The current study investigated child heritage speaker’s overregularization of Spanish past participles, as in
escribido (cf.
escrito), by means of an elicited production task. Four research questions guided the study, the first focusing on what factors predicted whether the children would respond with a participle in the production task. Meanwhile, the second question examined the overall rates of overregularization and the ages at which overregularization occurs. The third question focused on the impact of lexical frequency on overregularization, which has ramifications for our understanding of the role of language experience underlying overregularization and children’s retreat from it. Finally, the last question focused on whether overregularization is predicted by Spanish language experience and proficiency, factors that have been shown to predict other areas of the developing child heritage grammar (e.g.,
Shin et al. 2023). In this section, we discuss each research question in turn. Regarding the first research question, it must be noted that seven out of 20 children produced no past participles at all (
Table 5). Compared to the children who did produce participles, the ones who did not were significantly younger and had significantly lower language background questionnaire scores, indicating less Spanish language experience in the home and at school, as well as lower Spanish morphosyntax scores. It is possible that these children had not yet acquired past participles, although it would be necessary to test the children’s receptive knowledge of these forms to establish that this is the case (e.g.,
Giancaspro and Sánchez 2021). The lack of a comprehension task to test for the children’s knowledge of past participles in the structure studied is a limitation of the current study. Next, we turn our discussion to overregularization among the 13 child heritage speakers whose responses included past participles.
5.1. Overall Rates and Age of Overregularization
Our second research question addressed the overall rates and ages at which Spanish-English bilingual children born and raised in an English-dominant society produce overregularized past participles. The sampled children overregularized past participles at unusually high rates; 74% of their past participles were an overregularized variant (e.g.,
hacido). Not only did children produce expected overregularized variants like
hacido using the infinitival stem and the regular -
do morpheme, but there were also innovative variants like
hicido, pusido, and
muriedo. Table 10 presents the overall frequency of occurrence of the past participle variants produced by the children.
Table 10 demonstrates that the most frequently produced variants were overregularized variants like
hacido. In these tokens, children used the infinitival base of the verb and applied the regular -
do inflection for past participles. However, children also produced more innovative forms like
hicido, in which cases they applied the regular -
do inflection and a stem change. The use of these types of variants across children suggests that these children were aware that the participle forms had irregularities; however, they were not able to produce the adult-like irregular form and instead produced an innovative variant with an irregular stem and a regularized inflection. With
hicido and
pusido, it appears they applied the regular participle form -
do to the base used to form preterit verbs (
hice, hiciste, etc. ‘I did/made, you did/made’;
puse, pusiste, etc. ‘I put, you put’). Further, these results also demonstrate that children do not just overregularize past participles in one way, but rather, these forms can vary according to speakers as well as lexical items.
The high rates of regularized forms contrast with previous research on overregularization of Spanish verbal morphology. For example, for irregular present and past tense morphology,
Clahsen et al. (
2002) found rates of 14% and 1.5%, respectively. Meanwhile,
Baker Martínez (
forthcoming) examined Spanish children’s overregularization of Spanish past participles in corpus data and found 7% overregularization. Why were the overregularization rates so high in the current study, especially in comparison to the rates in Baker Martínez’s corpus study of Spanish children’s past participles discussed above? One possibility is related to differences in the populations studied; Baker Martínez included 47 Spanish children living in Spain, whereas the current study included U.S. born bilingual English–Spanish speaking children. Children in Spain are exposed to abundant instances of past participles due to the frequency of the present perfect tense, including hodiernal perfective uses (
Schwenter and Torres-Cacoullos 2008). Previous research has found that children in Spain produce present perfect verbs earlier than do children learning varieties of Spanish in which the past participles are less frequent. For example, the first tense/aspect contrast among Mexican monolingual children is the present simple/preterit (
Jackson-Maldonado 2012, p. 162), whereas it is the present simple/present perfect among monolingual children in Spain (
Mueller Gathercole et al. 1999, p. 145;
Grinstead 2000, p. 128). Given the frequency of past participles in Spanish in much of Spain, which affords children extensive practice with these forms, Spanish children may overregularize these forms less often and may retreat from overregularization earlier. In fact, in Baker Martínez’s study, the 45 monolingual children in Spain retreated from overregularization by 5 years of age.
Another difference between the population in Baker Martínez’s study and that in the current study is that the former included only two bilinguals who overregularized at overall rates of 12% and 16% and were living in Spain, where the dominant language is Spanish. In contrast, in the current study, the bilingual children were living in the U.S. in an environment where the dominant language is English. Clearly, these are vastly different bilingual language development scenarios. Such differences across communities are also evident in studies of 2sg preterit forms:
Baker (
2022) found lower overregularization rates among children in Spain (18%) as compared to
Lerner (
2016), whose study focused on bilingual Spanish–English children in Philadelphia; these children overregularized the 2sg preterit forms 66% of the time. As such, the current study’s high overregularization rates are comparable to
Lerner’s (
2016) 2sg overregularization rates among bilingual children, which demonstrates that rates of overregularization among bilingual children in the U.S. are extended in comparison to monolingual children living in Spanish-speaking countries. These findings bolster the conclusion that language experience and bilingualism play a role in overregularization, a point to which we will return in our discussion of research question 4 below.
Another possible explanation for the differences in rates across studies has to do with methodology.
Baker Martínez (
forthcoming) studied overregularization of past participles in naturalistic production data, whereas the current study employed an elicited production task. Other studies also suggest differences between experimental and corpus data. For example, children overregularize English past tense -
ed at higher rates in experimental studies than in natural language production (
Ambridge and Rowland 2013). The same is true for Spanish gender agreement; children produce mismatches in gender more often in experimental studies than in natural language production (e.g.,
Goebel-Mahrle and Shin 2020). There are various possible explanations for higher rates of overregularization or mismatching during experiments. Experiments may be more taxing and thus could slow down retrieval of an irregular form. Also, the words children rely on during natural conversations may be especially frequent words, which in general are less susceptible to overregularization. Furthermore, the use of the complex form of the auxiliary verb
haber used in the elicited production task (i.e.,
hubiera) could have also amplified overregularization rates, as the complexity of a morphological form can affect its likelihood of overregularization (
Bybee 1985;
Mueller Gathercole et al. 1999). In sum, the vastly different rates of overregularization across linguistic structures and across studies may be due to population differences, the methodology used, as well as the structures and lexical items targeted in each study.
Regarding the age at which children overregularize past participles, children in the current sample produced overregularized variants from the ages of 5;1 up to 11;4. Results from a mixed-effects regression analysis (
Table 8) found a main effect of age; the older the children were, the less likely they were to produce an overregularized form like
escribido. This age effect is most likely tied to increased experience with the Spanish language.
Frank et al. (
2021) found that the onset of overregularization in English, Norwegian, Danish, and Slovak correlates more tightly with vocabulary size than with age.
Kirjavainen et al. (
2012) found that Finnish children’s vocabulary size was a strong predictor of Finnish past-tense verbs. The current study suggests a similar conclusion for the retreat from overregularization and the production of adult-like irregular participles. As demonstrated in
Table 8, the effect size for the morphosyntax score was larger than the one for age, indicating that increased knowledge of Spanish grammar was a stronger predictor of adult-like irregular participle production as compared to age. In sum, while the current study found an age effect indicating that the older children overregularized less than younger children, it seems that retreat from overregularization is more closely tied to language experience and proficiency than to age.
4 We discuss the impact of these factors in more detail in the following sections.
5.2. Lexical Frequency
The third research question dealt with the impact of lexical frequency on children’s past participle overregularization. The study included low-frequency past participles
escrito,
roto,
muerto and high-frequency past participles
puesto,
dicho,
hecho (
Table 4). The children were more likely to overregularize the low-frequency past participles like
roto in contrast to the high-frequency ones like
hecho (
Table 8). More specifically, children overregularized low-frequency past participles at a rate of 83%, while they overregularized 65% of their high-frequency past participles (
Figure 2). Thus, while both low- and high-frequency past participles were overregularized often in the experimental data, the low-frequency forms were significantly more likely to undergo overregularization and be produced as variants like
rompido. These results suggest that the more often children are exposed to irregular past participles, the less likely these forms will be overregularized (
Bybee 2007).
5The frequency results align with previous research on overregularization in corpus studies, which finds that lexical frequency plays an important role in children’s overregularization of irregular verbal morphology in Spanish, German, English, and French (
Baker 2022;
Clahsen et al. 2002;
Clahsen and Rothweiler 1993;
Szagun 2011).
Baker’s (
2022) corpus study on Spanish children’s overregularization of the 2sg preterit forms (e.g.,
pintastes) found that children were more likely to overregularize low- and mid-frequency verbs (e.g.,
pintaste 24%,
pusiste 11%) than high-frequency 2sg forms (e.g.,
fuiste 6%). Regarding past participles,
Baker Martínez (
forthcoming) found that Spanish children overregularized low-frequency past participles like
abierto 27% of the time, while their high-frequency participles like
hecho were only overregularized 1% of the time.
While lexical frequency was a significant predictor of children’s overregularization, with low-frequency past participles increasing the likelihood of an overregularized form (e.g.,
escribido), it is worth noting that even the high-frequency past participles did undergo quite a bit of overregularization in the current study (65%); however, the interaction between lexical frequency and morphosyntax scores (
Figure 5) indicates that overregularization of frequent forms was found among children with lower morphosyntax scores. The higher the children’s grammatical proficiency, the less likely they were to overregularize the high-frequency past participles like
dicho. Furthermore, an additional interaction between lexical frequency and age showed that the children between the ages of 5 and 8 years were more likely to overregularize the high-frequency participles like
dicho, while these forms were less likely to be overregularized in the older children. Meanwhile, there was no age effect found for the low-frequency participles like
escrito. These findings further bolster the conclusion that children’s retreat from overregularization is tied to overall grammatical development and experience with the language.