Next Article in Journal
Phonation Patterns in Spanish Vowels: Spectral and Spectrographic Analysis
Next Article in Special Issue
A Constructionist and Corpus-Based Approach to Formulas in Old English Poetry
Previous Article in Journal
L1–L2 Influence in Intonation: A Case of Russophone Immigrants in Brazil
Previous Article in Special Issue
For-Verbs in Old English
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Updating Old English Dative–Genitives: A Diachronic Construction Grammar Account

by
Juan G. Vázquez-González
Department of English Philology, Faculty of Humanities, University of Huelva, 21007 Huelva, Andalucía, Spain
Languages 2024, 9(6), 213; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9060213
Submission received: 16 April 2024 / Revised: 29 May 2024 / Accepted: 31 May 2024 / Published: 11 June 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Corpus-Based Linguistics of Old English)

Abstract

:
This article conducts a corpus linguistics analysis of the dative–genitive subconstruction within the broader context of Old English double object complementation. The ditransitive construction in Old English has traditionally been perceived as a network of alternating subconstructions, including dat-acc, acc-dat, acc-gen, dat-gen, and acc-acc, as the most productive variants. Recent literature has primarily focused on dat-accs and acc-dats because they are the most productive patterns across the history of English, giving also rise to the current ditransitive construction. However, the less productive case frames have received considerably less recent attention. This work, part of an ongoing investigation aimed at creating an OE dat-gen database, builds upon Visser’s list, verified and implemented by findings obtained from a search conducted in the Dictionary of Old English Web Corpus. We obtain 88 verb types and 443 tokens, incorporating 19 new verb types and 260 tokens into the database. More significantly, we offer a detailed description of the conceptual domains and verb classes associated with OE dat-gens, which display a semantics characterized by the presence or absence of actual transfer, as well as transitions from literal to metaphorical transfer, with speech verbs playing a significant role.

1. Aims and Scope

In modern English, the ditransitive construction is characterized by a threefold combination of subject plus objects 1 and 2, corresponding to the semantic roles of agent, recipient, and theme, whether literal or metaphorical—such as in Sally baked her sister a cake and Bob told Joe a story (Goldberg 1995, pp. 141, 143). Word order now strictly governs this construction, favoring the indirect-direct object pattern over its reverse variant, which is primarily confined to Northern Englishes: She gave it the man (Hughes et al. 2013, p. 20; Yañez-Bouza and Denison 2015). A thousand years ago in Old English (OE), the case system allowed for greater variation as functions were marked by case endings. The two double object patterns mentioned above were not only possible but actively used. In fact, one of the most recent contributions to the analysis of double object constructions in OE suggests that acc-dats and dat-accs function as an alternation (Levin 1993) on their own terms (de Cuypere 2015a). Considering the current state as the endpoint, de Cuypere (2015b) recently conducted a study of the OE to-dative construction. Nevertheless, and despite their extremely high productivity, dat-accs/acc-dats represent just one aspect of the case frame system traditionally associated with ditransitives in OE. In what follows, I provide examples for the remaining case frames, which the literature assumes to be less productive (Visser 1963, pp. 621–35; Mitchell 1985, p. 452). These quotations are extracted from Visser (1963, pp. 608–11):
(1)& gif man    cyninges   ðegn     beteo manslihtes, […]       [acc-gen]
If some.NOM king.GEN.servant.ACC sues   murder.GEN
‘and if someone accuses a king’s thane of murder, …’ (LawAGu B14.5; eWS)
(2)Me hingrode. and ge     me    ætes     forwyrndon;[dat-gen]
I was angry and you.NOM me.DAT food.GEN denied
‘I was hungry and you refused to give me food’   (CHom II, 7 B1.2.8; lWS)
(3)[…] hine axodon þæt bigspell   þa   twelfe    þe mid him wæron. [acc-acc]
him.ACC asked the.parable.ACC the.twelve.NOM who with him were
‘the twelve that were with him asked him about the parable.’
(Mk WSCp B8.4.3.2; lWS)
In Old English, as observed in examples (1)–(3) above, the ditransitive construction encompasses at least three more distinct case frames: acc-gens, dat-gens, and acc-accs, ordered according to their assumed productivity. This suggests that the scope of the OE ditransitive construction includes a minimum of four major subconstructions, with the possibility of more. In this context, Visser includes another case frame, acc-dat(abl), which is also acknowledged by Mitchell (1985, p. 452). In this frame, the dative functions as ablative, partially overlapping with the instrumental case (Visser 1963, pp. 618–20):
(4)Ahrede me    hearmcwidum   heanra manna
Rid   me.ACC reproaches.DAT   ignoble.men.GEN
‘Free me from the insults of miserable men.’ (PPs A5; lWS, poet.)
Finally, de Cuypere (2015a, p. 9) hints at the possibility of finding dat-dat combinations, which would not be surprising given their existence in other historical Germanic languages like Old Norse–Icelandic (Barðdal et al. 2011, pp. 70–76). Although lying relatively nearby, prepositional object constructions also compete with the previous variants in the space of double object complementation:
(5)[…] þæt se man wandaþ þæt he hi æfre asecgge,
That the man turns away that he them.ACC ever tell,
buton se mæssepreost hie æt him geacsige.[acc-prep]
unless the.priest.NOM these.ACC to.PREP him.DAT ask
‘[…] that the man flinches from ever confessing his sins unless the
priest may ask about them to him’ (HomS 14 [BlHom 4] B3.2.14; lWS)
(6)hu dear se gripan on ða scire ðæt he ærendige oðrum monnum to Gode [dat-prep]
how dare he seize to the job that he.NOM intercede other.men.DAT to God.PP
‘(Or) how dare he (a bishop) seize the responsibility of interceding to God for other
men he who…’ (CP B9.1.3; eWS)
The two quotations selected involve two speech verbs operating ditransitively, with the difference being that in these double object constructions, one object is introduced prepositionally. The two patterns, acc-prep and dat-prep, are acknowledged by Visser (1963, pp. 637–39) and Mitchell (1985, p. 452) as alternatives to the case frames analyzed above. As observed in quotation (5), the preposition involved does not necessarily have to match the formal predecessor of the current ‘to’. This extremely variegated framework for double object complementation may even accommodate cases of prep-prep, where both objects are used prepositionally, and instances of triple object complementation (Mitchell 1985, p. 453)—for an example of dat-gen-prep in our corpus, see quotation (31) in Section 5.7 following.
It is against the backdrop of this complex scenario that the present work arises—a corpus linguistics study of dat-gens based on the Dictionary of Old English Web Corpus (DOEWC; Healey et al. 2015). To my knowledge, the recent literature on ditransitives has concentrated on the highest-frequency case frame(s) (acc-dat: de Cuypere 2015a; dat-acc: Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 555–620) while it is pending a much-needed corpus linguistics update of the remaining accepted case frame options and prepositional double object constructions.
In this work, we focus on dat-gens, an area of research relatively neglected during the last few decades. This serves as the first step toward reconstructing the full scope of the OE ditransitive construction. dat-gens are assumed to be less productive than acc-dats, dat-accs, and acc-gens, and their semantics tend to be succinctly described as being related to verbs of granting, thanking, refusing, and depriving. Aiming for the development of a dat-gen database in the near future, we begin with Visser’s list of verb types and tokens, augmenting it through a series of proximity searches in DOEWC, ultimately adding 19 unacknowledged verb types and 260 tokens to the future database.
The increase in the dat-gen data, both in terms of verb types and tokens, undoubtedly contributes significantly to our understanding of how this subconstruction operates. A second major finding, derived from the expanded dataset and more noteworthy, is that dat-gens distribute themselves into the same conceptual areas used by dat-accs (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 571–92). Accordingly, all the verb types currently comprising the database are directly related to most of the verb classes and cognitive domains used by dat-accs. While the overlap is not complete, as dat-gens obviously display a distinct semantics of their own, this partial contiguity in semantic space undoubtedly points to the typological validity of the semantic map proposed for the ditransitive construction in a previous contribution (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 591–97).
Barðdal et al. (2011) is the only study on historical Germanic languages that has weighed the strength of the five different case frames (including dat-dats) within the scope of the Old Norse–Icelandic ditransitive construction. We pay special attention to their findings regarding dat-gens, which serve as a starting point for comparison and contrast. We also consider their comprehensive analysis of the partial overlap among the existing subconstructions in their proposed semantic map for the Old Norse–Icelandic ditransitive.
This work is primarily centered around dat-gens. We do not analyze why many of the verb types belonging to our database may also make use of alternating case frames and to what extent. It is indeed not uncommon to find instances of verbs displaying two slots—dat-gen and dat-acc, dat-gen and acc-gen, or acc-gen and dat-acc. In fact, a few verbs may seem to work with three patterns, and fewer still with all options. However, this reality, which is usually perceived as being one of the causes for the eventual collapse of the case frame system in ditransitives, is left out in favor of an account of dat-gens per se, how they work and what they express. Nevertheless, we sometimes formulate some hypotheses concerning the dat-gen/dat-acc alternation in Section 5.1, Section 5.2, Section 5.3, Section 5.4, Section 5.5, Section 5.6, Section 5.7, and Section 5.8. Finally, since the kind of Diachronic Construction Grammar that we apply here (DCxG; Barðdal and Gildea 2015, pp. 1–50) is inextricably grounded on the syntax-semantics interface, we do not partake of the skepticism that sometimes appears in the literature concerning the impossibility of providing sound classifications for the verb types appearing in each of these case frames.
This article is structured as follows: In Section 2, theoretical background, we begin with a description of the scope of the current ditransitive construction as summarized by Goldberg, which is later enhanced and adapted for the current Germanic languages, Old Norse–Icelandic and Proto-Germanic. Due to the affinity holding between dat-gens and dat-accs, we explain the semantic map proposal for this other subconstruction. In Section 3, overview and data, we survey the most important contributions to the study of dat-gens in OE studies, focusing on Visser’s and Mitchell’s lists. After that, and to open up a comparative perspective, we include a brief analysis of dat-gens in Old Norse–Icelandic. Section 4, methodology, describes the criteria followed for a critical assessment of Visser’s and Mitchell’s lists, also explaining why we chose DOEWC and the type of searches conducted. In Section 5, the scope of the OE dat-gen subconstruction, we proceed to a detailed account of the conceptual domains and verb classes involved, providing an overall explanation for each, followed by the corresponding type lists, number of tokens for each type, and examples. Section 6, analysis and formalization, contains a semantic map proposal for OE dat-gens, which is explained in full. Making use of box formalism, the section also describes the most prototypical constructions in this subconstruction, before proceeding to partially reconstruct dat-gens in North-West Germanic by using a small correspondence set. Finally, in Section 7, we draw conclusions and specify directions for further research.

2. Theoretical Background

In this work, we define a ditransitive construction as a predicate consisting of three components: a subject, indirect object, and direct object, typically corresponding to the roles of agent, recipient, and theme (Goldberg 1995, pp. 141–42). As observed in the previous section, during the OE period, the construction’s scope allowed for this threefold predicate to be structured into each of the five major subconstructional case frames: dat-acc, acc-dat, acc-gen, dat-gen, or acc-acc. Unlike OE dat-accs, dat-gens morphosyntactically feature the direct object in the genitive. As illustrated in examples (7) and (8), the case frame alternates between dat-gen and gen-dat order:
(7)&   se arcebiscop    him   þæs   tiðude
And the.archbishop.NOM him.DAT that.GEN concede
‘And the archbishop granted that to him’(Ch 1464 [Rob 80] B15.5.23; WS)
(8)Ðæs    him    getiþað   drihten crist:
That.GEN them.DAT    concede    lord.Christ.NOM
‘That (healing for ailing men) is granted to them by Christ our Lord.’
(ÆCHom I, 4 B1.1.5; lWS)
Visser (1963, pp. 606–7) affirms that the genitive object should be interpreted as some kind of adjunct (for an example, see quotation (9) in Section 3 below). We revisit these two issues in Section 6.
We utilize the framework of Diachronic Construction Grammar (Barðdal and Gildea 2015; Gildea and Barðdal 2023). This approach aims for the syntactic reconstruction of argument structure constructions in the world’s languages (Gildea and de Castro Alves 2020: pp. 47–107), the Indo-European family (Frotscher et al. 2022; Luján and Ruiz Abad 2014; Luján and López-Chala 2020, pp. 336–70), and, more particularly, in Germanic languages (Barðdal 2023; Bucci and Barðdal 2024; Barðdal et al. 2019; Dunn et al. 2017). In this contribution, we partially reconstruct dat-gens in North-West Germanic. Our approach primarily focuses on OE and contrasts results with a few data gathered from Old Norse–Icelandic. It is important to note that our analysis is limited to North-West Germanic, and that the reconstruction of dat-gens presented here is partial and preliminary. Further research will be necessary to fully reconstruct the scope of dat-gens in North-West Germanic and Proto-Germanic.
For decades, a widely held belief in structural and generative linguistics has been the impossibility of reconstructing syntax (Barðdal et al. 2020, pp. 9–13; Barðdal and Eythórsson 2020). This conviction stems from the view that syntax lacks inherent semantics, functioning solely as a repository of pattern-based information. Consequently, traditional approaches to comparative reconstruction, which rely on units comprising both form and meaning, have avoided the incorporation of syntax into their analysis. However, in Construction Grammar (CxG), constructions are conceived as form-meaning pairings, facilitating the operationalization of syntactic patterns in reconstructive terms (cf. Michaelis 2012, pp. 134–35, for an expanded definition of construction). In this article, we present a few correspondence sets containing Old English/Old Norse–Icelandic pairs to illustrate the reconstructive method employed. Additionally, following the customary practice in CxG formalism, we will employ boxes to describe the argument structure of the most relevant verb-specific types used in the Old English dat-gen subconstruction.
The current English ditransitive construction is associated with a well-defined and closed list of specific verb classes. In her pioneering 1995 contribution to CxG, Goldberg (1995, p. 38) specifies nine different verb classes.
As observed in Figure 1 below, the nine verb classes are divided into core and periphery and are accounted for polysemically. The central sense encompasses verbs of giving, spontaneous, and continuous causation. Depending on their position along the cline moving from A to E, the remaining senses are perceived as more or less prototypical.
Using a corpus of 18th-century Late Modern English, Colleman and De Clerck (2011) demonstrate that the scope of the ditransitive construction was broader during that period. While the verb classes summarized by Goldberg above were already fully operative, the authors also identify additional phenomena. These include terms for banishment (banish, dismiss, expel), manner of speaking verbs like whisper, as well as benefactive usages such as holding sb the torch, and malefactive units related to verbs of dispossession (spoil).
In the field of Germanic linguistics, and from a constructional perspective, Barðdal (2007, pp. 9–30) demonstrates that the list of verb classes for the ditransitive construction in Icelandic and Proto-Germanic is notably larger, comprising 17 verb classes. We proceed to list and exemplify those absent from English (Barðdal 2007, pp. 12–13) since many of them are included in the semantic map for OE dat-gens to appear in Section 6 below. For clarity, we retain the original numbering of each verb class, as shown in Table 1.
In 2011, a systematic study (Barðdal et al. 2011, pp. 53–104) of all West Scandinavian languages, both present and past, confirmed the existence of the 17 verb classes. Following Croft’s requirement for a lexicality–schematicity hierarchy (Croft 2003), which entails a continuum ranging from verb-specific vocabulary to highly schematic argument structure constructions, the authors categorized the 17 verb classes into nine major conceptual domains: Delivering (1), Enabling (2), Deictically Directed Transfer (3), Intention (4), Creation and Miscreation (5), Possession and Dispossession (6), Retaining (7), Mode of Communication (8), and Mental Processes (9). The taxonomy was validated in a later publication for the Old English dat-acc subconstruction and Proto-Germanic (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 574–91). Also, the data collected for Old English dat-gens in Section 5.1, Section 5.2, Section 5.3, Section 5.4, Section 5.5, Section 5.6, Section 5.7, and Section 5.8 below confirm a practically identical picture.
It is important to remark the consistency that a lexicality–schematicity approach provides for the analysis of argument structure constructions in historical linguistics because it frames the study of these into a chained sequence arranged from bottom-to-top which makes formal reconstruction possible at all different levels, namely verb-subspecific (prefixed verbs), verb-specific, verb subclass-specific, verb-class-specific, higher-level conceptual domain, and event-type, the most schematic. In this work, we cover the first five levels for OE fully, but the reconstruction obtained for North-West Germanic of the event-type constructional level is as partial as the fragmentary data used from Old Norse–Icelandic (see Section 6 below).
Finally, we also frame the results of this work typologically. In this sense, the findings obtained by Malchukov et al. (2007, 2010) for the current English ditransitive construction encapsulate the former’s scope into a semantic map consisting of core (Theme-Recipient construction), extensions (Patient-Beneficiary construction), and edges (Theme-Goal construction, Internal Possessor construction, External Possession construction, Malefactive Source construction, and Patient Instrumental construction; see Figure 2 below). In a previous publication, we demonstrated how the extremely productive diversity of dat-accs in OE could be represented in such a map, which originally consisted of a small number of verb types, since ditransitivity is not very prolific among non-Indo-European languages. Introduced in Figure 2 below, we distributed the cited 17 verb classes into the following map, which serves for OE (as exemplified below), Gothic, Old Norse–Icelandic, and Proto-Germanic at the same time (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 595–96).
There are also some minor differences between the cited 2011 and 2019 proposals. The 2019 semantic map repositions Enabling, placing it at the core of Actual Transfer, together with the different verb classes for giving. Verbs of lending are renumbered from 3 to 9 since they are reclassified as part of Enabling. Finally, class 14, verbs of utilizing, is not present because OE cognates like neotan (‘to enjoy, have the benefit of’) do not use the dat-acc slot. In this work, we take the 2019 semantic map proposal for Old English dat-accs as the starting point of comparison and contrast for the study of Old English dat-gens, assuming that dat-accs (and/or acc-dats) are the most productive subconstructional option (Mitchell 1985, p. 452). We draw the corresponding dat-gen map version, showing for the first time how dat-gens systematically match most of the major areas found for dat-accs and how they specialize in these areas, too.

3. Overview and Data

In this section, we mainly focus on the overviews of Visser (1963, p. 607) and Mitchell (1985, pp. 452, 455–64) since the two analyses shape the contemporary perspective on double object complementation in OE. Also, the list of verb types and tokens produced by Visser and Mitchell’s list of verbal rections make up the starting point of our dat-gen database. Apart from the cited authors, we briefly include Zanchi and Tarsi’s (2021, pp. 31–87) study of valency patterns in Gothic and Barðdal, Kristoffersen and Sveen’s analysis (2011: 53–104) of the ditransitive case frame system in Old Norse–Icelandic.
In the section devoted to syntactical units with one verb, Visser performs an exhaustive analysis of double object complementation (II. Two objects) starting with the study of dat-gens. The construction, according to the author, is frequent in OE and consists of an ‘Indirect Object + Causative Object’. The author seems to restrict the semantics of the indirect object to recipients—we have found addressees, benefactives, malefactives, and experiencers, and combinations of these, too—while at the same time providing an adverbial account of the direct object, which is why he terms it causative. In his words, the direct object expresses ‘a thing or a circumstance which occasions the action or with which the action has concernment’ (Visser 1963, p. 608). Accordingly, he believes that the genitive object in (9) below should not be interpreted literally as a direct object (‘you wouldn’t give me clothes’) but as some kind of adjunct (‘you did not give me concerning clothes’). We partially concur with his analysis, particularly in the case of abstract references, but not as much with literal ones, where it is difficult to escape the recipient-based semantics of this subconstruction.
(9)Ic wæs nacod.    nolde   ge    me   wæda   tiðian
I was    naked    wouldn’t you.NOM me.DAT clothes.GEN concede
‘I was naked, and you did not consent to clothe me’ (ÆCHom II, 7 B1.2.8; lWS)
Visser also produces a list of the most frequent verb types: forwyrnan, (ge)styran, (ge)tiþian, (ge)þancian, (ge)unnan, ofteon, ofunnan, tilian and wenan. The author proceeds to select biddan ‘to ask’ as an instance of dat-gen/acc-gen case frame alternation. He somehow seems to relate the cited alternation in particular and the diversity of alternating patterns in general to the gradual dissolution of the dat-gen case frame, which he dates back to the transition from the end of OE to the beginning of Middle English.
Section §677 is an alphabetical list containing verbs operating in this pattern (+3+2) and the tokens that justify their inclusion. I enclose a slightly reduced list comprising 48 verb types after excluding a small group of 11 units. We will revisit this in Section 4, methodology. The definitions are mainly sourced from Bosworth and Toller (1973) or A Thesaurus of Old English (Roberts et al. 1995): ærendian ‘to report, intercede on an errand’, asecean ‘to demand’, behatan ‘to promise’, beniman ‘to take away’, biddan ‘to ask, make a request’, ceapian ‘to buy’, ceosan ‘to choose’, forwyrnan ‘to refuse, deny’, geandwyrdan ‘to answer’, gedripan ‘to drip’, gehatan ‘to promise’, gelyfan ‘to trust, hope’, gescrifan ‘to shrive, impose penance’, gestyran ‘to restrain, withhold’, geswutelian ‘to explain, show’, getiþian ‘to grant, allow’, geþafian ‘to permit, consent’, geþancian ‘to thank, give thanks’, geunnan ‘to grant, allow, concede’, gewyrcan ‘to get by working, gain, obtain’, gewyscan ‘to wish for sth for sb’, gyrnan ‘to desire’, healdan ‘to keep, preserve’, hycgan ‘to think, consider’, oflætan ‘to let off’, ofsceamian ‘to refute, put to shame’, ofteon ‘to take away, deprive’, ofunnan ‘to refuse to grant’, oncweðan ‘to reply, respond’, ondrædan ‘to dread, fear’, onlænan ‘to lend, grant’, onleon ‘to grant the loan of sth’, onwendan ‘to subvert, disturb’, sprecan ‘to say, tell’, styran ‘to prevent sb from sth’, tilian ‘to get after seeking’, tiþian ‘to grant, concede’, þancian ‘to thank, give thanks’, þencan ‘to think’, unnan ‘to grant sb sth’, wealdan ‘to rule, have dominion over’, wenan ‘to think, suppose’, wyrnan ‘to refuse, refrain from granting’, wilnian ‘to desire’, wiþbregdan ‘to withhold, restrain’, wiþcweðan ‘to refuse, reject’, wiþstandan ‘to hinder, prevent’, and wyscan ‘to wish for, desire’. Over 70 years after the publication of Visser’s first volume, the list remains the most substantial contribution to the study of dat-gens to this day, comprising 48 verb types and 183 tokens. As we will see in Section 5.1, Section 5.2, Section 5.3, Section 5.4, Section 5.5, Section 5.6, Section 5.7, and Section 5.8, the current availability of computerized corpora can help advance the study of this subconstruction.
Mitchell’s overview (1985, pp. 449–64) of double rection complements that of Visser’s. The author lists four main frame options (acc-dat, dat-acc, acc-gen, and dat-gen; dat-accs are taken for granted), noting the existence of a less productive acc-dat (abl), which is one of the major types in Visser, and briefly referring to acc-preps and dat-preps. He relates dat-gens to actions such as ‘thanking, giving, refusing, and taking from’, selecting unnan and þancian for illustration.
In a way, Mitchell’s statements concerning the inconsistency of the rules for simple and double object complementation (1985: pp. 449–450) in OE focus on alternations and how these may have been partially responsible for the dissolution of the ditransitive case frame system. Although he does not explicitly admit it, he appears to share Visser’s viewpoint in this respect as well. In our opinion, his acknowledgment of the fact that ‘the same verb can take different constructions not only in the works of different writers or in different places in the works of the same writers, but even in the same sentence’ (1985: p. 453) should be interpreted as an indirect recognition of the role that case frame alternations like dat-accs/dat-gens play within the wider scope of the OE ditransitive construction. As stated in Section 2, these overlaps, seemingly arbitrary for Mitchell, are partial but systematic. We will demonstrate this in Section 5.1, Section 5.2, Section 5.3, Section 5.4, Section 5.5, Section 5.6, Section 5.7, and Section 5.8 below.
Mitchell’s list of verbal rections includes 48 verb types. We specify the following 19, not included in Visser’s: geærendian ʿgo on an errand for sbʾ; ascian ʿto ask sb for sthʾ; abiddan, gebiddan ʿto ask, pray for sth for sbʾ; ætbregdan ʿto take sb/sth from sbʾ; dwellan, gedwellan ʿto lead sb astray from sthʾ; friþian, gefriþian ʿto protect sb/sth from sthʾ; tolætan ʿto releaseʾ; leanian, geleanian ʿto reward sb for sthʾ; benæman ʿto deprive sb of sthʾ; secgan ʿto say sth to sbʾ; getilian ʿto strive after, acquire sth for oneselfʾ; truwian, getruwian ʿto clear sb of sthʾ; þafian, ʿto permit sth to sbʾ; and gewenan ʿto hope for sth for sbʾ. We have only been able to retrieve from the corpus some of these terms. This is a topic we will revisit in Section 4, methodology.
Zanchi and Tarsi (2021, pp. 31–87) systematize the range of valency patterns existing in Gothic for three-place predicates from a typological perspective. Their contribution is based on the Valency Patterns Project or ValPaL, which retakes Levin’s belief (1993) that it is possible to provide a semantic classification for verbs by operationalizing their syntax. The project compiles a corpus of 80 basic verbs, studying the patterns associated with each item in 36 different languages. They ultimately develop a database of morphosyntactical patterns and a list of the major alternations for each of the languages covered. The two authors cited utilize an enlarged corpus of 87 verb types and 3447 tokens for their overall study of valency options in Gothic. In their analysis of three-verb predicates (agent, recipient-like, and theme arguments), they draw on previous literature (Ferraresi 2005; Rousseau 2016; Miller 2019) to propose the following case frame system for Gothic, where only major patterns are included: acc-dat, dat-acc, acc-gen, and acc-acc. The authors specify that acc-dats and dat-accs seem to be the highest-frequency pattern, and they also seem to list the case frames in terms of productivity, with acc-gens ranging second and acc-accs last. Finally, they proceed to exemplify the most relevant verbs exhibiting the options listed above (2021: pp. 49–51). Unfortunately for our purposes, since their excellent work systematizes data starting from a selection of 87 verb types and only the major valency patterns, they do not include the study of dat-gens, which seem to be less productive but may also be part of the picture (Miller 2019, p. 162; apud Rousseau 2016). The lack of data in Gothic is one of the reasons why we leave the reconstruction of the dat-gen subconstruction in Proto-Germanic for future works.
Barðdal, Kristoffersen, and Sveen’s contribution (2011: 53–104) is, to our knowledge, the only full-length analysis of the ditransitive case frame system for a historical Germanic language produced so far. The authors divide ditransitivity in Old Norse–Icelandic into five different case frames (dat-acc, acc-dat, acc-gen, dat-gen, and dat-dat), thus adding dat-dats to the case frame system, a slot still unacknowledged in OE. These subconstructions are quantitatively weighed in terms of the number of verb types, displaying an imbalance between the most productive variant (dat-accs: 140 predicates) and the rest: 43, 22, 15, and 15, respectively. They then proceed to exemplify and analyze each of the subconstructions in terms of the verb classes and conceptual domains involved in their usage, instantiating these. For the dat-gen subconstruction, which ranks low in the cited graded sequence (15 verb types), the authors cite the following quotation (2011: 73):
(10)Þrándur    synjaði honum   ráðsins.
Þrándur    denied    him.DAT option-the.GEN
‘Þrándur denied him that option.’ (Flóamanna saga 1987: 742)
Synja relates to verbs of future transfer and to one of the major conceptual domains associated with the ditransitive construction, Retaining. The authors find evidence for the following areas of semantic space, displayed in Table 2 below.
Old Norse–Icelandic dat-gens are thus used in five major cognitive domains (small caps in Table 2; verbs of obtaining were later reclassified as part of Possession) and six related verb classes. The verb classes involved range from verbs inherently signifying giving or delivering to those for mental activity. For the sake of conveniency, and since we are going to compare reconstructively in Section 6 below, it is perhaps more important to be aware of the semantic gaps applicable to Old Norse–Icelandic dat-gens. In this sense, the article does not include Enabling, Deictically Directed Transfer (verbs of sending; verbs of bringing), Creation (and Miscreation) or Mode of Communication (verbs of communicated message; verbs of instrument of communicated message). Concerning this last cognitive domain, we do find promises (væna e-m e-s) and refusals (synja e-m e-s), but the terms are distributed into different verb classes and domains.
The authors finally compare the semantic scope of the five Old Norse–Icelandic subconstructions, inserting these into a semantic map (2011: 75), see Figure 3 immediately below:
Drawing upon Croft’s semantic connectivity hypothesis (2003), they provide typological validation to their semantic map proposal encompassing all West Scandinavian languages, past and present, by proving that the five different Old Norse–Icelandic subconstructions accommodate to the same areas currently occupied by languages like Norwegian and Icelandic. The contiguous nature of these five case frame slots, which occupy ‘adjacent regions’ in the semantic map, is particularly noteworthy. Upon examination of the map, it becomes evident that besides the areas that may be exclusive and defining for each subconstruction, there are also significant partial overlaps among them. Notably, some of these overlaps—such as in the case of Actual Transfer—span more than two conceptual domains. While these overlaps might easily escape notice, they are crucial for the comparative analysis of Old English dat-gens and dat-accs. We will now describe the methodological criteria employed in this study.

4. Methodology

This section begins by revisiting Section 2, overview and data, to elucidate the criteria we employed for excluding a small group of verb types and tokens from Visser’s list. Following that, we discuss some of the issues raised by Mitchell’s list of verbal rections for the future OE dat-gen database. Subsequently, we detail the reasons for selecting DOEWC (Healey et al. 2015) as a corpus and proceed to explain the rationale behind the query searches conducted. Finally, the nature of the corpus thus gathered is framed both diatopically and diachronically.
Visser’s list, outstanding as it is, raises some issues regarding the compilation of types and tokens for our database. Since our analysis is exclusively focused on Nom-Dat-Gens, that is, on three-place predicates exhibiting a prototypical agent for the nominative case, we have left out a few tokens in the passive voice and a relatively significant group of verb types operating as what the literature unjustly terms impersonal constructions. Visser must have had his reasons for not categorizing them apart from regular Nom-Dat-Gens (cf. Somers et al. 2024, pp. 1–35, for a related alternation), but since they do not canonically comply with the pattern under study here, we have postponed their study. The list of excluded units exhibiting non-nominatives in the tokens is as follows: beon þearf (wana) ‘to be in need’, earmian ‘to commiserate, feel pity’, hlystan ‘to listen’, hreowan ‘to rue, grieve’, ofhreowan ‘to cause grief or pity’, ofþyncan ‘to cause regret or sorrow’, sceamian ‘to feel shame’, and tweoni(ge)an ‘to doubt’.
We have also ruled out afyrsian ‘to drive away, dispel’ and fyllan ‘to fill, replenish’ because the two quotations adduced are not clear instances of dat-gens. In the case of afyrsian, it is doubtful if the strong feminine nouns ðrowung and forhtung are either genitive or dative: þæt hit (þis fyr) þam geleaffullum afyrsige þære ðrowunge forhtunge ‘so that it (the fire) may drive the believers away from suffering (and) torment’ (ælfric’s Lives of Saints). If the feminine nouns are in the dative, they can be interpreted adverbially. Regarding fyllan, the DOEWC includes an extra word in the sentence, wis, adding ambiguity to the analysis. The quotation is Psalm 145, v. 16: <Onhlidest> ðu þine handa and hi hraðe fyllest, ealra wihta gehwam (wis) bletsunga ‘You open your hand and satisfy the desire of every living thing’. In the quote, the verb is omitted; wis bletsunga can be interpreted as an epithet for God, signifying ‘wise in/with blessings’; ealra withta gehwam may function adverbially, and the Latin input Aperis tu manum tuam, et imples omne animal benedictione shows a different syntactic pattern—accusative plus an instrumental. In our opinion, there is too much uncertainty to consider this type/token valid. In addition, a simple search conducted in DOEWC for fyll (217 matches) did not yield any dat-gen tokens.
We have also skipped one or two cases when the quotation is actually dat-prep, containing a preposition introducing the indirect object: No þæs fela Daniel to his drihtne gespræc soðra worda þurh snytro cræft ‘Daniel could not say so many true words to his Lord using all his wits’ (Dan A1.3). More noticeably, Visser does not provide for semantic definitions of the verb types listed, nor does he discriminate between the different senses of a given term, giving us a straightforward list of tokens for just one unit each time. This involves forwyrnan ‘to refuse, deny’ and ‘to hinder, prevent’; ofteon ‘to take away, deprive’ and ‘to withhold, withdraw’; ofunnan ‘to refuse to grant’ and ‘to wish to deprive sb of sth’; and steoran ‘to prevent sb from’, ‘to reprove, rebuke’, and ‘to correct’. Since each sense relates to a specific verb type, we have proceeded to number the former conveniently. We have also reclassified Visser’s tokens into their corresponding sense for the construction of our database.
We now return to Mitchell’s alphabetical list of verbal rections (1985: 454–464). In Section 3 above we included a group of 19 verb types proposed by Mitchell which were absent from Visser’s. However, we have not been able to validate nine of these, in DOEWC or elsewhere: geærendian ʿgo on an errand for sbʾ; gebiddan ʿto ask, pray for sth for sbʾ; friþian, gefriþian ʿto protect sb/sth from sthʾ; leanian ʿto reward sb for sthʾ; getilian ʿto strive after, acquire sth for oneselfʾ; truwian ʿto clear sb of sthʾ; þafian ʿto permit sth to sbʾ; and gewenan ʿto hope for sth for sbʾ. The searches we have conducted in DOEWC involve the most common base form for each of these terms, and sometimes even more than one base form if the term is productive—gebid- and gebæd for present and past tense options from gebiddan, for instance. We have also checked DOE dictionary entries when available. In this respect, geærendian is highly illustrative of less productive units. The DOE entry includes 17 out of a total of 18 quotations existing for the term. None of the instances included in the entry specify dat-gen usage. We have additionally checked in DOEWC for geær(e)nd-, geerend, and giærend-, but have not retrieved the last remaining token. This leads us to think that this term and many of the units listed above do not use the related pattern. In fact, we are under the impression that except for þafian and getilian, whose related derivative and non-derivative variants are common exponents of dat-gens, this confusion arises from Mitchell’s editing decision of grouping prefixed and unprefixed terms together in the list. If this is so, the corpus we have gathered covers all terms except for friþian, whose pattern seems to be acc-gen.
It is now time to describe why DOEWC has been chosen and the queries conducted, starting with the latter. The main search string involves two members: a personal pronoun word form like him situated in the neighborhood of a genitive singular -es. We have focused on him because pronominality is one of the triggers activating the use of ditransitives nowadays. So, according to probabilistic syntax (Bresnan 2007, pp. 27–96), it would not seem very risky to argue that the situation in OE times may have been similar. For now, we concentrate on just this word form for the third-person singular pronoun option, leaving aside a full analysis of personal pronouns for future works, both regarding the dat-gen pattern and the rest.
Focusing on a personal pronoun word form has its consequences, particularly concerning corpus selection. We have opted to use the DOEWC since it is the only existing tool that allows the retrieval of the totality of him occurrences in the over 3,000,000 OE words distributed among the totality of 3060 extant texts, prose and poetry. Other equally valid and powerful resources like the syntactically annotated York-Toronto-Helsinki Parsed Corpus of Old English Prose (Taylor et al. 2003) contain a selection of prose texts and cannot provide us with a full account for him in all OE works. Pronominality, together with the low productivity of dat-gens, which necessitates compensation with massive amounts of data, are the two major reasons why we chose to use the DOEWC. Additionally, by focusing on the him + -es pattern over other alternatives, we aim to describe the entirety of a selected sample, thus adhering to the maxims of corpus size (McEnery and Brookes 2022, pp. 36–41) and authenticity, too. These considerations somewhat compensate for the forced lack of corpus balance existing in Old English corpora.
Among the research tools available in DOEWC, we chose to utilize proximity searches. As mentioned earlier, our query comprised two elements, him followed by -es, allowed to collocate within a maximal separation of 120 characters. The query returned 11,050 matches, which were then examined individually. Initially, we did not intend to conduct single searches in DOEWC since the dat-gen subconstruction requires two word forms that may be separated in space. Unlike Boolean searches, proximity searches enable us to restrict the distance between query members. We selected a separation of 120 characters rather than 80 or 40 because, considering the relative freedom of word order in Old English, we aimed for our query to be as comprehensive as possible within the limited range derived from searching only for him.
As is evident from the explanation above, one of the elements in our query is a complete word, while the other is just a word ending. We chose the third-person singular masculine pronoun to represent the indirect object because, unlike other options such as me, þe, or us, which can also function as accusatives, him is exclusively dative. Despite some overlap with the third-person plural option, this word form is exceedingly common in Anglo-Saxon records. The second query member is the nominal ending -es. We chose this ending to avoid ambiguity deriving from the high level of syncretism in Anglo-Saxon noun declensions. OE -es can only refer to strong masculine and neuter nouns in the singular, making it the most productive nominal subcategory. Additionally, this ending also covers athematic nouns. For comparison, consider the ineffectiveness that would result from hypothetically selecting -e/-a (strong feminine nouns in singular and plural) or -an (weak nouns in singular and plural).
It is important to note here that the string used operationalizes our search for a more relevant third hidden component—verbs collocating with him and -es. In other words, at this initial stage, our focus is on designing a query that can efficiently retrieve not just an ad hoc specific verb type, but every one of them together with their corresponding tokens. The 11,050 quotations obtained in the DOEWC were like quotations (11) and (12), corresponding to matches 1 and 15, respectively, under the first 100 series:
(11)Næs him   fruma æfre, or geworden, ne nu ende   cymþ ecean drihtnes
Wasn`t him.DAT start ever or become, nor   end.NOM come eternal.lord.GEN
‘There was never a beginning for Him, and there will be no end to the
Eternal Lord’(GenA,B A1.1; lWS)
(12)Hwy sceal ic æfter his hyldo ðeowian, bugan him   swilces geongordomes?
Why shall I.NOM after his favor    obey    bow    him.DAT such.obedience.GEN
‘Why should I serve after his favor, bow to him in such young subjection?’
(GenA,B A1.1; lWS)
Example (11) serves as an illustration of the over 10,500 quotations that we have meticulously examined and dismissed, since the query members are not morphosyntactically related or do not render the dat-gen pattern. However, quotation (12) showcases one verb type, bugan ‘to subject, bow, yield’, which fits the pattern. Despite this, the term is unacknowledged as dat-gen, and the pattern is not included in the DOE (see entry for bugan).
While verifying the validity of the 11,050 matches, we also checked those instances from Visser’s list containing him + -es. Nevertheless, Visser’s list is obviously not restricted to this personal pronoun, containing others and noun phrases, too. After completing the analysis of the main proximity search, during a second phase of corpus compilation, we proceeded to validate the rest of Visser’s verb types and tokens not covered yet. We did so by performing single searches in DOEWC which retrieved the exact context for each token. In those few cases when Visser’s token did not clearly conform to dat-gen, we performed proximity searches using other personal pronouns and/or single searches for specific word forms to prove if the term involved operated the pattern. Sometimes, particularly if the term showed a high frequency, it was possible to find dat-gen usages. However, this proved more difficult for units displaying poor productivity, and frequently, combining single and proximity searches did not suffice. We also had a look at the corresponding DOE dictionary entries if available (Healey 2016). Regarding Mitchell’s list, as he does not provide examples in his list of verbal rections, we were able to incorporate four more types into our database, which we acknowledge here: geleanian ‘to reward, recompense’, getruwian ‘to clear sb from sth’, dwellan ‘to lead into error, deceive’, and gedwellan ‘to deceive, lead astray’. Additionally, we also proceeded to detect unacknowledged types we came across through collocations, validating a few of these, too.
During this second phase of corpus compilation, the scope of our search widens, not restricting itself to him + -es instances, but also accepting indirect objects rendered by noun phrases, other genitive endings (gen. pl. -a; gen. sg. (þæ)-s), and a few doubtful cases, usually involving strong feminine nouns whose verb is acknowledged to render the pattern—for instance, unnan ‘to grant’. Therefore, we end up accepting other variants to the main query search, sometimes. After all, the database we intend to create is built upon cumulatively. While this may be irrelevant in terms of the compilation of a future dat-gen database, it matters in terms of frequency. In this work, we confine ourselves to presenting raw frequency, indicating the total number of occurrences found for a given term in our corpus. We refrain from calculating normalized frequencies (Jones 2022, pp. 127–30) for each of the verb types identified using the dat-gen pattern, which might be highly unlikely in numerous cases of low-frequency terms—44 types show only one quotation so far. In the end, we obtained a total of 88 verb types and 443 tokens for the future dat-gen database, adding 19 new verb types and 260 new verb tokens to Visser’s list. This increase in the data, already a contribution to the study of OE dat-gens on its own terms, more significantly allows for the reconstruction of the scope of this ditransitive subconstruction.
Old English is a convenient label for texts from different regions (Northumbrian, Mercian, Kentish, and West Saxon) and periods, with early OE spanning from 700 AD to around 880–890 and late OE covering approximately 900–1150. It is well known that the four manuscripts containing most of the poetry have been preserved in texts written in late West Saxon, despite clear Anglian influences. Similarly, the majority of OE prose texts also show late West Saxon provenance. The lack of records and the influence of the court have thus probably prevented the DOEC from being a balanced corpus in terms of geographic and chronological representation. There is much less representation of the Anglian (Northumbrian, Mercian) and Kentish varieties, and a predominance of late OE (West Saxon) works which are largely prose (Mölig-Falke 2015, pp. 399–402).
The data gathered from the DOEC for this investigation align with the cited predominance of late West Saxon over the rest of the options. However, we have also found many quotations attributed to early West Saxon, which, when gathered together with the late ones, support our findings with a sense of continuity during the whole OE period. For referencing, we have followed the DOE’s classification of OE texts, adding a tag distinguishing between early West Saxon (eWS) and late (lWS). In a few cases when dating is unclear, only the variety has been specified (WS). When the quotations are taken from poetic works, the lWS tag is understood to also include Anglian features. We now turn to describing the verb classes and conceptual domains involved in Section 5.

5. The Scope of the OE dat-gen Subconstruction

In this section, we present the evidence gathered for the dat-gen subconstruction. Assuming the existence of the semantic connectivity hypothesis within a CxG framework, and considering the close relations between the dat-gen and dat-acc subconstructions, we proceed to describe the conceptual domains and verb classes identified for the dat-gen subconstruction in the corpus we have assembled. We start by describing each conceptual domain. After this, we catalog the verb classes and types associated with the given domain, providing raw frequencies for the latter and selecting representative quotations for elucidation.

5.1. Verbs Inherently Signifying Giving and Delivering

This conceptual domain, positioned at the core of the Theme-Recipient construction, exhibits remarkably low productivity (Perek 2020). In the dat-acc subconstruction, there are four verb classes—verbs of giving, entrusting, distributing, and giving back (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 574–76)—with verbs of paying (Class 4 in 2011) being a subclass of giving back. However, we have only been able to provide evidence of the dat-gen subconstruction for the first of these, verbs of giving. We list the verb types included first and then proceed to exemplify sellan and brucan2, which have not been acknowledged as dat-gen usages so far, in quotations (13) and (14).
list of verb types. Verb class 2, verbs of giving: sellan ‘to give’ (1), gedripan ‘to drip’ (1), brucan2 ‘to feed oneself’ (1).
(13)Ne syle þu   unscyldigra sawla deorum   þe þe andettað earme þearfan;
Not give you.NOM innocent.souls.GEN beasts.DAT who you    praise    miserable.people
‘Don’t give to wild beasts innocent souls of those the miserable poor who praise
you.’(PPs A5 [0345 (73.17)]; lWS, poet.)
(14)he    sceall […], forgange flæsc and win, and bruce him   oðra metta,
he.NOM will   forget food and win    and feed him.DAT other.edibles.GEN
‘Then he shall fast […], abstaining from food and win and enjoying other food
for himself.’(Conf 2.1 [Spindler A-Y] B11.2.1; lWS)
Quotation (13) exemplifies a single search (syl(l)e) conducted in DOEWC to confirm that sellan operationalizes the dat-gen subconstruction. This term serves as the prototype for ditransitive constructions, thus explaining its wide range of meanings as documented in Bosworth-Toller—‘to give sth to sb, pay tribute, offer, furnish, give one thing for another, sell, hand over, deliver, entrust, give up, betray, give an answer, give leave, give punishment, reward, endow, give one’s heart to sb’. Observing this term functioning as dat-gen partially supports the assumption that, as a prototype, sellan must represent all alternating case frames within the ditransitive construction. A proximity search combining one word form (sell, syl(l), seald, etc.) with him in DOEWC produces dat-acc usages in the two figures. On the other hand, brucan2 presents a distinct, more specialized case. This verb should be interpreted as an extension of brucan ‘to enjoy, make use of’, with a specialization in edibles as the referent.
Given the symbolic quantity of verb types (three) and tokens (three, each represented by a single quotation), and the absence of instances for verbs of entrusting, distributing, and giving back (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, p. 609), it is reasonable to conclude that the dat-gen subconstruction was infrequently utilized among the described verb classes.

5.2. Enabling

Occupying a relatively secondary position in modern English (verbs of future transfer under sense D in Goldberg’s scheme), this conceptual domain is repositioned in OE as an integral part of the Theme-Recipient construction alongside verbs of giving (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 576–78). There are four verb groups identified for dat-accs: verbs of granting or conferring, lending (loans are conceptualized as temporal gifts), allowing, and permitting or consenting (2019: 609). This domain exhibits high productivity for dat-gens, encompassing the four cited verb classes and comprising 11 types and 112 tokens out of a total of 443 in the database. Notably, hyran ‘to accede to, grant’ and lætan ‘to allow, let go’ have hitherto been unrecognized uses of dat-gen. We proceed to enumerate the verb types identified and their respective classes, providing examples for many of them due to their significance.
list of verb types. Verb class 9. Verbs of conferring: unnan ‘to grant’ (22), geunnan ‘to grant, concede’ (31), ti(g)þian ‘to grant, concede’ (17), geti(g)þian ‘to grant, concede’ (25), and hyran ‘to accede to, grant’ (1). Verbs of lending (9): onlænan ‘to lend, grant’ (4), onleon ‘to grant the loan of sth’ (4). Verbs of allowing reluctantly (9): lætan ‘to allow, let go’ (1), oflætan ‘to let off’ (1), tolætan ‘to release, let go’ (1). Verbs of permitting or consenting (9): geþafian ‘to permit, consent, approve’ (5).
(15)[…] wen is, þæt […] he    him    ne   unne naðer    ne æhta ne    lifes.
norm is that   he.NOM him.DAT not    grant neither    goods.GEN nor life.GEN
‘(If the lord then through his servants constantly reminds him of the tax,
and he (the tenant) is inflexible, opting for withholding the payment
wrongfully), the norm is that he (the Lord) will not grant him (the tenant)
either land or life’.(LawIVEg B14.18; lWS)
(16)he    us    georne    friðes bæd    ac we    him   nanes    ne    tiþodon
He us    eagerly    peace asked but we.NOM him.DAT none.GEN not conceded
‘(we sinned against our brother; we saw his anguish), he asked us for peace but
we granted him none, (hence our troubles now)’(Gen [Ker]   B8.1.2; lWS)
(17)Be ðon    þe    mon    sweordes   onlæne    oðres ðeowe.
About    the.free.man.NOM sword.GEN   lends   other’s.servant.DAT
‘Regarding the freeman who may lend a sword to another freeman’s servant.’
(LawIneRb B14.4.2; eWS)
(18)& se Godes wer    nænigra þinga    him    hyran nolde to þon þe he hine bæd,
  god’s.man.NOM no.things.GEN him.DAT accede not about what he him bid
‘Then this man of God would not consent to any of the things he had asked him’
(GDPref and 3 [C] B9.5.5; eWS)
(19)[…] þene bid ic Eadmær þæt he    him   læte    þara twegra landa, […]
Then    ask I    Eadmær that he.NOM him.DAT let go the.two.lands.GEN
‘(And if God wills that Eadwold grows up in his father’s time so that he may hold land), then I ask Eadmr to let go of two lands to him, (either at Coleshil or at Eadburgusbury.’      (Ch 1535 [Whitelock 3] B15.6.52; lWS)
Unnan, tiþian, and their derivatives comprise 95 tokens, constituting an overwhelming majority of the usages within this conceptual domain. Except for some sporadic acc-gen usages for tiþian and a late Old English dat-acc instance for geunnan mentioned by Visser (1963, p. 607), verbs of conferring mostly operate within the subconstruction under study. However, verbs of lending, allowing, and permitting or consenting are also active in the dat-acc subconstruction, showing a higher number of types for lending and permitting—including lænan ‘to lend, grant’, gelænan ‘to lend, lease’, leon ‘to lend, grant’, lyfan ‘to give leave, allow’, alyfan ‘to give leave, grant’, þafian ‘to consent to, permit’, and geþafian ‘to favor, support’ (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, p. 609). In this regard, it is conceivable that high-frequency dat-acc verb types like giefan ‘to give, assign’ and/or forgiefan ‘to allow, permit’ could compete with unnan and tiþian. Across both subconstructions, most of the types are predominantly speech verbs or have the potential to function as such.

5.3. Intention

The ditransitive construction in modern usage is primarily focused on the idea of actual transfer, wherein the theme undergoes a movement from an agent to a recipient. In the context of Intention, this transfer typically occurs in the future, although it may not occur at all, as the emphasis lies on the agent’s desire for it to be accomplished. The verbs involved belong to class 7, which includes the so-called verbs of future transfer and those that meet Goldberg’s pragmatic Conditions of Satisfaction (CS; Goldberg 1995, p. 38). We will proceed to list the types, providing examples of some tokens for clarification.
list of verb types. Verb class 7: geleanian ‘to reward, recompense’ (1), beodan ‘to offer’ (1), foresceawian ‘to provide for in advance’ (1), behatan ‘to promise sth to sb’ (3) (CS), and gehatan ‘to promise’ (2) (CS).
(20)[…] þæt Crist […] þonne geleanað manna gehwylcum   ærran gewyrhta.
That Christ.NOM then   reward   men.GEN.each.DAT    earlier.deeds.GEN
‘(Then everyone who did not want to believe in the truth before will understand)
that Christ (through his power) will then reward every one of them according
to their deeds.’ (WHom 3   B2.1.3; lWS)
(21)Se þe […] næfð nænne truwan […] þæt he him foresceawie andlyfene & gewæda.
He who hasn’t no faith that he.NOM him.DAT foresee food.GEN? clothes.GEN
‘(Whoever turns to monastic life with double purpose) […] and has no faith in
that the Lord will foresee sustenance and clothing for him’
(ÆCHom I, 27    B1.1.29; lWS)
(22)(an his cnapena) eode þa to anum drymen […], and behet him   sceattes,
One his.servants went then to a.sorcerer and promised him.DAT money.GEN
‘One of his young servants went then to a sorcerer, promising him money (if)…’
(ÆLS (Basil) B1.3.4; lWS)
Quotation (20), an excerpt from one of Wulfstan’s homilies, verifies one of Mitchell’s proposed terms, geleanian. We retrieve a new dat-gen type, foresceawian, in (21), from a line coming from Ælfric’s Catholic Homilies. Finally, promises are fulfilled by means of either behatan or gehatan. The number of types and tokens for Intention is indeed very low, four and seven, respectively. The types listed above are all operative in the dat-acc slot, which shows 12 types (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 580–81). The quotation found for beodan in the dat-gen pattern, also attested in the corresponding DOE entry, seems to be more an exception than the norm, since the unit’s unprefixed and prefixed options are commonly rendered by dat-accs. The DOE entries for the rest of the terms seem to give higher productivity to dat-accs. In conclusion, it can be deduced that Intention is sometimes activated for dat-gen usage, but its productivity remains low, just as happens with domain 1 above.

5.4. (Mis)Creation

This conceptual domain comprises two verb-specific classes. The first, 12, encompasses verbs associated with creation and preparation. The second, 8, involves transfer along a path. The distinction between creation and preparation lies in the fact that in the latter case, the theme (accusative object) and, less prototypically, the indirect object (dative) in reflexive ditransitives require preparation or implementation in some manner.
These two verb-specific classes are associated with the Patient-Beneficiary construction. However, they are also operative within the Malefactive Source and External Possession constructions—blocking the path (18) and miscreating (12). The list of types includes strynan2 and ryman, which are currently unattested dat-gens, along with getruwian, dwellan, and gedwellan, terms derived from Mitchell’s list, now validated through newly obtained tokens.
list of verb types. Verb class 12, creation: strynan2 ‘to beget, generate, create’ (1). Preparation: getruwian ‘to clear sb from sth’ (1). Miscreation: ofunnan2 ‘to wish to deprive sb of life’ (3), dwellan ‘to lead into error, deceive’ (1), gedwellan ‘to deceive, lead astray’ (1). Verb class 18, transfer along a path: ryman ‘to clear a way, make room for sb’ (1).
(23)Adam […] ongan    him   to <eðelstæfe> oðres   strienan bearnes   be bryde,
Adam   began    him.DAT as family   another.GEN beget child.GEN by bride
‘Adam (was), when he then began to beget children through the bride into
the household,..’(GenA,B A1.1; lWS, poet.)
(24)þa   seo sæ    […] him    rymde   þreora mila    dries færeldes […]
Then the.sea.NOM them.DAT opened three.miles.GEN dry.journey.GEN
‘Then the sea (through God’s command) by flowing out cleared for them
(the Christians) three miles of dry path…’(ÆCHom I, 37 B1.1.39; lWS)
(25)Oft him brogan to laðne    gelædeð, se þe    himlifes   ofonn,
Often them terror   to fiery    directs    he.who.NOM them.DAT life.GEN destroy
‘Often someone (the devil) directs fiery terror into them (hermits), he who
would kill life in them’ (GuthA,B A3.2; lWS, poet.)
In line with our findings for domain 1, the situation in domain 3 exhibits similar characteristics. We have identified a total of six types along with their corresponding eight tokens, indicating the presence of the dat-gen subconstruction within this domain. Unlike with domain 2, we do not observe the same level of productivity. Nevertheless, in contrast to domain 1, we have sufficient evidence to support the existence of both verb classes (12 and 18) and all verb-specific subclasses, with the exception of blocking the path.
The higher productivity of dat-accs compared to dat-gens is evidenced by a substantial list of confirmed types for the former (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 610–11). Among the units functioning as dat-accs, examples include strienan ‘to gain, beget’, cennan ‘to beget, conceive’, and acennan ‘to bring forth, conceive’ for verb class 12 and ryman ‘to clear, make room’ for class 18. However, our data confirm that the remaining terms listed at the beginning of this section are exclusively associated with the dat-gen subconstruction. Quotation (24) illustrates how some of these units combine creation with modification, as exemplified by ryman ‘to make clear by removing obstructions, to clear a way’, according to Bosworth-Toller. Regarding (25), ofunnan2 is defined in lexicographical works as part of dispossession rather than miscreation. In our corpus, we have identified four instances of dat-gen usage, three of which clearly specify ‘lifes’ as a genitive object. This serves as evidence for the unit’s classification as Miscreation in our database.

5.5. (Dis)Possession

In this domain, two major verb classes are prominent: verbs of owning (1) and obtaining (13). Additionally, within class 1, there exist two verb-specific subclasses, delineating the contrast between stative and dynamic verbs, namely owning and getting (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, p. 573). Furthermore, we have identified usages for the two verb-specific subclasses associated with Dispossession, encompassing verbs of removal and spoliation. Notably, instances where types exhibit more than one token do not significantly differentiate between the two usages, removal and spoliation. However, since the two subclasses clearly exist independently within the OE dat-acc subconstruction, we have specified below when both subclasses are applicable.
Domain 5 comprises 14 verb types and 42 tokens, rendering it larger than Giving, Intention, and (Mis)Creation, yet falling short of Enabling. Notably, the types brucan, neotan, strynan, and wyrcan are unattested dat-gen usages. It is essential to recognize that this domain transcends mere expressions of ‘taking away’, as it encompasses both stative and dynamic variants of owning verbs, which are well represented.
list of verb types. Class 1, Possession, verbs of owning, stative: wealdan ‘to rule, have dominion over’ (4), healdan ‘to keep, preserve’ (1), brucan ‘to have and enjoy, make use of’ (2), neotan ‘to have the benefit of, enjoy’ (2). Class 13, verbs of owning, dynamic: tilian ‘to get after seeking’ (13), strynan ‘to get, obtain’ (1), wyrcan ‘to work, attain an object’ (2), gewyrcan ‘to get by working, gain, obtain’ (1), ceapian ‘to buy’ (3), ceosan ‘to choose’ (1). Verb class 1, Dispossession: ofteon ‘to take away, deprive’ (7) (removal, spoliation), benæman ‘to take away’ (3) (removal, spoliation), beniman ‘to take away’ (1) (removal), and ætbre(g)dan ‘to take away’ (1) (removal).
(26)‘ac    niotað    inc    þæs oðres ealles,   forlætað    þone ænne beam, […] ’
and enjoy   you.two.DAT the.other.all.GEN   leave out the.one.tree.ACC
‘you can enjoy yourselves with everything else, but you should leave out this
particular tree,’ (GenA,B A1.1; lWS, poet.)
(27)Brihtric […] þohte þæt he    him   micles wordes    wyrcan sceolde, […]
Brihtric   thought that he.NOM him.DAT big.words.GEN    make   would
‘(Then this) Brihtric (took 80 ships), (he) thought he would gain acclaim for
himself,’(ChronD (Cubbin) B17.8; lWS)
(28)sum wælhreow heretoga […] wolde him benæman his lifes and his rices.
a.cruel.chief.NOM   would him.DAT rob his.life.GEN and his.kingdom.GEN
‘Then a barbarous general (…) (he) wanted to deprive him of his life and
kingdom’(ÆCHom II, 19 B1.2.22; lWS)
Quotation (26) illustrates a small subset of units, neotan and brucan, which add the notion of enjoyment to that of possession inherent in recipient-based semantics. This belongs to class 14, a verb-specific option found in contemporary West Scandinavian languages as well as Old Norse–Icelandic (Barðdal et al. 2011). However, in Old Norse–Icelandic, this class is exclusively associated with the dat-acc subconstruction, and in languages like Icelandic, Norwegian, or Faroese, it aligns with the current ditransitive (Barðdal 2007, p. 11; Barðdal et al. 2011, p. 71). In Old English, neotan and brucan typically function with two arguments, operating as monotransitive verbs requiring a genitive object (Ogura 2010), which is the prevalent pattern. Although evidence for their usage with three arguments is limited, it does exist, primarily involving reflexivity. This syntactic–semantic mismatch between Old Norse–Icelandic and Old English warrants further investigation.
The list of dat-acc types for (Dis)Possession is extensive. Among the 25 types, there are some overlaps with dat-gens. Ceosan and geceosan appear to be exclusively used in the highest-frequency option, unless a partitive genitive is involved, as happens with the only token we could retrieve for ceosan here. Ætbre(g)dan and beniman function in both subconstructions. However, despite the limitations of our corpus, stative verbs of class 1 seem to be relatively well-attested in the dat-gen subconstruction. Quotation (28) exemplifies how units like benæman and ofteon are used to express removal (of one’s life) and spoliation (of somebody’s kingdom) interchangeably. Finally, as with Possession in modern Icelandic, OE, and Old Norse–Icelandic dat-accs, all stative and most dynamic variants of owning verbs are straightforward instances of reflexive ditransitives.

5.6. Retaining

Retaining comprises classes 15 and 16, consisting of verbs of hindrance and constraining, respectively. For these verb-specific groups, actual transfer may be rendered difficult or impossible. Despite the difficulty, transfer may eventually occur, typically within a malefactive context. Many constraining verbs express social norms. Goldberg’s verbs of refusal are a subclass within class 15 (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, p. 573).
list of verb types. Verb class 15: forwyrnan2 ‘to hinder, prevent’ (28), wiþstandan ‘to hinder, obstruct, prevent’ (4), wiþbregdan ‘to withhold, restrain, hold back’ (1), ofteon2 ‘to withhold, withdraw’ (4), styran ‘to prevent sb from’ (1), gestyran ‘to restrain, withhold’ (9). Verb class 15, subclass verbs of refusal: wyrnan ‘to refuse, refrain from granting’ (16), forwyrnan ‘to refuse, deny’ (28), ofunnan ‘to refuse to grant’ (1), wiþcweþan ‘to refuse, reject’ (2), and ofsceamian ‘to refute’ (1). Verb class 16: bugan ‘to subject, bow, yield’ (1), gescrifan ‘to shrive, impose penance’ (1), and onwendan, ‘to subvert, disturb’ (1).
(29)[…] eac se broc, […], ðonne þær micel stan […] him   his rihtrynes wiðstent.
too the brook   when    there big.stone.NOM him.DAT his.course.GEN checks
‘(Look,) also the brook, […] when a great stone (rolling down from the high hills
falls in there, […],) withholding it (the brook) from its right course’ (Bo B9.3.2; eWS)
(30)He com hider mid hiwunge, […] ac se cingc him    <ælces> þinges    forwyrnde.
He came here with    trick   but the king him.DAT each.thing.GEN refused
‘He came here pretending, said (that he would be his man), […] but the king
refused him everything’(ChronC (O’Brien O’Keeffe) B17.7; lWS)
The domain is similar in size to (Dis)Possession, comprising 13 types, but larger in tokens, displaying 84 instances. Except for a newly attested type, bugan (refer to quotation (12) above for the corresponding context), and the addition of 42 new verb tokens to the database, the remaining terms and usages can be found in Visser’s list. Classes 15 and 16 are confirmed, but their distribution is not the same, with a clear emphasis on 15, where hindering and denying are extensively covered. Unlike the 12 attested dat-acc types in verbs of constraining (2019: 612), their counterparts in the dat-gen subconstruction do not appear to be equally productive. However, examination of quotation (12) above clearly confirms their presence and connections with social norms. In fact, bugan is very similar to dat-acc alternatives like hyrsumian ‘to obey, serve’ or ðegnian ‘to serve, minister’.
Quotation (29), the only token from Visser’s list included in this section so far, illustrates a non-prototypical non-human agent for hindering. Quotation (30) exemplifies the most productive type, forwyrnan ‘to refuse, deny’ (28 tokens). Despite competition from forbeodan ‘to forbid, prohibit’ and more directly from the family-related geteon wearne ‘to give (sb) a denial/refusal’ (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, p. 612), the 44 valid tokens for wyrnan and forwyrnan, as well as the existence of wiþcweþan and ofunnan, provide sufficient evidence to certify that denying, refusing, or rejecting was primarily performed by the dat-gen subconstruction.

5.7. Mode of Communication

This domain consists of verbs of communicated message (10) and instrument of communication (11). The diversity of types found (22) manifests the various ways in which metaphorical transfer (Reddy 1979; Lakoff and Johnson 1980; Lakoff et al. 1991; Lakoff 1993; Middeke 2022, p. 2) is expressed in OE. The 100 tokens available (nearly one-fourth of the database) further demonstrate the significance of speech verbs in the dat-gen subconstruction. The existence of class 11, verbs of instrument of communication, relates to concepts involving errands, songs, and charms (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 573–74). In this work, only evidence for the role of messengers is found. Additionally, we retrieve the following unattested dat-gen types: æteowian, andwyrdan, manian, and frignan.
list of verb types. Verb class 10: secgan ‘to say’ (3), spræcan ‘to say, tell’ (1), þancian ‘to give thanks’ (23), geþancian ‘to thank’ (4), fægnian ‘to rejoice, applaud’ (1), ætywan ‘to show, reveal’ (2), geswutelian ‘to explain, show’ (1), andwyrdan ‘to respond’ (3), geandwyrdan ‘to answer’ (2), oncweðan ‘to reply, respond’ (1), manian ‘to remind, suggest’ (1), asecan ‘to demand’ (1), styran2 ‘to reprove, chide, rebuke’ (4), styran3 ‘to correct’(1), biddan ‘to ask, make a request’ (36), abiddan ‘to pray for sth to sb’ (1), frignan ‘to ask sb about sth’ (5), rædes axian ‘to ask counsel of sb’ (1), wilnian ‘to desire, ask for’ (5), gewyscan ‘to wish for sth for sb’ (2), gyrnan ‘to desire’ (1). Verb class 11: ærendian ‘to report, intercede on an errand’ (1).
(31)an seoc man […] bæd    him    helpes   æt þan mannum    þe þær forðferdon.
a.sick.man.NOM asked him.DAT help.GEN to.the.men.DAT   that there went
‘(Then it happened one day that) an ill man begged for help for himself to
the men that went there’ (LS 9 (Giles) B3.3.9; lWS)
(32)ða […] cweð, feder, is me alyfed þet    ic    þe    mote    ohtes   fregnan?
Then said father is me allowed that I.NOM you.DAT may something.GEN ask
‘Then (he who had heard the heavenly song turned, bowing down onto the floor
and) said, Father, am I allowed to task you something?’(LS 3 (Chad)   B3.3.3; lWS)
(33)And se munuc   him   ða woplicre stefne    georne    þæs fultumes    þancode
And the monk him.DAT then weeping.voice sincerely    the.help.GEN    thanked
‘And the monk, with a mournful voice, sincerely thanked him for the help’
(LS 35 (VitPatr) B3.3.35; lWS < Angl.)
(34)we […] witon ðæt se esne    ðe ærendað his woroldhlaforde wifes,
we know that the.servant.NOM who pleads    his.lord.DAT    wife.GEN
‘Look! We know well enough that the servant who intercedes for his lord
about his wife’ (CP   B9.1.3; eWS)
The diversity of types is noticeable, encompassing options for saying, showing, telling, explaining, thanking, answering, reminding, asking for or demanding, desiring, etc. The most productive units are related to requests (biddan, frignan) and thanking (þancian, geþancian), but the remaining terms show low outputs. This is in contrast to what happens with Enabling, where there are 114 tokens distributed among a very small number of types.
Mode of Communication displays a diversity of subconstructional patterns. In speech verbs, the usual roles for the Nom and Dat cases are the communicator and addressee, respectively. Consider þancian in quotation (33) and frignan (32) as examples. The former operates exclusively as dat-gen, while the latter alternates between dat-gen and dat-acc, but both exhibit an addressee in the indirect object. However, a verb like biddan, which has 36 tokens in the database, features a Dat case that actually represents a beneficiary matching the agent, not the addressee. In these cases, we are dealing with a dative of interest. Thus, bæd him helpes in (31) means ‘begged for help for himself’. Optionally, the addressee may appear, but when it does, it is introduced prepositionally by æt ‘to, from’—æt þan mannum ‘to the men’. Other verbs like ærendian in quotation (34), wilnian, gewyscan, and gyrnan operate similarly.
In the dat-acc subconstruction, the total number of types rises to 59 (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, p. 613). Many terms alternate between the two subconstructions: secan, sprecan, andwyrdan, ætywan, geswutelian, and geærendian (instrument of communication). Some lexical families are more productive in the dat-acc subconstruction (cweðan, tocweðan, swutelian and geswutelian, ywan and ætywan), but the existence of types like biddan or acsian is marginal among dat-accs. Sometimes, we find idiomatic phrases operating as alternatives to types restricted to dat-gensdon þancas ‘to thank, give thanks’.
We have also validated the presence of benefactive and malefactive usages in Mode of Communication (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 589–90). Thanking, the second-highest-frequency term displaying 27 tokens for its unprefixed and prefixed variants, centers around a positive evaluation performed by the communicator. For instance, see quotation (33) from Ælfric’s Lives of Saints. Both types, prefixed and unprefixed, are exclusively linked with the dat-gen subconstruction. The evaluation, however, turns negative in cases like styran2 ‘to reprove, chide, rebuke’. Example (34) is the only attested dat-gen existing for class 11, instrument of communication. The quotation, extracted from King Alfred’s West Saxon version of Gregory’s Pastoral Care, depicts messengers who fall in love with the ladies they were tasked with acquiring. Finally, units like wilnian, gewyscan, and gyrnan also specify the close conceptual links existing between Mode of Communication and Mental Processes, the domain we now turn to studying.

5.8. Mental Processes

Mental Processes encompasses class 17, Mental Activity, and subclasses for verbs of thinking and wishing. We have also found evidence for fearing, but since there is only one type involved, we include the latter under subclass for thinking, leaving this aside for further investigation. Class 17 is constructionally very different from verbs of giving or granting, which display prototypical recipient-based semantics. In this domain, we find Nom experiencers operating different sorts of metaphorical transfer. We have found 13 verb types and 53 tokens. Gewilnian, cepan, and adrædan have not been attested in the dat-gen subconstruction so far.
list of verb types. Class 17. Subclass for thinking: wenan ‘to ween, suppose, think’ (23), hycgan ‘to think, consider’ (1), þencan ‘to think’ (2), gelyfan ‘to confide, trust, hope’ (4), ondrædan ‘to dread, fear’ (13), and adrædan ‘to fear, be afraid’ (1). Subclass for wishing: wilnian2 ‘to wish, desire’ (2), gewilnian ‘to wish, desire’ (1), wyscan ‘to wish for, desire’ (1), unnan2 ‘to wish sth for sb’ (1), cepan ‘to meditate, desire’ (2), unnan yfeles ‘to wish sb ill’ (1), and þencan yfeles ‘to wish sb ill’ (1).
(35)[…] him gescruncan ealle þa ædra, þæt himmon    þæs lifes ne wende
him dried up all the veins that him.DAT someone.NOM life.GEN not thought
‘(Then he began to bathe […] so sweaty that, due to the cold,) all his arteries
dried up to such an extent that nobody thought he would escape with his life’
(Or 3 B9.2.4; eWS)
(36)Se ðe oðerne lufað […] nele   he     him   hearmes   cepan
He who other loves no.want he.NOM him.DAT harm.GEN observe
‘He who loves another (without pretense) should not wish him any harm.’
(ÆCHom II, 40    B1.2.44; lWS)
(37)[…] he bið ðonne him self gewita ðæt he   wilnað him selfum   gielpes;
he is    then    himself   witness that he.NOM wishes him.self.DAT boasting.GEN
‘(He who then does not obtain the bishopric in pursuit of these (good)
deeds,) is himself a witness that he just desires vainglory for himself ’
(CP B9.1.3; eWS)
Quotation (35), extracted from Visser, is taken from the OE prose translation of the Book of Orosius, where the most frequent verb for thinking, wendan ‘to ween, think’, is used. Quotation (36) exemplifies an unattested unit for wishing, an antecedent to current idiomatic expressions like meaning someone harm. The dat-acc subconstruction shows a similar number of types this time (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, p. 614). Finally, (37) instantiates a group of verbs which may alternate between Mental Processes and Mode of Communication.

6. Analysis and Formalization

In this section, we begin by briefly considering the possibility of a dat-gen/gen-dat alternation. Then, building upon the findings described in Section 5.1, Section 5.2, Section 5.3, Section 5.4, Section 5.5, Section 5.6, Section 5.7, and Section 5.8 above, we proceed to represent the scope of the subconstruction into a semantic map (Malchukov et al. 2007, 2010). Additionally, we design the corresponding lexicality–schematicity hierarchy for dat-gens in OE, allowing us to progress from bottom to top, starting from the subclass-verb-specific level to the most schematic. Subsequently, we utilize CxG boxes to describe the most representative instances of OE dat-gens. Following a comparison of the OE data with those of Old Norse–Icelandic, we then proceed to produce a partial reconstruction of the dat-gen subconstruction for North-West Germanic.
In Section 3, we briefly mentioned that this subconstruction could manifest itself into either dat-gen or gen-dat. We have conducted a comprehensive analysis of five highly productive types to ascertain whether differences in pattern order indeed suggest the presence of an alternation. The units examined have been sampled out from different domains, geunnan (31 types) and getiþian (25) from Enabling, tilian (13) from Possession, wyrnan (16) from Retaining, and þancian (23) from Mode of Communication. Our investigation reveals that the five high-frequency types exhibit a diverse array of patterns, ranging from nom-dat-gen-v (44 tokens; see quotation (7) above) to gen-dat-v-nom (4 tokens; see quotation (8) above). In between, we observe other common options like split ditransitives of the (nom)-dat-v-gen type (16 tokens):
(38)God ælmihtig    himgeuðe swa micles wisdomes þæt æfre ænig ne wearð
God.almighty.NOM him.DAT gave so.great.wisdom.GEN that ever none not was
‘God Almighty granted him such powerful intelligence that there was never
anyone (like him)’(WHom 18   B2.3.6; lWS)
The writer’s choice of pattern seems to be influenced by various factors, with focalization and left fronting, for example, playing a role in the quotation provided in (8) above. Consequently, we can infer that, unlike what sometimes happens with the dat-acc/acc-dat alternation (de Cuypere 2015a, pp. 05–06), there is no preference for a particular type or group of types to follow either pattern exclusively.
The data collected from our corpus demonstrate that the scope of the dat-gen subconstruction encompasses verbs inherently signifying Giving, Enabling, Intention, (Mis)Creation, (Dis)Possession, Retaining, Mode of Communication, and Mental Processes. It is noteworthy that Deictically Specified Transfer remains unattested. All this evidence suggests that despite their idiosyncrasies, the dat-gen subconstruction shares the same major conceptual areas with the dat-acc subconstruction. This similarity extends to the activated verbal classes, with 13 out of the 15 proposed for the dat-acc subconstruction also being utilized. The exceptions are verbs of bringing (5) and sending (6), precisely the verb groups ascribed to Deictically Specified Transfer. Sharing an almost identical list of conceptual domains (8/9) and verb classes (13/15) confirms a high degree of similarity in the scope of the two subconstructions. Despite the graphical limitations of the one-dimensional representation below, Figure 4 effectively illustrates how the different verb classes are represented in the semantic map:
The layout enhances the typological validity of our study by distributing OE dat-gen ditransitive types among several constructional options. The Theme-Recipient construction holds a central position, with other options situated at the periphery. Ditransitive verb types are conveniently distributed across these options. We have adjusted the character size to reflect the number of quotations found for each type, with larger units indicating more productivity. Additionally, we specify the verbal class adscription (Barðdal et al. 2011). Similarly to contemporary English, the Internal Possessor and Patient Instrumental constructions remain inactive.
As observed in the map, the Theme-Recipient construction is more closely associated with Enabling than with Giving, where it is very poorly attested. Enabling contains 11 verb types and 112 tokens, with terms like geunnan and getiþian displaying around 30 quotations each. The terms in Enabling express actual and metaphorical transfer, relating to gift-giving but also responding positively to requests of various sorts. Mode of Communication, with transfer being effected only metaphorically, shares a similar total number of tokens with Enabling (100) but has a higher number of types (22). The data we have gathered show 36 quotations for biddan ‘to ask for’ compared to the 27 obtained for þancian ‘to give thanks’, including derived forms. Asking for (biddan) is not listed among Visser’s most frequently used terms (although he does exemplify the term profusely; 1963: 607–608), nor is it included in Mitchell’s description of section 1086 (1985: 452). Nevertheless, the usages are related to requests in the former case and, very frequently, to granted demands in the case of þancian.
In other areas of constructional space, productivity is slightly lower than in the previous two but is still adequately attested: Retaining (9 types, 50 tokens), (Dis)Possession (14, 40), and Mental Processes (13, 53). The first of these is situated between the spaces of the Malefactive Source and External Possessor constructions. Forwyrnan stands out for its polysemy and productivity. The term may mean ‘to refuse, deny’ (forwyrnan2, 28 tokens), being used malefactively, when satisfaction conditions are not met, or it may translate ‘to hinder, prevent’ (15 tokens, class 15). Indeed, the two types express different conceptualizations for the blocking of transfer. In (Dis)Possession, the most productive type is tilian, displaying 13 tokens in our corpus and being clearly related to recipient-based semantics. This contradicts the belief that the malefactive option of ‘taking away’ is one of the main concepts associated with dat-gens. Verbs of removal and spoliation are also represented (ofteon and benæman, class 13), but they account for almost a third (12/30) of the tokens compared to verbs of owning, stative and dynamic. Without the inclusion of wenan in Visser’s list, Mental Processes would seem to have been almost overlooked in the existing literature. However, this domain, where recipient-based semantics is envisioned by experiencers, displays nearly as many types as (Dis)Possession, with approximately the same number of tokens (13, 53). In the map, we have selected wenan (23 tokens) for verbs of thinking and wilnian2 for wishing. The second term also illustrates the close links between Mode of Communication and Mental Processes.
Unlike the domains analyzed above, Giving, Intention, and (Mis)Creation exhibit very poor productivity in terms of types and tokens. In Intention, this lack may be due, among other reasons, to a more common use of other subconstructional options—dat-acc for geleanian and gehatan, for instance. For Miscreation, which lies in the areas for the Patient-Beneficiary and Malefactive Source constructions, we nevertheless managed to verify the two classes involved and most of the verbal subclasses.
Gaps in the semantic map also provide significant insights. Despite the obvious limitations of our query and the need for further research on dat-gens, the absence of a particular verbal class or subclass that is present in the dat-acc subconstruction serves as additional (provisional) evidence for semantic specialization. In this regard, the most notable difference is the lack of a continuum moving from Theme-Recipient to Theme-Goal (compare Figure 2 and Figure 4 above) in the map. Besides the absence of verb classes 5 (bringing) and 6 (sending), subclasses for entrusting, distributing, and giving back (which includes verbs of paying) are missing from class 2 in verbs of giving, a domain with very limited instances. Additionally, we have not been able to identify the subclass for blocking the path (Miscreation). Another notable gap is the absence of subclass-specific verbs for forgiving in class 9 under Enabling.
Despite incorporating prefixed and unprefixed variant verb forms, the semantic map displayed above does not fully represent the hierarchical organization of the constructicon (Boas 2010; Fillmore et al. 2012), which progresses from the most specific to the highest schematic level in a lexicality–schematicity continuum. Drawing on a previous proposal for Proto-Germanic that equally applies to OE dat-accs (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 596–97; apud Croft 2003), we postulate the following lexicality–schematicity hierarchy for the dat-gen subconstruction in OE. Figure 5 and Figure 6 below immediately display this hierarchy.
The proposed hierarchy should be perceived as being complementary to the semantic map above, as it facilitates navigation below and above the verb-specific level. Observe that prefixed verbs, which operationalize sub-specific constructions, occupy the lowest position in the hierarchy. Moving one step upwards, we can observe verb-specific constructions containing the verbs used in the dat-gen pattern, each displaying its own argument structure formalization.
At a higher level, we encounter the level of verb-class-specific constructions. This is, for instance, where we find verbs of owning and obtaining (classes 1 and 15) differentiating themselves constructionally, and where we detect the absence of some verb-subclass-specific constructions in Giving (see Figure 5 above). Verb-class-specific constructions, such as owning and obtaining, in turn merge into the corresponding higher-level conceptual domain, (Dis)Possession. Thus, we obtain the eight higher-level conceptual domains discussed in Section 5.1, Section 5.2, Section 5.3, Section 5.4, Section 5.5, Section 5.6, Section 5.7, and Section 5.8 above. Finally, we reach the highest level representing the patterning of V-Dat-Gen with the most schematic meaning. We postulate a linking mechanism operating at the highest schematic level for the conflation of dat-gens with the rest of subconstructions. It is now time to operationalize the syntax–semantics of OE dat-gens into DCxG.
In the diachronic theoretical approach followed here, we utilize traditional boxes from CxG, adapting them for historical reconstruction (Barðdal 2023; Barðdal et al. 2023, 2020). Each box is divided into three fields, form, syn (syntax), and sem (semantics). Please refer to Figure 7 below for an illustration. Since this work focuses primarily on the analysis of the OE dat-gen subconstruction, form represents verb-subclass-specific and verb-specific instantiations of real constructions initially. The syn area provides required information about a unit’s argument structure, specifying the arguments and cases activated in OE. The sem section in the box outlines the semantic frame (Baker et al. 2003; Boas 2005) and roles involved. Each role is assigned a syntactic argument by indexing.
The arguments in contemporary constructions tend to be fixed. This, to some extent, also holds for the (nom-)dat-acc subconstruction (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 598–606). As mentioned earlier, the relative freedom of word order allowed for many variants to coexist, particularly for dat-gens. For convenience, we have opted for an all-inclusive patterning, nom-dat-gen, which best describes the frontings of subjects and of datives versus genitives, two very frequent options in our data.
We now proceed to display the formalism described above in a series of boxes, aimed at portraying the complexity of the dat-gen subconstruction, which irradiates from the Theme-Recipient construction into a diversity of typologically distinct constructional areas. This diversity results in a constantly evolving argument structure for dat-gens, especially given the expanded scope of our subconstruction when compared to modern English. Figure 7, presented above, illustrates the verb-specific construction for unnan ‘to grant’.
As observed, in this subconstruction, donors and recipients sometimes yield to speakers and addressees. The most frequently used verb of speech in dat-gens involves requests directed to someone for yourself—biddan. In this frame (see Figure 8 immediately below), the roles of speaker and beneficiary coincide (nomi-dati in syn). The addressee in this case is optional, and when included, it is typically presented within a prepositional phrase introduced by æt ‘to’. For an example, please check quotation (31) above.
At other times, the transfer may be blocked. This occurs most frequently in the domain of the Malefactive Source construction, and also applies to speech verbs like forwyrnan ‘to deny’, which are related to Goldberg’s verbs of refusal and categorized as Retaining here. For lack of space, we only specify the frame for this verb-specific construction, namely deny-permission, and the thematic roles used (syn is the same as above). The agent/speaker is now identified as an authorityi, the addressee as a protagonistj, and the message pertains to an actionk (or perhaps a material possession in prospect).
Finally, in Mental Processes, neither agents (or donors) nor speakers are found, but rather experiencers. Wyscan ‘to wish for’ serves as a case in point. This time the frame is desiring, the nominative is an experiencer, the dative a focal participant (sometimes affected), and the genitive denotes the intended event.
In order to further develop the box formalism for the reconstruction of the dat-gen subconstruction, it is essential to compare OE with other Germanic languages. The following section represents an initial attempt to partially reconstruct the core structure of the North-West Germanic dat-gen subconstruction. We also propose a few possible Proto-Germanic verb-specific constructions, acknowledging the necessity of conducting a comprehensive analysis of dat-gens in Old Norse–Icelandic and, if available, in Gothic—this analysis is deferred for future research. We utilize the concise outline provided for Old Norse–Icelandic (Barðdal et al. 2011) as summarized in Section 3 above, and then establish a correspondence set (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 601, 603). Drawing from the former and incorporating some new findings, we present a provisional partial reconstruction of the dat-gen subconstruction in North-West Germanic, which will necessitate a more detailed analysis in the near future.
In Table 2, Section 3 above, the following evidence was provided for the low type frequency dat-gen subconstruction in Old Norse–Icelandic: Giving (fá e-m e-s ‘give sb sth’), Intention (væna e-m e-s ‘promise sb sth’), Possession (afla e-m e-s ‘get sb sth’), Retaining (synja e-m e-s ‘deny sb sth’), and Mental Processes (æskja e-m e-s ‘wish sth for sb’). Starting from the list of verb types compiled for OE, we consulted the etymological dictionary of Indo-European (Pokorny 1959–1969) as well as the etymological dictionary of Proto-Germanic (Kroonen 2013) in the search for cognates, and later used Cleasby et al. (1986) to verify if the cognates found operationalized the dat-gen pattern. Despite obtaining poor results, we managed to retrieve a few more cognates: unna e-m e-s ‘to grant, allow sb sth’ (ann ek honum ísetu í dómi ‘I granted him a sitting in judgement’; Grág. i. 17, 78), varna e-m e-s ‘to deny sb sth’ (skal þér eigi þessa varna ‘I will not deny you these’; Ld. 250), biðja e-m e-s ‘to beg sb for sth’ (er þér þess ekki biðjanda. ‘…who is not begging you this’; Eg. 423), and væna e-m e-s ‘to ween, think’, which was acknowledged already as ‘to promise’ (Barðdal et al. 2011, p. 74). We can now propose the following verb-specific correspondence sets as shown in Table 3 immediately following.
We thus find conclusive evidence for the existence of the domains of Enabling (unnan/unna) and Mode of Communication (biddan/biðja) for the first time. Therefore, dat-gens in Old Norse–Icelandic are also attested in most domains associated with dat-accs except for Deictically Specified Transfer in both languages and (Mis)Creation in Old Norse–Icelandic. However, the number of validated verb classes (eight) is more limited. It is now time to return to boxes to provide an instance of a verb subclass-specific (re)construction, which is situated immediately above the verb-specific level, as shown in Figure 9 below.
At this level, the form parameter is no longer applicable, since the verb-specific constructions embedded are phonologically distinct. We have chosen verbs of conferring under Enabling, adding a dotted line to the OE and Old Norse–Icelandic sections in the SEM frame in the hope of identifying new incorporations into these groups in future works. It is important to note that, at this level, we formalize verb-subclass-specific results for both languages and should thus be able to perform reconstruction. Due to the lack of a larger stock of correspondence sets focusing on each verbal subclass, reconstruction has not been possible at this level, except for *unnan- ‘to grant, concede’. Perhaps the low productivity of Old Norse–Icelandic dat-gens plays a role in this respect, or, more probably, we simply need a larger sample size from this language. Nevertheless, despite being extremely limited, some North-West-Germanic dat-gen (re)constructions are feasible at the next, higher level, as displayed in Figure 10 following.
We have utilized Proto-Germanic etymons because both *unnan- and *lihwan- have corresponding word form variants in Gothic, Old English, and Old Norse–Icelandic, so the correspondence sets this time clearly function. All that remains is to examine the pattern in quotations for the corresponding Gothic word forms. The results are understandably very limited, but it has been possible to reconstruct two constructions associated with Enabling, namely conferring and lending. We can also reasonably assume at this level other (re)constructions in other domains: *bedjan- ‘to ask, beg, pray’ (Mode of Communication), *wunskjan- ‘to wish’ (Mental Processes), and *wǣnjan ‘to think, hope’ (Old Germanic this time; cf. dictionary entry for ween (Oxford University Press 2023)). We were unable to find the Proto-Germanic etymon for the pair wyrnan/varna ‘to deny, refuse’ (Retaining), even though the two terms are ascribed to a lengthened grade of *u̯er- ‘to guard, deny’ in Pokorny.
Finally, the DCxG approach we adhere to completes the analysis by furnishing a description of the most schematic level of all, the event type.
Figure 11 below offers a very limited reconstruction of the event-type construction for Noth-West Germanic dat-gens. The data in the correspondence set above prove the existence of four major conceptual domains: while Enabling conveys pure recipient-based semantics, Retaining focuses on the prevention of transfer. Mode of Communication metaphorically adjusts actual transfer to speakers and addressees, while the persistent presence of Mental Processes (wyscan/æskja, wenan/væna) also illustrates that experiencers play a role in recipient-based semantics. The reconstruction encompasses only four out of possibly eight conceptual domains for which we have evidence, but it suffices to demonstrate level-specific and schematic affinity between the two languages and partial overlap with the corresponding dat-acc Proto-Germanic subconstruction (Vázquez-González and Barðdal 2019, pp. 574–91). The validation of the four other attested domains awaits further investigation. We now turn to drawing conclusions.

7. Conclusions and Directions for Further Research

This study advances prior research about the OE dat-gen subconstruction in several ways. Firstly, it builds upon Visser’s list of 54 types and 183 tokens, expanding it to a total of 88 types and 443 tokens, with 19 types and 260 tokens newly added. The increase in data can be attributed to the current possibility of performing computerized searches in corpora like DOEWC, as well as to the specific searches conducted in this investigation, which have yielded a significant amount of new information. Therefore, this work utilizes Visser’s and Mitchell’s lists, expanding upon them to provide a more comprehensive analysis.
The augmented dataset concerning the OE dat-gen subconstruction, along with the application of the DCxG model as explained above, enables a comprehensive reassessment of the issue. Some traditional accounts have downplayed the necessity for a separate analysis of this subconstruction. By rightly arguing that the same type may appear operating in other alternating subconstructions, it seems that a detailed analysis of dat-gens per se may not be necessary. Other extremely valuable proposals, such as Visser’s, offer insights into the most frequent verbs and organize results into an A-Z list but refrain from offering a detailed account of the semantics for the units or grouping them into specific sets. This study presents a DCxG account of old and new data, demonstrating that dat-gen usages are clearly related to the 8 conceptual domains and 13 verb-specific classes described earlier. Furthermore, it shows that the corpus assembled for this investigation cannot be categorized differently. Thus, we can confidently assert the presence of (sub)constructional systematicity. It is upon this systematicity that the verb-specific constructions operating as dat-gens are based, with each one contributing its own specificity, particularly those that exclusively or predominantly utilize the cited pattern.
An examination of the semantic map proposed in Figure 4 and the discussions in Section 5.1, Section 5.2, Section 5.3, Section 5.4, Section 5.5, Section 5.6, Section 5.7, and Section 5.8 above can help delineate the intricate scope of the dat-gen subconstruction. As previously noted, more than half of the attested types are speech verbs. One key aspect concerns actual transfer, which is expressed in various ways, such as in Anglo-Saxon gift-giving (unnan, tiþian), while also encompassing other options, like obtaining things through effort (tilian) or, ultimately, enjoying them (neotan). Sometimes, rather than actual transfer, we find the desire for it—like wishing things for oneself (wilnian2). On numerous occasions, including those encompassing some of the aforementioned options, transfer takes on a metaphorical dimension. This phenomenon is particularly evident in Domain 7, Section 5.7 above, which centers on requests (biddan), expressions of gratitude (þancian), and various forms of communication elucidated in this study. Finally, as crucial as the concept of actual transfer is for dat-gens, whether literal or metaphorical, the absence of it also plays a significant role, too—highlighting the importance of recipient-based semantics indirectly. While it is easy to envision the role Anglo-Saxon lords may have played in denying gifts (forwyrnan), we should also acknowledge the demonstrated significance of events that involve hindrance and withholding (forwyrnan2, gestyran), apart from deprivation (ofteon).
The existing constructional systematicity also extends to the clear-cut affinities between the dat-gen and dat-acc subconstructions. We have corroborated for OE dat-gens and dat-accs a previous claim, applicable to Old Norse–Icelandic, concerning the existence of partial overlap between the subconstructions of the ditransitive construction. By demonstrating that these two variants predominantly operate within the same conceptual domains and activate an extremely similar catalog of verb classes, we confirm Croft’s semantic connectivity hypothesis (apud Barðdal et al. 2011, p. 75) for OE data. Thus, we show that OE dat-gens and dat-accs share the same constructional areas, with a dat-gen verb-specific type or perhaps an entire constructional class participating in the same space with other subconstructional options that may be conceptually proximate. In this regard, despite not being an alternational account, this work also provides some insights into how this competition may occasionally manifest.
The findings obtained for OE have also been contrastively verified, as Old Norse–Icelandic dat-gens activate 7/8 conceptual domains and over half of the verb classes displayed by their OE counterparts. In this regard, Enabling and Mode of Communication are added to the list of domains for Old Norse–Icelandic, along with a few more verb-specific types. Going beyond its primary aim, this work has also partially reconstructed the dat-gen subconstruction in North-West Germanic, serving as an illustration of the potential of the DCxG model employed. With the support of an additional correspondence set, the partial reconstruction of the event type construction in North-West Germanic includes those domains that are precisely more frequent in OE: Enabling, Mode of Communication, Retaining, and Mental Processes. While the reconstruction may be partial, it stands on a solid foundation.
Regarding the North-West Germanic reconstruction of dat-gens, there is still work to be done on Old Norse–Icelandic and on Old English. Further research is needed for the latter, while the former requires more data to obtain a broader overview of dat-gens. Only by increasing the number of correspondence sets can a complete reconstruction of the subconstruction be achieved. A similar situation applies to Gothic, which has not been covered in this work. Further analysis of the Old Norse–Icelandic and Gothic data is thus required in the near future. Otherwise, the ultimate reconstruction of the dat-gen subconstruction in Proto-Germanic will not be possible.
As stated in the introduction, the primary search conducted in this work represents only the initial phase of a more comprehensive analysis that will eventually encompass the entirety of personal pronouns, with the possibility of later incorporating the study of OE noun phrases. In our view, there is still ample opportunity for new findings regarding OE dat-gens before the final picture is completed. Nonetheless, we believe our study offers a detailed description of the scope of the OE dat-gen subconstruction. The 88 types and 443 tokens covered serve as just the beginning in the design of a future OE dat-gen open access database, and questions regarding its design will require our attention in the near future.

Funding

This research was funded by the I+D+I project grant IPID2020-119200GB-100 [(MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033/)], which is gratefully acknowledged here.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Research data available at https://www.uhu.es/fhum/documentos/estudios/profesores/107-cv.pdf (accessed on 15 April 2024).

Acknowledgments

I would like to express my sincere gratitude to Juan A. Estévez-Sola from Huelva University for his unwavering support in reviewing the syntax and semantics of many original Latin sources. His contributions have undoubtedly enhanced the quality and reliability of this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Baker, Collin F., Charles. J. Fillmore, and Beau Cronin. 2003. The Structure of the FrameNet Database. International Journal of Lexicography 16: 281–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Barðdal, Jóhanna, and Spike Gildea. 2015. Diachronic Construction Grammar: Epistemological context, basic assumptions and historical implications. In Diachronic Construction Grammar. Edited by Jóhanna Barðdal, Elena Smirnova, Lotte Sommerer and Spike Gildea. Amsterdam and New York: John Benjamins, pp. 1–50. [Google Scholar]
  3. Barðdal, Jóhanna, and Thórhallur Eythórsson. 2020. How to identify cognates in syntax? Taking Watkin’s legacy one step further. In Reconstructing Syntax. Brill’s Studies in Historical Linguistics, 11. Edited by Jóhanna Barðdal, Spike Gildea and Eugenio Luján. Leiden and Boston: Brill, pp. 197–238. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barðdal, Jóhanna, Eleonora Cattafi, Serena Danesi, Laura Bruno, and Leonardo Biondo. 2023. Non-nominative subjects in Latin and Ancient Greek: Applying the subject tests on early Indo-European languages. Indogermanische Forschungen 128: 321–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Barðdal, Jóhanna, Kristian E. Kristoffersen, and Andreas Sveen. 2011. West Scandinavian ditransitives as a family of constructions: With a special attention to the Norwegian V-REFL-NP construction. Linguistics 49: 53–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Barðdal, Jóhanna, Spike Gildea, and Eugenio Luján. 2020. Reconstructing Syntax. Brill’s Studies in Historical Linguistics, 11. Leiden and Boston: Brill. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barðdal, Jóhanna, Thórhallur Eythórsson, and Tonya K. Dewey. 2019. The alternating predicate puzzle: Dat-nom vs. nom-dat in Icelandic and German. Constructions and Frames 11: 107–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Barðdal, Jóhanna. 2007. The semantic and lexical range of the ditransitive construction in the history of (North) Germanic. Functions of Language 14: 9–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Barðdal, Jóhanna. 2023. Oblique Subjects in Germanic: Their Status, History and Reconstruction. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Boas, Hans C. 2005. Semantic Frames as Interlingual Representations for Multilingual Lexical Databases. International Journal of Lexicography 18: 445–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Boas, Hans C. 2010. The syntax-lexicon continuum in construction grammar: A case study of English communication verbs. Belgian Journal of Linguistics 24: 54–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bosworth, Joseph, and Thomas. N. Toller. 1973. An Anglo-Saxon Dictionary. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bresnan, Joan. 2007. Is syntactic knowledge probabilistic? Experiments with the English dative alternation. In Roots: Linguistics in Search of Its Evidential Base. Studies in Generative Grammar Series; Edited by Sam Featherston and Wolfgang Sternefeld. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 77–96. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bucci, Giacomo, and Jóhanna Barðdal. 2024. Dative Subjects in Gothic: Evidence from Word Order. Indogermanische Forschungen 129. Forthcoming. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cleasby, Richard, Guðbrandur Vigfusson, and William A. Craigie. 1986. An Icelandic-English dictionary. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Colleman, Timothy, and Bernard De Clerck. 2011. Constructional semantics on the Move: On semantic specialization in the English double object construction. Cognitive Linguistics 22: 183–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Croft, William. 2003. Lexical rules vs. constructions: A false dichotomy. In Motivation in language: Studies in Honour of Günter Radden. Edited by Hubert Cuyckens, Thomas Berg, René Dirven and Klaus-Uwe Panther. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 49–68. [Google Scholar]
  18. de Cuypere, Ludovic. 2015a. A multivariate analysis of the old English ACC + DAT double object alternation. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 11: 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. de Cuypere, Ludovic. 2015b. The Old English to-dative construction. English Language and Linguistics 19: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dunn, Michael, Tonya. K. Dewey, Carlee Arnett, Thórhallur Eythórsson, and Jóhanna Barðdal. 2017. Dative sickness: A phylogenetic analysis of argument structure evolution in Germanic. Language 93: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ferraresi, Gisella. 2005. Word Order and Phrase Structure in Gothic. Leuven: Peeters. [Google Scholar]
  22. Fillmore, Charles J., Russell R. Lee-Goldman, and Russell Rhodes. 2012. The FrameNet Constructicon. In Sign-Based Construction Grammar. CSLI Lecture Note 193. Edited by Hans Boas and Ivan Sag. Stanford: SCLI Publications, pp. 309–72. [Google Scholar]
  23. Frotscher, Michael, Guus Kroonen, and Jóhanna Barðdal. 2022. Indo-European inroads into the syntactic-etymological interface: A reconstruction of the Pie verbal root menkw- ‘to be short; to lack’ and its argument structure. Historische Sprachforschung 133: 62–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Gildea, Spike, and Flávia de Castro Alves. 2020. Reconstructing the Source of Nominative-Absolutive Alignment in Two Amazonian Language Families. In Reconstructing Syntax. Brill’s Studies in Historical Linguistics, 11. Edited by Jóhanna Barðdal, Spike Gildea and Eugenio Luján. Leiden and Boston: Brill, pp. 47–107. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gildea, Spike, and Jóhanna Barðdal. 2023. From grammaticalization to Diachronic Construction Grammar: A natural evolution of the paradigm. Studies in Language 47: 743–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Healey, Antonette diPaolo, ed. 2016. Dictionary of Old English in Electronic Form A-H. Toronto: Dictionary of Old English Project. [Google Scholar]
  28. Healey, Antonette diPaolo, John P. Wilkin, and Xin Xiang. 2015. The Dictionary of Old English Web Corpus. Toronto: University of Toronto. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hughes, Arthur, Peter Trudgill, and Dominic Watt. 2013. English Accents and Dialects. An Introduction to Social and Regional Varieties of English in the British Isles. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jones, Christian. 2022. What are the basics of analysing a corpus? In The Routledge Handbook of Corpus Linguistics. Edited by Anne O’Keeffe and Michael. J. McArthy. London and New York: Routledge, pp. 126–39. [Google Scholar]
  31. Kroonen, Guus. 2013. Etymological Dictionary of Proto-Germanic. Leiden: Brill. [Google Scholar]
  32. Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. 1980. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  33. Lakoff, George, Jane Espenson, and Alan Schwartz. 1991. Second Draft Copy. Master Metaphor List. Berkeley: Cognitive Linguistics Group, University of California at Berkeley. Available online: https://www.lang.osaka-u.ac.jp/~sugimoto/MasterMetaphorList/MasterMetaphorList2.pdf (accessed on 7 February 2024).
  34. Lakoff, George. 1993. The contemporary theory of metaphor. In Metaphor and Thought, 2nd ed. Edited by Andrew Ortony. Cambridge: Cambride University Press, pp. 202–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Levin, Beth. 1993. English Verb Classes and Alternations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  36. Luján, Eugenio, and Ángel López-Chala. 2020. Reconstructing Semantic Roles: Proto-Indo-European *-bhi. In Reconstructing Syntax. Edited by Jóhanna Barðdal, Spike Gildea and Eugenio Luján. Brill’s Studies in Historical Linguistics 11. Leiden and Boston: Brill, pp. 336–70. [Google Scholar]
  37. Luján, Eugenio, and César Ruiz Abad. 2014. Semantic roles and word formation. Instrument and Location in Ancient Greek. In Perspectives on Semantic Roles. Typological Studies in Language 106. Edited by Silvia Luraghi and H. Narrog. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 241–70. [Google Scholar]
  38. Malchukov, Andrej, Martin Haspelmath, and Bernard Comrie. 2007. Ditransitive Constructions: A Typological Overview. First draft, September 2007. Available online: https://www.keel.ut.ee/sites/default/files/www_ut/4-ditransitiveoverview.pdf (accessed on 16 October 2014).
  39. Malchukov, Andrej, Martin Haspelmath, and Bernard Comrie. 2010. Ditransitive constructions: A typological overview. In Studies in Ditransitive Constructions: A Comparative Handbook. Edited by Andrej Malchukov, Martin Haspelmath and Bernard Comrie. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 1–64. [Google Scholar]
  40. McEnery, Tony, and Gavin Brookes. 2022. Building a written corpus: What are the basics? In The Routledge Handbook of Corpus Linguistics. Edited by Anne O’Keeffe and Michael J. McArthy. London and New York: Routledge, pp. 35–47. [Google Scholar]
  41. Michaelis, Laura A. 2012. Sign-Based Construction Grammar. In The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar. Edited by Thomas Hoffmann and Graeme Trousdale. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 133–52. [Google Scholar]
  42. Middeke, Kirsten. 2022. The Old English Case System. Case and Argument Structure Constructions. Brill’s Studies in Historical Linguistics, 12. Leiden and Boston: Brill. [Google Scholar]
  43. Miller, D. Gary. 2019. The Oxford Gothic Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  44. Mitchell, Bruce. 1985. Old English Syntax, Vol. 1: Concord, the Parts of Speech, and the Sentence. Oxford: Clarendon. [Google Scholar]
  45. Mölig-Falke, Ruth. 2015. Using the Dictionary of Old English Corpus for linguistic analyses: A basic classification of the textual sources. Neuphilologische Mitteilungen 116: 395–420. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ogura, Michiko. 2010. Old English Verbs with a Genitive Object: A Doomed Group? In Aspects of the History of English Language and Literature: Selected Papers Read at SHELL 2009, Hiroshima. Edited by Osamu Imahayashi, Yoshiyuki Nakao and Michiko Ogura. Frankfurt am Main, Berlin, Bern, Bruxelles, New York, Oxford and Wien: Peter Lang, pp. 55–72. [Google Scholar]
  47. Oxford University Press. 2023. Ween v. In Oxford English Dictionary. Oxford: Oxford University Press, Retrieved 17 March 2023. [Google Scholar]
  48. Perek, Florence. 2020. Productivity and schematicity in constructional change. In Nodes and Networks in Diachronic Construction Grammar. Edited by Lotte Sommerer and Elena Smirnova. Constructional Approaches to Language. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 141–66. [Google Scholar]
  49. Pokorny, Julius. 1959–1969. Indogermanisches Etymologisches Wörterbuch. Bern: Francke. [Google Scholar]
  50. Reddy, Michael J. 1979. The conduit metaphor. A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In Metaphor and Thought. Edited by Andrew Ortony. Cambridge: University Press, pp. 284–324. [Google Scholar]
  51. Roberts, Jane, Christian Kay, and Lynne Grundy. 1995. A Thesaurus of Old English. Amsterdam: Rodopi, Volume I–II. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rousseau, André. 2016. Gotica: Études sur la Langue Gotique. Paris: Champion. [Google Scholar]
  53. Somers, Joren, Gard. B. Jenset, and Jóhanna Barðdal. 2024. Argument structure constructions in competition: The Dat-Nom/Nom-Dat Alternation in Icelandic. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 2024: 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Taylor, Ann, Anthony Warner, Susan Pintzuk, and Frank Beths. 2003. The York-Toronto-Helsinki Parsed Corpus of Old English Prose. York: Department of Language and Linguistic Science, University of York. [Google Scholar]
  55. Vázquez-González, Juan G., and Jóhanna Barðdal. 2019. Reconstructing the ditransitive construction for Proto-Germanic: Gothic, Old English and Old Norse–Icelandic. Folia Linguistica Historica 40: 555–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Visser, Frederik T. 1963. An Historical Syntax of the English Language. Leiden: Brill. [Google Scholar]
  57. Yañez-Bouza, Nuria, and David Denison. 2015. Which comes first in the double object construction? English Language and Linguistics 19: 247–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Zanchi, Chiara, and Matteo Tarsi. 2021. Valency patterns and alternations in Gothic. In Valency over Time. Diachronic Perspectives on Valency Patterns and Valency Orientation. Edited by Silvia Luraghi and Elisa Roma. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 32–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The polysemy of the ditransitive construction (Goldberg 1995, p. 38).
Figure 1. The polysemy of the ditransitive construction (Goldberg 1995, p. 38).
Languages 09 00213 g001
Figure 2. The semantics of the ditransitive construction in Old English.
Figure 2. The semantics of the ditransitive construction in Old English.
Languages 09 00213 g002
Figure 3. The five case subconstructions in Old Norse–Icelandic (Barðdal et al. 2011, p. 75).
Figure 3. The five case subconstructions in Old Norse–Icelandic (Barðdal et al. 2011, p. 75).
Languages 09 00213 g003
Figure 4. The semantics of the dat-gen subconstruction in Old English.
Figure 4. The semantics of the dat-gen subconstruction in Old English.
Languages 09 00213 g004
Figure 5. The lexicality–schematicity hierarchy of the OE dat-gen subconstruction, 1.
Figure 5. The lexicality–schematicity hierarchy of the OE dat-gen subconstruction, 1.
Languages 09 00213 g005
Figure 6. The lexicality–schematicity hierarchy of the OE dat-gen subconstruction, 2.
Figure 6. The lexicality–schematicity hierarchy of the OE dat-gen subconstruction, 2.
Languages 09 00213 g006
Figure 7. Formalizing the verb-specific nom-unnan-dat-gen construction.
Figure 7. Formalizing the verb-specific nom-unnan-dat-gen construction.
Languages 09 00213 g007
Figure 8. Formalizing the verb-specific nom-biddan-dat-gen construction.
Figure 8. Formalizing the verb-specific nom-biddan-dat-gen construction.
Languages 09 00213 g008
Figure 9. Verb-subclass-specific constructions for North-West Germanic conferring.
Figure 9. Verb-subclass-specific constructions for North-West Germanic conferring.
Languages 09 00213 g009
Figure 10. Verb-class-specific reconstruction of Enabling in North-West Germanic.
Figure 10. Verb-class-specific reconstruction of Enabling in North-West Germanic.
Languages 09 00213 g010
Figure 11. The event-type dat-gen subconstruction in North-West Germanic.
Figure 11. The event-type dat-gen subconstruction in North-West Germanic.
Languages 09 00213 g011
Table 1. Additional senses of the ditransitive construction in the Germanic languages.
Table 1. Additional senses of the ditransitive construction in the Germanic languages.
verb classconstructional types
1Verbs denoting possession/owning:eiga sér e-ð ‘have sth’, geyma sér e- ð, ‘save sth for oneself’, spara sér e-ð ‘’ save sth for oneself’, etc.
3verbs of lending:lána e-m e-ð ‘lend sb sth’, leigja e-m e-ð ‘hire sth out to sb’, ljá e-m e-ð ‘lend sb sth’, etc.
4verbs of paying:borga e-m e-ð ‘pay sb for sth’, gjalda e-m e-ð ‘pay sb sth’, launa e-m e-ð ‘reward sb with sth’, etc.
8verbs denoting transfer along a path:brjóta sér leið ‘break oneself a passage’, opna e-m e-ð ‘open up a passage for sb’, etc.
14verbs of utilizing:(hag)nýta sér e-ð ‘make most of/use of sth’, nota sér eitthvað ‘use sth for oneself’, etc.
15verbs of hindrance:banna e-m e-ð ‘forbid sb to do sth’, byrgja e-m sýn ‘block the view for sb’, etc.
16verbs of constraining:einsetja sér e-ð ‘resolve to do sth’, setja e-m e-ð ‘give sb a task’, etc.
17verbs of mental activity:gera sér e-ð ljóst ‘realize sth’, hugsa sér e-ð ‘think of sth’, etc.
Table 2. The low type frequency dat-gen subconstruction (Barðdal et al. 2011, p. 74).
Table 2. The low type frequency dat-gen subconstruction (Barðdal et al. 2011, p. 74).
actual transfer
verbs of giving or delivering:fá e-m e-s ‘give sb sth’
verbs of lending:ljá e-m e-s ‘lend sb sth’
verbs of obtaining: afta e-m e-s ‘get sb sth’, leita e-m e-s ‘get sb sth’
intention
verbs of future transfer:væna e-m e-s ‘promise sb sth’
retaining
verbs of hindrance:synja e-m e-s ‘deny sb sth’
mental processes
verbs denoting mental activity:æskja e-m e-s ‘wish sth for sb’, unna e-m e-s ‘not begrudge sb sth’
Table 3. Correspondence sets for Old English/Old Norse–Icelandic dat-gens.
Table 3. Correspondence sets for Old English/Old Norse–Icelandic dat-gens.
Old EnglishOld Norse-Icelandic
nom-unnan-dat-gennom-unna-dat-gen
nom-leon-dat-gennom-ljá-dat-gen
nom-(for)wyrnan-dat-gennom-varna-dat-gen
nom-biddan-dat-gennom-biðja-dat-gen
nom-wyscan-dat-gennom-æskja-dat-gen
nom-wenan-dat-gennom-væna-dat-gen
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Vázquez-González, J.G. Updating Old English Dative–Genitives: A Diachronic Construction Grammar Account. Languages 2024, 9, 213. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9060213

AMA Style

Vázquez-González JG. Updating Old English Dative–Genitives: A Diachronic Construction Grammar Account. Languages. 2024; 9(6):213. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9060213

Chicago/Turabian Style

Vázquez-González, Juan G. 2024. "Updating Old English Dative–Genitives: A Diachronic Construction Grammar Account" Languages 9, no. 6: 213. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9060213

APA Style

Vázquez-González, J. G. (2024). Updating Old English Dative–Genitives: A Diachronic Construction Grammar Account. Languages, 9(6), 213. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9060213

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop