Effects and Prerequisites of Self-Generation in Inquiry-Based Learning
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Self-Generation within Inquiry-Based Learning
1.2. Prerequisites of Self-Generation within an Authentic Learning Environment
1.3. Research Questions
- Q1
- Does self-generating content knowledge during inquiry affect long-term retention among 6th and 7th graders?
- Q2
- Does additional feedback improve the long-term retention of generated information?
- Q3
- Does successful self-generation enhance the effect?
- Q4
- Does additional feedback (including a prompt to revise the generated answer) enhance successful self-generation during learning?
- Q5
- Do individual learning abilities and characteristics (e.g., need for cognition, cognitive load, reading competency, self-generation success) influence the learning outcome?
2. Method
2.1. Participants
2.2. Research Design
2.3. Learning Content
3. Materials
3.1. Introductory Session
3.2. Inquiry Task
4. Procedure
4.1. Introductory Session
4.2. Inquiry Task
5. Instruments
5.1. Learning Outcome
5.2. Learners’ Self-Generation Success
5.3. Learners’ Abilities
5.4. Data Analysis
6. Results
6.1. Retention by Treatment and Time (Q1, Q2)
6.2. Success in Self-Generation (Performance Success) (Q3, Q4)
6.3. Learners’ Abilities and Prerequisites (Q5)
7. Discussion
7.1. Self-Generating with Feedback and Rereading Both Promote Long-Term Retention in Inquiry-Based Learning (Q1)
7.2. Success in Self-Generating is Critical for Long-Term Retention (Q3)
7.3. Feedback Is a Key Prerequisite for Self-Generating Complex Content Knowledge (Q2, Q4)
7.4. Generating and Rereading Place Different Demands on Learners (Q5)
7.5. Limitations and Directions for Future Research
8. Implications
9. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Fit Values of the Path Models
Appendix B. German School System
Appendix C. The Term “Hypothesis”
References
- Hmelo-Silver, C.E.; Duncan, R.G.; Chinn, C.A. Scaffolding and Achievement in Problem-Based and Inquiry Learning: A Response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006). Educ. Psychol. 2007, 42, 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirschner, P.A.; Sweller, J.; Clark, R.E. Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 41, 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.; Renkl, A.; Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 38, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dean, D., Jr.; Kuhn, D. Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view. Sci. Ed. 2007, 91, 384–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, L.L. On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1978, 17, 649–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slamecka, N.J.; Graf, P. The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1978, 4, 592–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crutcher, R.J.; Healy, A.F. Cognitive operations and the generation effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1989, 15, 669–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNamara, D.S.; Healy, A.F. A Procedural Explanation of the Generation Effect: The Use of an Operand Retrieval Strategy for Multiplication and Addition Problems. J. Mem. Lang. 1995, 34, 399–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bertsch, S.; Pesta, B.J.; Wiscott, R.; McDaniel, M.A. The generation effect: A meta-analytic review. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 35, 201–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Klahr, D.; Dunbar, K. Dual space search during scientific reasoning. Cogn. Sci. 1988, 12, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klahr, D. Exploring Science: The Cognition and Development of Discovery Processes; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; Available online: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&scope=site&db=nlebk&db=nlabk&AN=32641 (accessed on 28 September 2020).
- Bell, R.L.; Smetana, L.; Binns, I. Simplifying inquiry instruction: Assessing the inquiry level of classroom activities. Sci. Teach. 2005, 72, 30–33. [Google Scholar]
- Chi, M.T.H. Active-constructive-interactive: A conceptual framework for differentiating learning activities. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2009, 1, 73–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Abrams, E.; Silva, P.C.; Southerland, S.A. (Eds.) Contemporary research in education. In Inquiry in the Classroom: Realities and Opportunities; IAP: Charlotte, NC, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Schwab, J. The teaching of science as enquiry. In The Teaching of Science; Schwab, J., Brandwein, P.F., Eds.; Havard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1962; pp. 1–103. [Google Scholar]
- Colburn, A. An inquiry primer. Sci. Scope 2000, 23, 42–44. [Google Scholar]
- Zion, M.; Mendelovici, R. Moving from structured to open inquiry: Challenges and limits. Sci. Educ. Int. 2012, 23, 383–399. [Google Scholar]
- Bjork, E.L.; Bjork, R. Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In Psychology and the Real World: Essays Illustrating Fundamental Contributions to Society; Gernsbacher, M.A., Ed.; Worth: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 59–68. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, O.; Kalyuga, S.; Sweller, J. Relations between the worked example and generation effects on immediate and delayed tests. Learn. Instr. 2016, 45, 20–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sweller, J.; van Merriënboer, J.J.G.; Paas, F.G.W.C. Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1998, 10, 251–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; Chandler, P. Why Some Material Is Difficult to Learn. Cogn. Instr. 1994, 12, 185–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, I.; Mayer, J.; Malai, D. Self-generation in the context of inquiry-based learning. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardiner, J.M.; Hampton, J.A. Semantic memory and the generation effect: Some tests of the lexical activation hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1985, 11, 732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nairne, J.S.; Pusen, C.; Widner, R.L. Representation in the mental lexicon: Implications for theories of the generation effect. Mem. Cogn. 1985, 13, 183–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDaniel, M.A.; Waddill, P.J.; Einstein, G.O. A contextual account of the generation effect: A three-factor theory. J. Mem. Lang. 1988, 27, 521–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lutz, J.; Briggs, A.; Cain, K. An examination of the value of the generation effect for learning new material. J. Gen. Psychol. 2003, 130, 171–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McElroy, L.A.; Slamecka, N.J. Memorial consequences of generating nonwords: Implications for semantic-memory interpretations of the generation effect. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1982, 21, 249–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foos, P.W.; Mora, J.J.; Tkacz, S. Student study techniques and the generation effect. J. Educ. Psychol. 1994, 86, 567–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richland, L.E.; Bjork, R.A.; Finley, J.R.; Linn, M.C. Linking Cognitive Science to Education: Generation and Interleaving Effects. In Proceedings of the Twenty-Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society; Bara, B.G., Barsalou, L.W., Bucciarelli, M., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 1850–1855. [Google Scholar]
- Clark, D.; Linn, M.C. Designing for Knowledge Integration: The Impact of Instructional Time. J. Learn. Sci. 2003, 12, 451–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; Ayres, P.; Kalyuga, S. Cognitive Load Theory Explorations in the Learning Sciences, Instructional Systems and Performance Technologies, 1st ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011. Available online: http://www.loc.gov/catdir/enhancements/fy1316/2011922376-d.html (accessed on 28 September 2020).
- Metcalfe, J.; Kornell, N. Principles of cognitive science in education: The effects of generation, errors, and feedback. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2007, 14, 225–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, S.H.K.; McDermott, K.B.; Roediger, H.L. Test format and corrective feedback modify the effect of testing on long-term retention. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2007, 19, 528–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hattie, J.; Timperley, H. The Power of Feedback. Rev. Educ. Res. 2007, 77, 81–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balzer, W.K.; Doherty, M.E.; O’Connor, R. Effects of cognitive feedback on performance. Psychol. Bull. 1989, 106, 410–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lysakowski, R.S.; Walberg, H.J. Instructional Effects of Cues, Participation, and Corrective Feedback: A Quantitative Synthesis. Am. Educ. Res. J. 1982, 19, 559–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harackiewicz, J.M.; Manderlink, G.; Sansone, C. Rewarding pinball wizardry: Effects of evaluation and cue value on intrinsic interest. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 47, 287–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kulik, J.A.; Kulik, C.L.C. Timing of Feedback and Verbal Learning. Rev. Educ. Res. 1988, 58, 79–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pashler, H.; Cepeda, N.J.; Wixted, J.T.; Rohrer, D. When does feedback facilitate learning of words? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2005, 31, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cacioppo, J.T.; Petty, R.E.; Feinstein, J.A.; Jarvis, W.B.G. Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 119, 197–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cazan, A.M.; Indreica, S.E. Need for cognition and approaches to learning among university students. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. J. 2014, 127, 134–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weißgerber, S.C.; Reinhard, M.-A.; Schindler, S. Learning the hard way: Need for cognition influences attitudes towards and self-reported use of desirable learning difficulties. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 38, 176–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G-Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behav.ioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meier, M.; Wulff, C. Daphnia magna as the ultimate classroom organism: Implementing scientific investigations into school practice. In Animal Science, Issues and Professions. Daphnia: Biology and Mathematics Perspectives; El-Doma, M., Ed.; Nova Science Publ.: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 225–244. [Google Scholar]
- Flesch, R. A new readability yardstick. J. Appl. Psychol. 1948, 32, 221–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Künsting, J. Effekte von Zielqualität und Zielspezifität auf selbstreguliert-entdeckendes Lernen durch Experimentieren. Ph.D. Thesis, Universität Duisburg-Essen, Essen, The Netherlands, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Preckel, F. Assessing Need for Cognition in Early Adolescence. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2014, 30, 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lienert, G.; Raatz, U. Testaufbau und Testanalyse; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Landis, J.; Koch, G.G. The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics 1977, 33, 159–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Eid, M.; Gollwitzer, M.; Schmitt, M. Statistik und –forschungsmethoden; Beltz Verlagsgruppe: Weinheim, Germany, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Geiser, C. Datenanalyse mit Mplus. Eine anwendungsorientierte Einführung; VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften/Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden GmbH Wiesbaden: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Renkl, A. Wissenserwerb. In Springer-Lehrbuch. Pädagogische Psychologie, 2nd ed.; Wild, E., Möller, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2015; pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, L.L. Remembering the data: Analyzing interactive processes in reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1983, 22, 485–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosner, Z.A.; Elman, J.A.; Shimamura, A.P. The generation effect: Activating broad neural circuits during memory encoding. Cortex 2013, 49, 1901–1909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Furtak, E.M.; Seidel, T.; Iverson, H.; Briggs, D.C. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Studies of Inquiry-Based Science Teaching. Rev. Educ. Res. 2012, 82, 300–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kant, J.M.; Scheiter, K.; Oschatz, K. How to sequence video modeling examples and inquiry tasks to foster scientific reasoning. Learn. Instr. 2017, 52, 46–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Phases | Levels of Inquiry | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Verification | Structured | Guided | Open | ||
Source of the question | Given | Given | Given | Open | |
Data collection methods | Given | Given | Open | Open | |
Interpretation of esults | Given | Open | Open | Open | |
Self-generation requirement | | ||||
low | high | ||||
Instructional support | | ||||
high | low | ||||
Cognitive load | | ||||
low | high |
Generation with Feedback | Generation | Rereading | |
---|---|---|---|
Posttest 1 (%) | 73.33 (20.33) | 62.99 (17.75) | 81.92 (8.75) |
Posttest 2 (%) | 70.00 (20.42) | 56.37 (21.33) | 77.42 (17.28) |
Self-generation success | 12.97 (4.37) | 8.62 (3.86) | - |
N | 34 | 40 (35 a) | 24 |
Variable | Short-Term (T1) | Long-Term (T2) | Self-Generation Success (GS) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
GF | RR | GF | RR | GF | |
GS | 0.52 *** | – | – | – | |
ind. (T1) | 0.34 *** | ||||
NFC | 0.46 *** | ||||
ind. (GS) | 0.24 * | 0.19 * | |||
CL | −0.37 *** | ||||
ind. (T1) | −0.27 ** | ||||
RC | 0.45 * | 0.44 * | |||
T1 | – | 0.54 *** | – | ||
R2 | 0.47 *** | 0.20 * | 0.43 *** | 0.20 * | – |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Streich, I.; Mayer, J. Effects and Prerequisites of Self-Generation in Inquiry-Based Learning. Educ. Sci. 2020, 10, 277. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10100277
Streich I, Mayer J. Effects and Prerequisites of Self-Generation in Inquiry-Based Learning. Education Sciences. 2020; 10(10):277. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10100277
Chicago/Turabian StyleStreich, Irina, and Jürgen Mayer. 2020. "Effects and Prerequisites of Self-Generation in Inquiry-Based Learning" Education Sciences 10, no. 10: 277. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10100277
APA StyleStreich, I., & Mayer, J. (2020). Effects and Prerequisites of Self-Generation in Inquiry-Based Learning. Education Sciences, 10(10), 277. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10100277