419
BFQ25−0.484
ITEM360.474 0.370
BFQ13−0.472
ITEM120.472
BFQ20−0.469
ITEM310.467
ITEM80.464 0.345
BFQ51−0.457
BFQ57−0.453
BFQ26−0.452 −0.303
BFQ60.4450.392
ITEM160.440 0.319
ITEM390.438 0.315
BFQ2−0.438
BFQ5−0.437 0.300 −0.318
ITEM110.437
BFQ53−0.429
BFQ18−0.426
BFQ16−0.421
BFQ37−0.420 0.343
ITEM130.414
ITEM20.409 0.369
ITEM190.408 0.384−0.354
ITEM70.400
BFQ60−0.396
BFQ24−0.378 −0.336
BFQ410.3760.358
BFQ27−0.376
BFQ64−0.374
BFQ43−0.370
ITEM200.350
ITEM350.349
BFQ40−0.348 0.306
ITEM140.341
BFQ21−0.327
ITEM340.321
ITEM150.303
BFQ39
BFQ36
BFQ59
BFQ33
BFQ170.4330.616
BFQ150.4970.591
BFQ80.3820.578
ITEM21 0.567
ITEM290.3900.540
BFQ4 0.512
ITEM260.4000.502
ITEM300.3140.427
BFQ61 0.425
BFQ54 0.417
BFQ290.3060.396 −0.320
ITEM17 0.387
BFQ9
BFQ1
BFQ56
BFQ46−0.345 0.351 −0.308
BFQ44−0.309 0.326
BFQ58 0.387 −0.441
ITEM4 0.363−0.415
BFQ310.336 −0.379
BFQ47−0.315 0.375
BFQ35 0.311 0.354
BFQ55 0.310 0.343
BFQ50 0.305
BFQ42
BFQ19−0.3590.318 0.439
BFQ23−0.361 −0.363
BFQ10 0.338
BFQ14
Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

References

  1. OCDE. El Trabajo de la OCDE Sobre Educación y Competencias; OECD: Paris, France, 2019; Available online: https://www.oecd.org/education/El-trabajo-de-la-ocde-sobre-educacion-y-competencias.pdf (accessed on 3 March 2022).
  2. OECD. Beyond Academic Learning: First Results from the Survey of Social and Emotional Skills; OECD: Paris, France, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Durlak, J.A.; Weissberg, R.P. Promoting Social and Emotional Development Is an Essential Part of Students’ Education. Hum. Dev. 2011, 54, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Taylor, R.D.; Oberle, E.; Durlak, J.A.; Weissberg, R.P. Promoting Positive Youth Development Through School-Based Social and Emotional Learning Interventions: A Meta-Analysis of Follow-Up Effects. Child. Dev. 2017, 88, 1156–1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Corcoran, R.P.; Cheung, A.C.; Kim, E.; Xie, C. Effective universal school-based social and emotional learning programs for improving academic achievement: A systematic review and meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Educ. Res. Rev. 2018, 25, 56–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Keefer, K.V.; Parker, J.D.A.; Saklofske, D.H. Emotional Intelligence in Education. Integrating Research with Practice; Springer International: Cham, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. MacCann, C.; Jiang, Y.; Brown, L.E.R.; Double, K.S.; Bucich, M.; Minbashian, A. Emotional intelligence predicts academic performance: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2020, 146, 150–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Lübke, L.; Pinquart, M.; Schwinger, M. The Role of Flexibility in the Realization of Inclusive Education. Sustainability 2021, 13, 4452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chiș, A.; Rusu, A.S. School-Based Interventions for Developing Emotional Abilities in Adolescents: A Systematic Review. In Education, Reflection, Development, Proceedings of the ERD 2018 Education Reflection Development, Cluj-Napoca, Romania, 6–8 July 2018; Available online: https://www.europeanproceedings.com/book-series/EpSBS/books/vol63-erd-2018/table-of-contents (accessed on 12 May 2022).
  10. Sande, M.C.; Fekkes, M.; Kocken, P.L.; Diekstra, R.F.; Reis, R.; Gravesteijn, C. Do universal social and emotional learning programs for secondary school students enhance the competencies they address? A systematic review. Psychol. Sch. 2019, 56, 1545–1567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Patafio, B.; Miller, P.; Baldwin, R.; Taylor, N.; Hyder, S. A systematic mapping review of interventions to improve adolescent mental health literacy, attitudes and behaviours. Early Interv. Psychiatry 2021, 15, 1470–1501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Sánchez-Gómez, M.; Oliver, A.; Adelantado-Renau, M.; Breso, E. Inteligencia emocional y ansiedad en adolescentes: Una propuesta práctica en el aula. Rev. Nuevas Tecnol. Soc. 2020, 91, 74–89. [Google Scholar]
  13. Piqueras, J.A.; Mateu-Martínez, O.; Cejudo, J.; Pérez-González, J.-C. Pathways into Psychosocial Adjustment in Children: Modeling the Effects of Trait Emotional Intelligence, Social-Emotional Problems, and Gender. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Dobrin, N.; Kállay, E. The investigation of the short-term effects of a primary prevention program targeting the development of emotional and social competencies in preschoolers. Cogn. Brain Behav. 2013, 17, 15–34. [Google Scholar]
  15. Domitrovich, C.E.; Durlak, J.A.; Staley, K.C.; Weissberg, R.P. Social-Emotional Competence: An Essential Factor for Promoting Positive Adjustment and Reducing Risk in School Children. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 408–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Pérez-González, J.-C.; Qualter, P. Emotional intelligence and emotional education in school years. In An Introduction to Emotional Intelligence; Dacree Pool, L., Qualter, P., Eds.; Wiley: Chichester, UK, 2018; pp. 81–104. [Google Scholar]
  17. Pérez Escoda, N.; Filella Guiu, G. Educación emocional para el desarrollo de competencias emocionales en niños y adolescentes. Prax. Saber 2019, 10, 23–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Van Rooy, D.L.; Viswesvaran, C. Assessing Emotional Intelligence in Adults: A Review of the Most Popular Measures. In Educating People to Be Emotionally Intelligent; Bar-On, R., Maree, J.G., Elias, M.J., Eds.; Praeger: Westport, MO, USA, 2007; pp. 259–272. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hidalgo Fuentes, S.; Martínez Álvarez, I.; Sospedra Baeza, M.J. La relación entre inteligencia emocional y personalidad en estudiantes universitarios españoles. Apunt. Psicol. 2021, 39, 87–93. Available online: https://apuntesdepsicologia.es/index.php/revista/article/view/903 (accessed on 10 January 2022).
  20. Hampson, S.E. Mechanisms by Which Childhood Personality Traits Influence Adult Well-Being. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 17, 264–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Tackett, J.L.; Slobodskaya, H.R.; Mar, R.A.; Deal, J.; Halverson, C.F.; Baker, S.R.; Pavlopoulos, V.; Besevegis, E. The Hierarchical Structure of Childhood Personality in Five Countries: Continuity from Early Childhood to Early Adolescence. J. Personal. 2011, 80, 847–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Merlyn-Sacoto, M.-F.; Acurio-Velasco, C.V.; Cabezas-Guerra, C.B.; Orbe-Nájera, C.E.; Riera-Vásquez, W.L. Rasgos de personalidad que afectan el desempeño de los profesores de la PUCE matriz en las funciones de docencia e investigación. Estud. Pedagógicos 2018, 44, 331–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Bisquerra, R.; Pérez-González, J.C.; García Navarro, E. Inteligencia Emocional en la Educación; Síntesis: Madrid, Spain, 2015; pp. 51–71. [Google Scholar]
  24. Eysenck, H.J.; Eysenck, M.W. Personality and Individual Differences; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  25. Costa, P.T., Jr.; McCrae, R.R. Revised NE0 Personality Inventory and NE0 Five-Factor Inventory Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  26. De Feyter, T.; Caers, R.; Vigna, C.; Berings, D. Unraveling the impact of the Big Five personality traits on academic performance: The moderating and mediating effects of self-efficacy and academic motivation. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2012, 22, 439–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Digman, J.M. Higher-order factors of the Big Five. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 73, 1246–1256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. DeYoung, C.G.; Peterson, J.B.; Higgins, D.M. Higher-order factors of the Big Five predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2002, 33, 533–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. DeYoung, C.G.; Hasher, L.; Djikic, M.; Criger, B.; Peterson, J. Morning people are stable people: Circadian rhythm and the higher-order factors of the Big Five. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2007, 43, 267–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Şimşek, F. Higher Order Structure of Personality and Mental Health: Does General Affectivity Matter? J. Personal. Assess. 2013, 96, 226–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Erazo, G.; Santos, D.; Pilco, A.; Llanga, J. Análisis de validez y confiabilidad del Tests de personalidad Big Five en estudiantes universitarios de Ecuador. Rev. Puce 2019, 109, 25–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gerosa, F. Personalidad, Autoestima y Desempeño Académico. Una aproximación complementaria a partir de la Teoría de los Cinco Factores de la personalidad. Psicosocial 2016, 2, 46–53. Available online: https://publicaciones.sociales.uba.ar/index.php/psicologiasocial/article/view/2146 (accessed on 18 February 2021).
  33. Caselles, A.; Micó, J.C.; Amigó, S. Factor General de personalidad: Historia de una aventura interdisciplinar. Rev. Int. Sist. 2018, 22, 7–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Rushton, J.P.; Irwing, P. The General Factor of Personality: Normal and Abnormal. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences; Chamorro-Premuzic, T., von Stumm, S., Furnham, A., Eds.; Wiley-Blackwell: London, UK, 2011; pp. 132–161. [Google Scholar]
  35. Van der Linden, D.; Pekaar, K.A.; Bakker, A.B.; Schermer, J.A.; Vernon, P.A.; Dunkel, C.S.; Petrides, K.V. Overlap between the general factor of personality and emotional intelligence: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 36–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Figueredo, A.J.; Woodley, M.A.; Jacobs, J.W. The General Factor of Personality: A Hierarchical Life History Model. In The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, 2nd ed.; Buss, D.M., Ed.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2016; Volume 2, pp. 943–967. [Google Scholar]
  37. Van der Linden, D.; Dunkel, C.S.; Petrides, K. The General Factor of Personality as social effectiveness: Review of the literature. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 101, 98–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Antoñanzas, J. The Relationship of Personality, Emotional Intelligence, and Aggressiveness in Students: A Study Using the Big Five Personality Questionnaire for Children and Adults (BFQ-NA). Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2020, 11, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Markey, P.M.; Markey, C.N.; Tinsley, B.J. Children’s behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2004, 30, 423–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Soto, C.J.; John, O.P.; Gosling, S.D.; Potter, J. Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 330–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Salovey, P.; Mayer, J.D. Emotional Intelligence. Imagin. Cogn. Personal. 1990, 9, 185–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mayer, J.D.; Salovey, P. What is emotional intelligence. In Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Implications for Educators; Salovey, P., Sluyter, D., Eds.; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
  43. Petrides, K.V.; Furnham, A. Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation with reference to established trait taxonomies. Eur. J. Personal. 2001, 15, 425–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Sánchez-Ruiz, M.J.; Pérez-González, J.-C.; Petrides, K.V. Trait emotional intelligence profiles of students from different university faculties. Aust. J. Psychol. 2010, 62, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Petrides, K.V.; Mikolajczak, M.; Mavroveli, S.; Sanchez-Ruiz, M.-J.; Furnham, A.; Pérez-González, J.-C. Developments in Trait Emotional Intelligence Research. Emot. Rev. 2016, 8, 335–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Andrei, F.; Mancini, G.; Baldaro, B.; Trombini, E.; Agnoli, S. A systematic review on the predictive utility of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue). Appl. Psychol. Bull. 2014, 271, 2–29. [Google Scholar]
  47. Mavroveli, S.; Sánchez-Ruiz, M.J. Trait emotional intelligence influences on academic achievement and school behaviour. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 81, 112–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Mancini, G. Trait Emotional Intelligence and Draw-A-Person Emotional Indicators: A First Study on 8-Year-Old Italian Children. Child Indic. Res. 2018, 12, 1629–1641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Andrei, F.; Mancini, G.; Mazzoni, E.; Russo, P.; Baldaro, B. Social status and its link with personality dimensions, trait emotional intelligence, and scholastic achievement in children and early adolescents. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 42, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mancini, G.; Andrei, F.; Mazzoni, E.; Biolcati, R.; Baldaro, B.; Trombini, E. Brief report: Trait emotional intelligence, peer nominations, and scholastic achievement in adolescence. J. Adolesc. 2017, 59, 129–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Agnoli, S.; Pittarello, A.; Hysenbelli, D.; Rubaltelli, E. “Give, but Give until It Hurts”: The Modulatory Role of Trait Emotional Intelligence on the Motivation to Help. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0130704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Frederickson, N.; Petrides, K.; Simmonds, E. Trait emotional intelligence as a predictor of socioemotional outcomes in early adolescence. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 323–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Jiménez Morales, M.I.; López-Zafra, E. Inteligencia emocional y rendimiento escolar: Estado actual de la cuestión. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 2009, 41, 69–79. [Google Scholar]
  54. Vernon, P.A.; Villani, V.C.; Schermer, J.A.; Petrides, K.V. Phenotypic and Genetic Associations Between the Big Five and Trait Emotional Intelligence. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2008, 11, 524–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Úbeda, A.P.; Lillo, V.B.; García, C.F.; Melero, M.J.R. La relación entre la inteligencia emocional y la personalidad en estudiantes de educación secundaria. Int. J. Dev. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 2, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Belmonte, V.M. Inteligencia Emocional y Creatividad: Factores Predictores del Rendimiento Académico. Ph.D. Thesis, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  57. Musek, J. Factor general de la personalidad: Diez años después. Psihol. Teme 2017, 26, 61–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Pérez-González, J.C.; Sánchez-Ruiz, M.J. Trait emotional intelligence anchored within the Big Five, Big Two and Big One frameworks. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 65, 53–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Van der Linden, D.; Schermer, J.A.; de Zeeuw, E.; Dunkel, C.S.; Pekaar, K.A.; Bakker, A.B.; Vernon, P.A.; Petrides, K.V. Overlap Between the General Factor of Personality and Trait Emotional Intelligence: A Genetic Correlation Study. Behav. Genet. 2018, 48, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Alegre, A.; Pérez-Escoda, N.; López-Cassá, E. The Relationship Between Trait Emotional Intelligence and Personality. Is Trait EI Really Anchored Within the Big Five, Big Two and Big One Frameworks? Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Kawamoto, T.; Kubota, A.K.; Sakakibara, R.; Muto, S.; Tonegawa, A.; Komatsu, S.; Endo, T. The General Factor of Personality (GFP), trait emotional intelligence, and problem behaviors in Japanese teens. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 171, 110480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Caspi, A.; Roberts, B.W.; Shiner, R.L. Personality Development: Stability and Change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2005, 56, 453–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Kawamoto, T.; Endo, T. Personality change in adolescence: Results from a Japanese sample. J. Res. Personal. 2015, 57, 32–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Soto, C.J. The Little Six Personality Dimensions from Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents’ Reports. J. Personal. 2015, 84, 409–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Pérez-Escoda, N.; López-Cassà, È.; Alegre, A. Emotional Development Questionnaire for Primary Education (CDE_9–13). Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Barbaranelli, C.; Carpara, G.; Rabasca, A.; Pastorelli, C. A questionnaire for measuring the Big Five in late Childhood. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003, 34, 645–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Del Barrio, V.; Carrasco, M.A.; Holgado, F.P. BFQ-NA Cuestionario de los Cinco Grandes para Niños y Adolescents; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  68. Bisquerra, R.; Pérez Escoda, N. Las competencias emocionales. Educ. XXI 2007, 10, 61–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Mavroveli, S.; Petrides, K.V.; Shove, C.; Whitehead, A. Investigation of the construct of trait emotional intelligence in children. Eur. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 17, 516–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Petrides, K.V. Technical Manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaires (TEIQue), 1st ed.; Psychometric Laboratory: London, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  71. Komatsu, S.; Hakoda, Y. The Manual for the Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire; Toyo Physical: Fukuoka, Japan, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  72. García-Sancho, E.; Salguero, J.M.; Fernández-Berrocal, P. Relationship between emotional intelligence and aggression: A systematic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2014, 19, 584–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Loehlin, J.C. How general across inventories is a general factor of personality? J. Res. Personal. 2012, 46, 258–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Dunkel, C.S.; van der Linden, D.; Beaver, K.M.; Woodley, M.A. Using a prison sample to assess the association between the general factor of personality and general intelligence. Intelligence 2014, 47, 72–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Roberts, B.W.; Luo, J.; Briley, D.A.; Chow, P.I.; Su, R.; Hill, P.L. A systematic review of personality trait change through intervention. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 117–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Inter-correlations among trait EI, the B5, the B2, and the GFP.
Table 1. Inter-correlations among trait EI, the B5, the B2, and the GFP.
GPFStabilityPlasticityOCEANTotal EI
GPF 1
Stability 0.783 **1
Plasticity 0.423 **0.73 **1
Openness 0.808 *0.650 **0.836 **1
Conscientiousness 0.912 **0.862 **0.807 *0.782 **1
Extraversion 0.584 **0.568 **0.833 **0.392 **0.563 **1
Agreeableness 0.851 **0.762 **0.699 **0.625 **0.761 **0.541 **1
Neuroticism −0.430 **0.135 *−0.261 **−0.326−0.327 **−0.109 **−0.361 **1
Emotional Awareness0.591 **0.484 **0.498 **0.436 **0.509 **0.394 **0.633 **−0.266 **0.861 **
Emotional Regulation0.585 **0.266 **0.406 **0.425 **0.531 **0.252 **0.536 **−0.294 **0.756 **
Autonomy0.348 **0.334 **0.367 **0.334 **0.394 **0.282 **0.335 **−0.257 **0.727 **
Social Competence0.543 **0.415 **0.453 **0.417 **0.470 **0.339 **0.622 **−0.335 **0.839 **
Life and Wellbeing Competencies0.567 **0.375 **0.413 **0.344 **0.511 **0.345 **0.528 **−0.398 **0.828 **
Total emotional intelligence (0.89)−0.693 **0.463 **0.556 **0.491 **0.600 *0.397 **0.700 **−0.484 **1
Note. n = 259. GPF = General Personality Factor, O = Openness, C = Conscientiousness, E = Extraversion, A = Agreeableness, N = Neuroticism, Total EI = Emotional Intelligence total coefficient. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. Multiple Regression of trait EI onto the B5 and trait EI onto the B2.
Table 2. Multiple Regression of trait EI onto the B5 and trait EI onto the B2.
Section ModelUnstandardized CoefficientsStandardized Coefficientstp
BStd. ErrorBeta
1(Constant)2.7770.488 5.6860.000
Conscientiousness0.0120.0090.1091.3250.187
Openness0.0070.0140.0340.5070.613
Extraversion0.0090.0110.0400.7770.438
Agreeableness0.0960.0130.4777.2410.000
Neuroticism−0.0430.007−0.284−6.3780.000
2(Constant)1.0030.567 1.7680.078
Stability0.0100.0060.1221.6160.107
Plasticity0.0610.0100.4676.1750.000
Note. n = 259.
Table 3. Principal Component Analysis of trait EI and B5 dimensions.
Table 3. Principal Component Analysis of trait EI and B5 dimensions.
Component
1
Agreeableness0.849
Responsibility0.808
Emotional Awareness0.803
Social Competence0.799
Life & Wellbeing Competencies0.778
Emotional Regulation0.728
Openness0.698
Autonomy0.688
Extroversion0.568
Neuroticism−0.533
Note. n = 259.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

López-Cassà, È.; Pérez-Escoda, N.; Alegre, A. The Relationship between Children’s Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Big Five, Big Two and Big One Personality Traits. Educ. Sci. 2022, 12, 491. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12070491

AMA Style

López-Cassà È, Pérez-Escoda N, Alegre A. The Relationship between Children’s Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Big Five, Big Two and Big One Personality Traits. Education Sciences. 2022; 12(7):491. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12070491

Chicago/Turabian Style

López-Cassà, Èlia, Núria Pérez-Escoda, and Alberto Alegre. 2022. "The Relationship between Children’s Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Big Five, Big Two and Big One Personality Traits" Education Sciences 12, no. 7: 491. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12070491

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop