Next Article in Journal
Can We Witness the (Re)making of a Pidgin in Real Time? Contact in the Russian–Chinese Border Area
Next Article in Special Issue
Language Control and Intra-Sentential Codeswitching among Bilingual Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Code-Switching by Spanish–English Bilingual Children in a Code-Switching Conversation Sample: Roles of Language Proficiency, Interlocutor Behavior, and Parent-Reported Code-Switching Experience
Previous Article in Special Issue
Regulation and Control: What Bimodal Bilingualism Reveals about Learning and Juggling Two Languages
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship?

1
Department of Linguistics and Multilingual Studies, Nanyang Technical University, Singapore 639798, Singapore
2
Department of Psychology, University at Albany, State University of New York, Albany, NY 12222, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Languages 2022, 7(4), 247; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247
Submission received: 15 May 2022 / Revised: 6 September 2022 / Accepted: 7 September 2022 / Published: 23 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Multilingualism: Consequences for the Brain and Mind)

Abstract

:
The relationship between executive functions (EF) and bilingualism has dominated debate in the field. This debate was characterised by optimism for a bilingual advantage until the last decade, when a steady stream of articles reported failure to find a consistently positive effect for bilingualism. In addition to addressing concerns about study quality, this turn of events has spurred research into other variables that may explain the conflicting findings. While recent studies have focused on sociodemographic variables and interactional contexts such as age, code-switching frequency, and socioeconomic class to account for various group and individual differences, the impact of culture is seldom scrutinised. This paper examines the possible effect of culture among bilingual studies on EF by first contextualising how bilingual EF are studied and outlining the absence of culture as a macro variable, followed by a discussion on how culture and language are often conflated. This paper directs attention to the small but emerging research that tracks the importance of culture as a separate variable from language. This review discusses why macro culture and individual monoculturalism or biculturalism need to be carefully elucidated as a factor that can interact with the bilingual experience in shaping EF.

1. Introduction

The last century has seen an acceleration in research interest on the impact of bilingualism on cognition. Depending on the period which we draw our sample of research from, we are likely to get a very different picture of this relationship. Multiple studies in the literature have charted the course of the withering (the 1920s), waxing (1960 onwards), and waning (post 2013) enthusiasm about the bilingual advantage (cf. Antoniou 2019; Lehtonen et al. 2018; Bialystok and Craik 2022 for a recent review on this topic). This review differs from earlier reviews examining the presence or absence of a bilingual advantage by focusing on the variable of culture in studies on bilingualism and executive functions (EF) in the last two decades. We evaluate the small but growing evidence of cultural effects in recent bilingualism studies and argue that there is compelling evidence for culture to be carefully scrutinised as a critical variable that relates to bilingual people’s EF.

2. The Bilingual Advantage in EF Debate

Ever since the seminal study by Peal and Lambert (1962), several studies comparing monolinguals and bilinguals seem to have yielded favourable findings regarding bilingual effects, with reports of cognitive advantages for bilinguals across their lifespans. Among the elderly, scholars argue that bilingualism may contribute to a “cognitive reserve” that may delay neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s and dementia by approximately four years compared with monolinguals (Bak et al. 2014; Bialystok et al. 2007; Calvo et al. 2016). Some researchers have proposed that bilinguals might have an advantage over their monolingual peers due to constant monitoring, selection, and inhibition of languages. In particular, they argue that bilinguals have an edge even in domain-general, non-linguistic tasks that tap into these cognitive processes (Green 1998; Green and Abutalebi 2013; Bialystok 2017).
While the exact definition and components of EF are still the subject of much debate (see Miyake and Friedman 2012 for a discussion), the consensus is that EF are higher-order cognitive functions that can be categorised into three core groups: inhibition and interference control (e.g., selective attention and behavioural and cognitive inhibition), cognitive flexibility (i.e., mental flexibility in task and set shifting), and working memory (Diamond 2013; Miyake et al. 2000). EF are believed to be the foundation upon which essential skills such as planning (Collins and Koechlin 2012) and critical reasoning (Lunt et al. 2012) are built. Moreover, they are also found to predict theory of mind in developing children (Sabbagh et al. 2006) and have significant implications for an individual’s personal, social, and academic development (Best et al. 2011).
Proponents of a bilingual advantage in domain-general EF argue that the advantage stems from a bilingual’s need to direct attention in monitoring speech input and selecting the appropriate language to respond while inhibiting the production of words and phrases in the other language(s). Some studies have suggested that all languages in a bilingual’s repertoire are jointly activated (Costa et al. 1999; Marian and Spivey 2003; Thierry and Wu 2007) during listening (Spivey and Marian 1999), reading (Dijkstra and Kroll 2005), and speech (Kroll et al. 2006). The act of “juggling” multiple activated schemas is said to hone domain-general interference and inhibition control (Kroll et al. 2015).
In the recent literature, we can track two dominant views explaining the roots of the bilingual advantage. The first model, put forth by Green (1998), has sometimes been termed the “Inhibition Control Model” proposes that bilinguals’ experience in inhibiting the co-activated non-relevant language is present even in monolingual speech contexts. This experience enhances their ability to inhibit distracting or irrelevant stimuli better than monolinguals. Some key studies supporting this view include those of Bialystok et al. (2004) and Kroll et al. (2008). Expanding on this model, Green and Abutalebi’s (2013) adaptive control hypothesis argues that different control processes, including goal maintenance, conflict monitoring, interference suppression, cue detection, response inhibition, task (dis)engagement, and opportunistic planning, are activated depending on the demands of different interactional contexts (single language, dual language, and dense code-switching). Emerging evidence suggests depending on the level of cognitive control, these different contexts could result in varying enhancements of cognitive control (Beatty-Martínez et al. 2020; Ooi et al. 2018). Other researchers have suggested that code-switching between languages intersententially and intrasententially might share similarities with domain-general task switching (Prior and MacWhinney 2009; Hartanto and Yang 2016). Supporting evidence for this hypothesis comes from brain imaging studies which showed an overlap in neural connections and architecture for language switching and domain-general non-linguistic switching tasks (de Baene et al. 2015; Weissberger et al. 2019). Proponents of this model theorise that these “cognitive exercises” augment domain-general EF and confer a bilingual advantage that is evident in children as young as 7 months old (Kovács and Mehler 2009).
The second model takes the view that inhibition and control alone are insufficient to explain the enhancement of bilinguals’ cognitive abilities. Bialystok et al. (2010; see also Bialystok 2015, 2017) discussed the possibility that the bilingual advantage in EF may stem from how bilinguals monitor, control, and direct attention and that bilingual exposure may alter how individuals manage these resources (cf. Bialystok et al. 2012; Colzato et al. 2008; Costa et al. 2009). This concept, put forth as attentional control, is thought of as being broader than any single component in Miyake’s (Miyake et al. 2000) three-component model of EF (inhibition and interference control, shifting, and working memory). It involves the ability to monitor, suppress, or ignore irrelevant stimuli and direct cognitive resources to either maintain or switch depending on the relevant information (Bialystok 2017). Indeed, Bialystok and Craik (2022) suggested that bilinguals’ ability to direct attention has a far-reaching prowess which is enough to “enhance processes of both facilitation and inhibition, as well as processes underlying cognitive flexibility and resource allocation”.
In the ensuing years since the early 2000s, numerous studies across the field have reported on how bilinguals excel in tasks requiring participants to pay attention to relevant information while suppressing distracting changes to rules or irrelevant cues. These advantages reportedly extend to adults, as evidenced by their greater accuracy and faster resolving of incongruent stimuli in the Simon (Antoniou et al. 2016; Bialystok et al. 2004; Tse and Altarriba 2014), Flanker (Costa et al. 2009), and Stroop tasks (Nayak et al. 2020; Poulin-Dubois et al. 2011). They were also observed among the elderly (above 60 years of age), who showed lower processing costs, suggesting greater efficiency even after considering the typical age-related decrease in performance. This led some scholars to suggest that lifelong bilingualism may mitigate specific age-related declines in cognitive performance when concerning inhibition and control (Bialystok 2021).
Bialystok (2011) argues that bilinguals’ ability to switch between languages or language varieties fluidly may enhance their cognitive flexibility, especially in switching and shifting tasks. A seminal study by Prior and MacWhinney (2009), which showed how proficient bilingual college students outperformed monolinguals in reaction time during switch trials but not in non-switch trials, points to a bilingual advantage in cognitive shifting. Similar studies, such as that by Wiseheart et al. (2014), have also reported how bilinguals were better able to resolve ambiguity in stimulus–response associations, resulting in lower global switching costs. Bilinguals are also thought to hone cognitive control as a response to their sociolinguistic environment, with varying levels and types of code switching (e.g., intersentential vs. intrasentential), resulting in more efficient cognitive processing faculties shown, for example, in reduced switching costs (Yang et al. 2016).
Up till 2010, the evidence for a bilingual advantage in the literature is robust. However, in more recent years, the bilingual superiority effect in EF has not been unanimously reported. In the last decade, many studies have either failed to replicate findings reporting a bilingual advantage or did not come to that conclusion when comparing bilingual and monolingual populations.

3. Conflicting Findings in EF Performance

3.1. Methodological Concerns

In recent years, challenges to the firm conclusion of a bilingual advantage have been highlighted from various perspectives and by different research groups (Hernández et al. 2013; Paap and Greenberg 2013; Paap and Sawi 2014; Paap et al. 2015; Antón et al. 2014; de Bruin et al. 2015; Von Bastian et al. 2016). A vital issue of concern that has been widely discussed is publication bias, where journal publications favour the publishing of significant effects in support of a bilingual advantage (de Bruin et al. 2015). Lehtonen et al.’s (2018) meta-analytic review is, by far, the most comprehensive attempt to synthesise existing studies relating to bilingualism and executive functions in adults. The review involved 152 studies covering a range of 6 executive function domains (inhibitory control, monitoring, shifting, working memory, attention, and verbal fluency). The authors included unpublished doctoral and masters’ theses in addition to journal articles while accounting for the various effect sizes of each paper to mitigate the effects of publication bias. They reported that no apparent advantage in any of the six executive function domains provided evidence that bilingual adults were at an advantage compared with their monolingual peers after correcting for publication bias.
Several studies have also been unable to replicate the findings of the original bilingual advantage found in seminal studies for the Stroop (Paap et al. 2018, 2019), Simon (Gathercole et al. 2014), and Flanker tasks (Paap et al. 2019) and switching ability (Goriot et al. 2018), even after matching monolingual and bilingual participants to a number of linguistic and sociodemographic variables. Paap’s (2019) comparison of mean reaction time differences in interference-control tasks across 177 studies for “benchmark” tests of interference-control, such as the Simon, Stroop, Flanker and Attention Network Tasks (ANT) also found that more than 80% of studies returned null results. Indeed, such studies raise important questions regarding both study quality and replicability. Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis by Lowe et al. (2021) synthesised data from a profusion of published studies and unpublished data sets to examine the effect of language status (bilingual vs. monolingual) on EF among children. After comparing more than a thousand effect sizes in studies with monolingual and bilingual participants between 3 and 17 years, the researchers detected a small overall effect of bilingual language status on children’s EF that was completely attenuated when corrected for publication bias.
Another extensive experimental study with more than 4500 participants by Dick et al. (2019) also failed to find evidence demonstrating better performance among bilinguals in the Flanker, Dimension Change Card Sort, and Stop-Signal Reaction Time tests, which measure common EF indicators including inhibition, attention, and set-shifting abilities among 9–10-year-old children in the United States. Bilingual participants (n = 1740) were grouped based on three different definitions of bilingualism: bilingual status (if children identified as speaking any language other than English), bilingual degree (ratio of non-English language use to English use), and bilingual use (continuous measure of non-English language frequency with different interlocutors). After controlling for demographic covariates such as socioeconomic status, age, education level, and intelligence, the regression analyses revealed a bilingual disadvantage in terms of poorer English vocabulary, consistent with other studies in the literature (e.g., Martin-Rhee and Bialystok 2008). While the researchers reported a bilingual disadvantage in the Stop-Signal Reaction Time test, no other significant differences were found between the monolingual and bilingual participants. Thus, despite the report of a bilingual advantage in the early 2000s, much of the current discussion is hotly contesting the validity and replicability of such claims.

3.2. Hidden Confounds

Other than the methodological issues raised, some researchers argue that differences in experimental designs and confounds in participant selection have led to discrepancies in findings. Variables such as ethnicity, social background, and social-economic status (Bak 2016; Blom et al. 2017; Morton and Harper 2007), different definitions of early or late bilingualism (Yang et al. 2016), a participant’s cultural upbringing (Tran et al. 2015), or immigrant status (Kousaie and Phillips 2012) have not always been well controlled or considered in many studies, showing a bilingual advantage in EF. Interestingly, much of the discussion is very similar to the discussion raised in Peal and Lambert’s (1962) watershed report on the effect of bilingualism on cognition. Researchers such as de Bruin (2019) have called for more nuanced studies that consider the complex social variables that influence bilinguals as part of different groups and communities. In the following section, we examine some possible reasons for the contradictory findings and put forth a case for why culture as a variable should be considered in earnest by researchers in the field.
The lack of a bilingual advantage in the abovementioned studies signals the need for closer scrutiny of the variables that may contribute to bilinguals’ seemingly variable performance. As Luk and Bialystok (2013) pointed out, the cognitive consequences of bilingualism need to be considered within the broader, multi-dimensional bilingual experience. This view is further echoed by Valian (2014) and de Bruin (2019), who argue that diverse differences between bilinguals’ environmental contexts, social interaction habits, and various sociolinguistic variables may influence the findings for EF.
First, the age group of the participants may play a role in detecting a significant bilingual advantage. Some articles suggest that the bilingual advantage seems to be most pronounced among young children and the elderly (Bak et al. 2014; Luo et al. 2010), while the differences between monolingual and bilingual young adults may sometimes show insignificant or no group differences, possibly because young adults are at the zenith of their cognitive functioning (Bialystok et al. 2004). Hence, bilingual effects may not be as pronounced in young adults compared with the elderly (for a counter perspective, see Samuel et al. 2018), as ageing has been found to take a toll on cognitive and executive function processing, and differences in performance among older bilinguals could be attributed to the cognitive reserve built up from lifelong bilingualism (Abutalebi et al. 2015; Antoniou and Wright 2017; Nickels et al. 2019).
Another possible source of variation involves the definition and classification of bilinguals based on their age of acquisition and fluency. Researchers in the field do not all agree on how “early” or “late” bilingualism may be unambiguously classified due to disagreement on the exact age range of the critical period for language acquisition (Byers-Heinlein and Lew-Williams 2013). Consequently, these different yardsticks can result in different conclusions. For example, Martin et al. (2013) suggested that early bilinguals (AoA: between 2 and 4 years) had the edge over late bilinguals. In contrast, Pelham and Abrams (2014) reported that while bilingualism had a facilitative effect on conflict resolution in incongruent trials over the monolinguals in their study, no differences in executive control were found between early (AoA: 7 years or younger) and late bilinguals (AoA: age 13 and above). When adopting the age of acquisition as a continuous variable in a mixed model regression, Von Bastian et al. (2016) could not detect a bilingual advantage in EF between participants for a broad range of tasks measuring inhibitory control, shifting, conflict monitoring, and general cognitive abilities. Indeed, such methodological differences can lead to dissonant findings and be an important source of variation in whether a difference in EF tests is found.
Additionally, concerns have also been raised regarding self-rated language proficiency when it is used to compare groups from different cultural or geographical backgrounds. Tomoschuk et al. (2018) found that self-rated proficiencies and picture-naming test scores were not consistently correlated among different bilingual populations, with Chinese-English bilinguals performing better in the picture-naming task compared with Spanish-English bilinguals who had identical self-rated proficiency scores. Notably, even grouping participants by language dominance (other language vs. English) did not remove these discrepancies. These findings indicate potential confounds, as the subjective, self-rated scores in one population may be conceptualised differently than another group of participants due to different social environments and upbringings, ages of acquisition, and language dominance, even if they are taken from the same community (e.g., college students).
In fact, there is evidence that it is not only language proficiency but how balanced the bilingual is in terms of proficiency and use in both languages that can impact performance in tests of EF. A seminal study by Tse and Altarriba (2012) involved administering a Stroop task on 110 bilingual adults to determine the effects of language proficiency on their attentional control system. The researchers reported that responses to the Stroop task were positively correlated to second-language proficiency and that bilinguals who were more balanced in both language proficiencies were more efficient at conflict resolution in incongruent trials. In particular, they argued that a bilingual’s language proficiencies honed aspects of attentional control (conflict resolution and goal maintenance) that explained the different performance in the Stroop task. Similarly, Yow and Li (2015) found that bilinguals who were both proficient and balanced in their use of both languages outperformed those who were less balanced in terms of use and proficiency in tasks of inhibition and set-shifting.
These studies, we think, provide compelling arguments to consider the issue of balance when assessing bilingual proficiency. While most studies offer some measure of proficiency, problems with validity and comparability can still arise as a result of methodological differences that can potentially mask the effects of a bilingual advantage in EF.

4. Culture: An Often-Overlooked Factor

Another factor that has seldom been discussed is the effect of culture as a potential confounding variable. Culture is undoubtedly a complex construct that can be defined at many levels. It must encompass the depth and breadth of interactions, behaviours, emotions, mindsets, and ways of being, both tangible and intangible. For the purpose of this article, culture will be taken to refer to the learned and shared system of beliefs, values, preferences, and social norms that are spread by shared activities (Altarriba and Basnight-Brown 2022; Arshad and Chung 2022; Bezin and Moizeau 2017).
Over the years, various operationalisations have been proposed to allow researchers to distinguish between cultures. One highly influential model frequently used to delineate the differences between cultures in cross-cultural psychology is Hofstede’s cultural dimensions (Hofstede 1980, 2001, 2011). These cultural dimensions—power distance, individualism vs. collectivism, uncertainty avoidance, masculinity vs. femineity, long-term orientation vs. short-term orientation, and indulgence vs. restraint—allow researchers to make broad-brush generalisations of the cultural traits typical to the residents of different countries and geographical regions (see Varnum et al. 2010 for a review). While this model uses nationality as a proxy for culture and is subject to the ecological fallacy of overgeneralisation on the individual level, scholars have used it extensively over the years to generalise traits for comparisons between participant samples. We felt it necessary to raise this issue at the outset, as nearly all instances of cultural comparisons in the literature used nationality as a proxy, and the majority made reference to Eastern (East Asian) compared with Western culture (Table 1).
Zooming in on research at the intersection of bilingualism, cultural differences, and EF, only a dozen or so studies in the past decade have explicitly investigated the impact of culture on bilingualism and EF (see Appendix A for an overview of the key studies). Some of the earliest studies of this nature were a response to Morton and Harper’s (2007) argument for the possibility of one’s ethnic (and cultural) background accounting for the bilingual advantage previously found in early childhood bilinguals. In the years that followed, several studies attempted to control for the effect of culture on EF by intentionally selecting samples with diverse language and cultural backgrounds based on nationality (e.g., Bialystok and Viswanathan 2009). Others sought to isolate bilingual effects by maintaining cultural homogeneity through sampling bilinguals and monolinguals with similar cultural backgrounds (Yang and Yang 2016). In the following section, we review select articles in the existing literature that have included cultural comparisons in studies of language status (mono- or bilingualism) and EF.

4.1. Studies in Young Children

As indicated earlier, over the years, multiple studies have emerged where bilinguals were found to have a global advantage in tasks of executive control (Bialystok 1999; Bialystok and Viswanathan 2009; Bialystok et al. 2010; Barac and Bialystok 2012; Tran et al. 2015, 2018; Yang et al. 2011; Yang and Yang 2016). Nevertheless, an effect of culture was found despite it not overriding the effect of bilingualism in many of these articles (see Appendix A for a breakdown of the bilingual or cultural effects reported among key studies). For example, while Yang et al.’s study in 2011 reported an overall bilingual advantage in terms of accuracy and reaction time, as well as in conflict resolution for the Attention Network Task (ANT), they also found that the overall accuracy of Korean monolinguals from Korea was higher than that of Korean or English monolinguals from the USA. Interestingly, both Korean and English monolinguals from the USA performed similarly. This suggests that an effect of culture, unrelated to the languages spoken, could be evident even in 4-year-old children.
Similarly, Tran et al. (2015) also found a similar effect in East Asian children who outperformed Western and Latin American children in terms of reaction time and accuracy, despite the bilingual advantage over monolinguals still prevailing overall. Another longitudinal study of Vietnamese, Argentinian, and American children by Tran et al. (2018) uncovered a global, bilingual cognitive advantage in their longitudinal study of 96 3-year-old children as well as a cultural effect where Eastern (Vietnamese) children outperformed Western (USA) and Latin American (Argentinian) children on the day/night task, which measures verbal response inhibition.
Some studies also suggest that cultural effects may modulate advantages in EF over bilingualism. Recent research seeking to disentangle the effects of language and culture among preschoolers matched participants on a measure of “country of origin” as a proxy for culture. Cho et al. (2021) tested Korean monolinguals (Korea), Korean-English bilinguals (Canada), and English monolinguals (Canada) on a modified colour and word Stroop task as a measure of inhibition control. The researchers found that while Korean-English bilinguals outperformed English monolingual children in terms of accuracy in incongruent trials, Korean-English bilinguals performed no differently than Korean monolinguals after controlling for age and SES. Critically, they found that the country of origin was the key modulating variable predicting accuracy in incongruent trials after controlling for demographic variables and performance in congruent trials.
Taken together, it seems plausible that cultural upbringing could play an important role in early EF development. In particular, cultural differences seem to be most evident in executive control tasks, possibly interacting with bilingual experience in shaping EF. One conjecture that could explain the inconsistencies reported could stem from differences in the cultural expectations of children regarding obedience and following directions. These expectations could be ingrained in children in different cultural contexts at a much earlier age. For example, children in East Asian cultures are typically expected to follow the rules more closely and practice response inhibition from a younger age than children from America (Kelkar et al. 2013; Lan et al. 2011). These could provide possible explanations for why the country of origin (as a loose proxy for “culture”) could help to explain the variability reported in the literature. Nevertheless, with the current dearth in this vein of research, the exact nature of the interaction between culture and language with EF among young children remains an open question. Further research is needed to elucidate what conditions might reveal cultural effects (i.e., beyond “East vs. West” distinctions, integrating other measures of culture such as long-term orientation vs. short-term orientation instead of only individualism vs. collectivism) that could have knock-on effects on EF components such as attention, especially since most existing studies have only examined the effect of culture and language on inhibition control.

4.2. Studies in Adults

Contrary to studies conducted with children, examining the effect of cultural differences in the EF of bilingual adults seems to paint a different but clearer picture. In a study disambiguating the effect of cultural background and language, Samuel et al. (2018) tested inhibition control using a Simon task on 211 adult participants from three cultural backgrounds: British, Korean, and mixed nationalities (drawn from 33 countries). Bilingualism was taken as a continuous measure of three factors: L2 proficiency, language dominance, and code-switching frequency. Analysis using linear mixed-effects regression revealed that Koreans outperformed the British group in every measure (RT, accuracy, and smaller Simon effect) and in every model while performing faster overall than the mixed group in two out of three models. The mixed nationality group also outpaced the British participants in nearly all measures across every model. This provides critical evidence that even macro-level cultural effects can possibly account for the different levels of performance on common tasks of inhibition and control, especially among adult participants.
In one of the few studies that explicitly separated multicultural identity and its effect on language and executive function, Treffers-Daller et al. (2020) found a bilingual advantage in a reduced Flanker conflict effect when comparing bilinguals and monolinguals. Notably, they reported that multicultural identity styles (Ward et al. 2018) were the key explanatory variable in explaining EF variance among bilingual subjects at an individual level in their model. Similarly, Xie and Ng (in preparation) also found a significant effect in resolving conflict in a Flanker task among high-proficiency bicultural bilinguals who differ in their frequency of cultural switching in their daily lives.
Preliminary evidence also suggests that activating different cultural frames could be associated with different performance in inhibition control tasks. Ye et al. (2017) reported a bilingual advantage in incongruent trials for a Flanker task in mixed cultural contexts. High-proficiency Mandarin-English bilinguals outperformed participants with high proficiency in Mandarin but low English proficiency when filler slides showed both Western (British and American) and Eastern (Chinese) cultural icons. Interestingly, the bilingual advantage was not replicated in single cultural contexts (e.g., fillers with only Eastern cultural icons) or in congruent trials. The authors speculated that the tasks may not be challenging enough to elucidate an advantage in conflict resolution. Further analyses also showed that bicultural contexts attenuate proficient bilinguals’ cognitive performance significantly when examining the results of both the mixed cultural and single cultural conditions for proficient bilinguals. Indeed, findings such as these beg the question as to whether existing inconsistencies reported in the literature about bilinguals’ (dis)advantage in EF could be explained by differences in individual participants’ cultural milieus, or if it is their cultural switching habits that have shrouded the “true” performance in tasks relating to EF.
In summary, while many of these studies on young children yielded mixed results, when the critical studies were examined (Appendix A), only four reported a complete absence of cultural effects. Indeed, Bialystok and Craik (2022) acknowledged that culture could be a possible confound to the bilingual advantage in tests on EF, although they highlighted several studies that have shown language effects overriding cultural effects. Yet, there seems to be a growing body of research suggesting that the effect of culture may be more pronounced when examining adult populations. We hypothesise that cultural effects might be more evident in adulthood due to the prolonged experience of honing their cultural selves and may also result from their exposure to multiple cultures and the acquisition of bicultural or multicultural identities. Indeed, this growing body of research suggests that culture as a variable of interest should be given a second look (e.g., Altarriba 2008).

5. Is There a Need to Disambiguate Cultural Effects from Language Effects in Studies on Executive Functions?

It has been standard practice that individuals recruited for bilingualism studies are matched by the languages they profess to know. In fact, it is not uncommon to include individuals from different language backgrounds, nationalities, races, and ethnicities so long as they broadly speak the same language, broadly because most studies typically report large language “families” instead of specific language varieties or dialects (e.g., English speakers may refer to speakers of any English dialect (American English, British English, Singapore English, Indian English, etc.)). However, this does not capture a sufficiently nuanced perspective of the cultural diversity among bilingual populations.
In recent years, scholars such as Grosjean (2015) have argued that bilingualism and biculturalism are often conflated in research on bilingualism. This is not a new idea, as Soffietti (1960) made a case for the reality that culture and language status are distinct, where bilinguals could be monocultural or bicultural and monolinguals could be monocultural or bicultural. Indeed, the reality is somewhat complex as just as there is a whole spectrum of bilinguals from dominant bilinguals to balanced bilinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, receptive bilinguals, etc., an entire range of bi- or even monocultural bilinguals exists. Grosjean (1992) argues that some Europeans may be multilingual, having studied two or more languages in a school setting. However, they are monocultural as they only work and stay in a country and a single cultural setting. On the other hand, individuals can be monolingual but multicultural by immersing themselves in different cultures across their social contacts but choosing not to learn their languages (Schwartz et al. 2017; Shih and Sanchez 2005).
While Grosjean (2015) limited his discussion of biculturalism and bilingualism to identity and personality, we believe it is critical to consider the implications of culture and biculturalism on cognition. Up until now, research has suggested that cross-cultural differences influence the development of EF even among preschool children as early as the age of three, where cultural variation in the development of EF is seen in various tasks of inhibition and cognitive flexibility (Legare et al. 2018; Ling et al. 2018; Norenzayan et al. 2002; Imada et al. 2012). To fully appreciate the distinction between bicultural and monocultural bilinguals, we first need to account for culture as a variable associated with cognition and particularly EF. We will first unpack the relevance of cross-cultural differences in shaping EF and then situate these differences in bicultural individuals.

5.1. Macro-Scale Cultural Differences and EF

An area of cross-cultural comparison that has received much attention over the last two decades pertains to the so-called “East-West difference”. Individuals from East Asian countries who are of East Asian descent or have been exposed to East Asian culture and values from a young age are commonly considered to embody Eastern cultural values. They are viewed as being more collectivist, having higher power distances in relationships with authority, being more likely to avoid uncertainty, and having a stronger sense of interdependence (Markus and Kitayama 1991, 2010; Oyserman and Lee 2008) compared with Americans who epitomise Western culture in their desire for individual autonomy, egalitarian relationships, and willingness to take risks (Triandis 1989, 2001; Harkness et al. 2000). The differences in Eastern and Western cultures have been well documented and shown to influence parents’ thoughts on how to raise their children (Chen et al. 1998; Bornstein 2013), socialisation and personality (Varnum et al. 2010), self-evaluation (Kim et al. 2009), individual’s ethics and moral values (Garcia et al. 2014; Jia and Krettenauer 2017), as well as self-regulation (Imada et al. 2012; Jaramillo et al. 2017; Krassner et al. 2016) and emotion (Kitayama et al. 2006; Masuda et al. 2008; Pavlenko 2005). Clearly, these distinctions may characterise many individuals. However, it is important to maintain that there is a broad range of these variables throughout any given group or culture; that is, they always reside as a distribution within and among these populations.
These very cultural differences have been theorised to influence cognition. Cultural value systems such as individualism vs. collectivism, tolerance towards uncertainty, sociolinguistic factors about when formal schooling should begin, as well as culture-specific parenting attitudes related to child autonomy, expected discipline, level of parental control, and emotional socialisation are critical determinants in shaping EF (Sarma and Thomas 2020).
Over the years, various studies have sought to distinguish the effects of culture on EF. In a seminal study, Masuda and Nisbett (2001) reported differences in memory recall for East Asians compared with Americans due to different cultural upbringings. The researchers reported that East Asian adults could remember more background information in the visual stimuli and became less accurate when background cues were changed compared with their American counterparts. The authors hypothesised that these differences could result from different modes of attentional direction and focus emphasised by Eastern and Western cultures. Another study investigating how different cultural backgrounds would influence cognition by Sabbagh et al. (2006) examined the interaction between EF and theory of mind in 5-year-old children from America and China. The researchers found that the Chinese children outperformed the American children in all EF tests, such as the Dimensional Change Card Sort and Stroop tasks. In the same vein, Oh and Lewis (2008) found that Korean preschoolers performed significantly better at inhibition and task switching than British children of a similar age, while no differences were found in the measures of working memory. More recently, Imada et al. (2012) presented further evidence relating differences in EF to cultural differences. They compared 175 children from America and Japan and found that sensitivity to contextual cues was highly correlated with performance in EF tasks. Specifically, the children from Japan outperformed American children in the set-shifting Dimension Change Card Sort Test but were also more likely to peek on impulse during a delayed gratification task, indicating poorer impulse control.
One influential theory that has emerged at the forefront to explain the cultural effect on EF proposes that differences between cultures could stem from the disparity in cultural values and upbringing (including family and societal education strategies) that influence what individuals from contrasting cultures focus on (Nisbett and Masuda 2003, 2006). According to this theory, the subdivision between Eastern and Western cultures in cross-cultural studies has been correlated with different types of attentional focus and cognitive processing. Individuals brought up with Eastern, collectivistic cultural influences tend to have attentional processing tuned to focus on contextual cues (e.g., non-verbal cues, more holistic, and distributed attention), while Western cultures direct attention to process-specific, individualised information (Nisbett et al. 2001; Nisbett and Masuda 2003). In a series of four studies examining how culture mediates statistical learning, Kiyokawa et al. (2012) found differences in local and global perceptual biases in their British and Japanese participants, respectively, even after manipulating participants’ familiarity with the sequence elements (strings of large (global) letters made out of small (local) letters). Their results provide evidence that cultural differences can influence the type of unconscious knowledge being learned (see also Kiyokawa et al. 2010 and Ling et al. 2018 for additional studies). Thus, scholars have hypothesised that with the emphasis on greater “general sensitivity”, Eastern cultures may have an advantage in recognising and reacting to stimuli that are displayed when reaction time is measured (Nisbett and Miyamoto 2005; Miyamoto et al. 2006; Kuwabara and Smith 2012).
Consequently, the evidence above suggests that even broad-brush cultural differences in nationality or ethnicity are associated with different performances in EF tasks in cross-cultural comparisons. Extending this to our discussion on bilingualism and EF, this indicates that comparisons of participants based on language status (bilingual or monolingual) may not be directly comparable around the world, even if factors such as socioeconomic status and other sociolinguistic variables are controlled for. It is perhaps even more important to distinguish between society-level culture and the cultural identities and values an individual adopts. This will be discussed in the following section on micro-level cultural factors.

5.2. Micro-Level Cultural Factors: Individual Acculturation and Bilingualism

Individual biculturalism describes an individual’s exposure and internalisation of two or more cultures as part of their identity (Nguyen and Benet-Martínez 2007, 2012). According to LaFromboise et al. (1993), multicultural individuals typically possess a certain degree of multicultural competence, be it in terms of explicit cultural knowledge, a tacit understanding of cultural scripts (culturally appropriate etiquette) and values, or friends and family within the other culture, and are often able to communicate in languages associated with the different cultures. In this paper, we define bicultural individuals more narrowly, as individuals who possess more than one cultural identity in their repertoire and can shift between cultural mindsets (frames) in their repertoire, choosing the appropriate actions, values, and norms when they interact with individuals and groups from other cultures (Benet-Martínez et al. 2002; Hong et al. 2000).
Early studies on biculturalism often focused on migrants’ and immigrants’ acculturation processes (Berry 1997). These studies have generated significant interest among cultural psychologists as they delineate how acculturation to a new culture can occur. One of the most influential models, Berry’s Acculturation Model (Berry 1997, 2003), investigates how individuals acculturate to the host society. Specifically, the model proposes that individuals acculturate through two main processes: cultural maintenance and contact and participation. Individuals typically adopt one of four distinct acculturation strategies that result in either cultural assimilation, integration, separation, or marginalisation (Table 2). Of relevance to our discussion is how this model delineates various acculturation outcomes and shows a distribution of outcomes ranging from monocultural to bicultural (e.g., Schwartz et al. 2017; Meca et al. 2017). This has an important bearing on a bilingual’s profile, as those who are more culturally integrated (i.e., more balanced) are likely to consider themselves more bicultural compared with bilinguals who do not identify as integrating (separating, assimilating, or marginalising), thus considering themselves more monocultural.
Research by social psychologists has drawn a theoretical link between how acculturating to different cultures can hone an individual’s cognitive abilities. The acculturation complexity model by Tadmor and Tetlock (2006) proposes that the level of immersion in new cultures (indexed by the willingness to acculturate) can hone an individual’s cognitive skills differently. Specifically, cognitive abilities such as selective attention and inhibition can be developed, resulting from the pressures of resolving diverse cultural complexities and the necessity of being able to behave “appropriately” among interactants of different cultures. These differences in an individual’s cultural preferences are thought to influence such cognitive gains, as individuals who prefer to use only one of the two cultures more frequently are less likely to experience and have fewer opportunities to resolve conflicts arising from cultural differences compared with someone with an equal preference for both cultures.
Over the years, research has suggested that bicultural people who integrate both cultures are able to provide more complex descriptions of each culture compared with monocultural people or bicultural people with a distinct preference for one culture (Benet-Martínez et al. 2006) and that a bicultural person’s acculturation strategies impact cognition through instances of conflict mitigation and behavioural inhibition (Crisp and Turner 2011). Recently, Spiegler and Leyendecker (2017) showed how Turkish-German immigrant children with a balanced view of both cultures outperformed their peers, who favoured one culture over the other in terms of cognitive flexibility. Indeed, converging evidence suggests that individuals who identify as bicultural frequently integrate different sets of cultural knowledge, or as Cheng et al. (2014) described it, “bicultural individuals possess ‘two cultural minds’—two sets of cultural knowledge, use two cultural schemas to guide their thoughts and behaviour, and can activate these two cultural frames of references” (p. 279).
Research building on this hypothesis posits a theory of cultural frame switching relevant to bilingualism studies and EF. Cultural frame switching is a term coined from the observation that individuals with multiple cultural identities are able to switch across cultural frameworks (or their various cultural minds) depending on the cultural cues being presented. In seminal studies by Hong et al. (2000) examining this phenomenon, the researchers found that bicultural individuals behaved differently depending on the cultural primes used. For instance, when bicultural individuals (Chinese Americans) were primed with icons representing the Chinese or American cultures (e.g., a dragon as an icon of Chinese culture or the American flag as an icon of American culture), the participants’ appraisal of an ambiguous situation tended to embody specific cultural values of the culture associated with the prime. In these studies, the participants were asked if they thought an animated video of a fish swimming in front of a school of fish was being chased by the other fish (an external push factor) or if it was leading other fish (an internal factor). When primed with Chinese cultural primes, bicultural participants were comparatively more confident that the fish was being chased (an external attribution). When primed with American primes, they were more confident that the fish was leading the school (internal attribution).
The researchers argued that bicultural individuals could tap into different systems and schemas of cultural meaning and switch between them depending on the environment and context. Building on this, further research supported bicultural individuals’ ability to switch unconsciously and seamlessly (Benet-Martínez et al. 2002) and even change identities based on cultural cues (Luna et al. 2008). Cultural frame switching often occurs when there is a significant difference in the interactant’s environment, such as when moving between the public and private spheres. For instance, Suárez-Orozco et al. (2008) brought attention to the situation of immigrants in Boston and San Francisco in their longitudinal study of 470 immigrant children from countries such as Mexico, Central America, and China. Using parental interviews, test scores, and case studies, the authors exemplify the different ways in which children cope with the disparity between their heritage culture and the broader American culture and how intentional switching between cultural frames may be observed when moving between these different domains.
Extending the theory of cultural frame switching to bilingualism, Ramírez-Esparza et al. (2006) reported that Spanish-English bilinguals presented different personality traits depending on the language in which they answered self-reporting personality questionnaires. In particular, they showed increased extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness when answering in English compared with Spanish. Another study by Chen and Bond (2010) provided further evidence for bilingual personality switching in examining how the interviewer’s ethnicity can influence the way interlocutors present themselves. Bilingual interviewers (two Caucasian and two Chinese) interviewed 76 Chinese-English bilinguals, and observers were asked to rate participants’ personalities by the traits of extraversion and openness. Each participant was interviewed by a Chinese and a Caucasian interviewer, both of whom used English and Chinese separately. The researchers found that the participants displayed increased extraversion and were more willing to speak about experiences with Caucasian interviewers compared with Chinese interviewers, regardless of the language mode used for the interviews. This highlights how bilingual and bicultural people have the resources to switch between cultural frames implicitly, often without conscious effort, depending on the appraisal of the social speech context and cultural environment.
In summary, these studies on bilingualism and culture support the idea that bicultural individuals have different cultural systems that are selected and inhibited fluidly and automatically, depending on the situational context. In addition, bicultural individuals may need to direct cognitive resources to monitor the situational context and choose the appropriate cultural values, attitudes, and ideologies which are relevant while inhibiting inappropriate behaviour when switching between cultures. These processes seem to mirror how a bilingual person’s language system is hypothesised to function in terms of language inhibition and control and in terms of directing attention for monitoring and language selection. If this is indeed the case, the cultural frame switching that bicultural bilingual people participate in could influence the development of executive functions in a similar fashion to how bilingualism hones the executive functions system.
This is a crucial issue to resolve as to date, bilingual research has frequently conflated bilingualism and biculturalism (Grosjean 2015). Moreover, the reality is somewhat complex. Just as there is a whole spectrum of bilinguals from dominant bilinguals to balanced bilinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, receptive bilinguals, etc., an entire spectrum of bi- or even monocultural bilinguals exists. According to Grosjean (1992), some Europeans may be multilingual, having studied two or more languages in a school setting. However, many are predominantly monocultural, as they only work and stay in one country and a single cultural setting. On the other hand, individuals can be monolingual but multicultural by immersing themselves in other cultures in their social contacts but choosing not to learn their languages (Padilla 2006). This nuanced view that forms a bilingual-bicultural separation is an aspect that was absent in nearly all of the literature we reviewed.

6. Future Directions

Evidence within the literature suggests that a comparison of cultural effects, be it generalisations based on nationality or a more individual scale (individual mono- or biculturalism), is associated with differences in EF. Among scholars interested in the intersection of languages and EF, it is essential that we consider how cultural variables can be a mediating factor when describing bilinguals’ EF.
Here, we would like to propose a few directions that the field could take to examine the effect of culture on bilinguals’ EF in greater detail. Most existing studies incorporating an aspect of culture within experiments looking at bilinguals’ EF have typically used a general “East vs. West” distinction for contrast. Future studies should move beyond the assumption that an individual’s culture is based on their nationality or that they belong to a particular culture simply due to being born and raised in his or her country of origin. With most existing studies matching participants based on geography or where they currently reside as an earmark for culture, we may be making assumptions about macro-level culture based on citizenship, nationality, and ethnicity, all of which may not necessarily reflect the individuals’ cultural affiliations or identities.
Similarly, research distinguishing the effects of bilingualism on different populations should elucidate bilinguals’ cultural allegiances, examine if they are bicultural, and test if they can switch between various cultural frames. Most studies examining the cognitive effects of bilingualism do not mention their participants’ cultural or bicultural affiliations. Thus far, studies have conflated bilingualism and biculturalism, with biculturalism subsumed within the construct of bilingualism or not considered a variable. This situation poses a problem when looking at the effects of language status and EF, as they may be separate constructs. Critically, researchers in the field need to be aware of and differentiate the cultural statuses of their participants, namely whether they are monocultural or bicultural and whether they have frequent practice in switching between cultural identities. In particular, bicultural switching effects may explain the discrepancies in the results among certain participant samples who adopted significantly different cultural behaviours at home and at work or school. One such example is that of participants who are second- or third-generation immigrants. While previous studies have addressed the importance of distinguishing between immigrant and non-immigrant populations (Mezzacappa 2004; Fuller-Thomson and Kuh 2014), most bilingual studies do not typically consider the “carryover” cultural effects for second- or even third-generation immigrants. Although these second-generation immigrants may be citizens of the host country, those whose parents (first-generation immigrants) come from a very different culture may find it difficult to reconcile the differences between the cultures within and outside the home. In fact, social and cultural psychologists have long been studying the different acculturation struggles of second-generation immigrants and the influence on their multicultural identity.
For example, Stroink and Lalonde (2009) reported on how some second-generation Asian-Canadian immigrants found it difficult to integrate the differences between their Eastern family culture with the general Western culture in the larger society, resulting in a conflicting bicultural identity. Lee and Kim’s (2014) qualitative study on second-generation Korean immigrants in Germany detailed different coping mechanisms and shifting strategies for bridging Eastern and Western cultures in their daily lives such that individuals could “blend into both Korean and German societies, similar to a chameleon” (p. 97). As such, we hypothesise that if switching between cultural frames occurs both commonly and frequently (e.g., in day-to-day life and interactions), bicultural switching could hone domain-general inhibition control and task-switching. The impact of bicultural switching might thus show similar EF gains reported among code-switching bilingual speakers, such as in the work of Hartanto and Yang (2016). Consequently, the level of bicultural switching (or non-switching) could be distinguished among samples of bilingual and monolingual participants to examine if bicultural switching might interact with the bilingual experience in shaping EF.
As bicultural individuals can select, inhibit, and switch from one cultural mindset (frame) to another depending on their interactional context, we hypothesise that similar cognitive processes may be involved, and its effects on executive control may be analogous to code-switching in bilinguals. According to the adaptive control hypothesis (Green and Abutalebi 2013), it is proposed that “language control processes themselves adapt to the recurrent demands placed on them by the interactional context” (p. 515). In particular, the researchers hypothesised that different interactional contexts (single language, dual language, and code-switching) will impose varying demands on the cognitive control system. For instance, using two languages separately, such as in school and at home (single-language context), is believed to require inhibition of a bilingual person’s other language. However, using two languages in the same context with different speakers (dual-language contexts) requires the most stringent cognitive control processes, such as monitoring, interference, and response inhibition. Finally, frequent code-switching in the same interactional context is hypothesised to require less cognitive suppression and control, as it allows for “opportunistic planning” to freely use lexical items from either language.
Similarly, we hypothesise that the process of selecting, juggling, and switching between multiple cultural mindsets may engage—and thus enhance—cognitive control mechanisms, including inhibitory control, monitoring, and shifting (Spiegler and Leyendecker 2017). While some aspects of EF have been compared in the existing literature on bilingualism, the few studies that have discussed the effects of culture on language and EF have mainly examined response inhibition control (e.g., the Simon and Stroop tasks) and attention-related tasks (e.g., the Attention Network Task). This leaves room for other components, including memory and cognitive flexibility (switching), to be further explored.

7. Conclusions

Although a plethora of evidence contradicting a bilingual advantage in EF exists, some papers still suggest that bilinguals might have the edge over monolinguals in specific contexts. We believe the disparity motivates questions about why there is so little consensus surrounding this complex set of topics, as well as establishing that it is unlikely that a single variable can fully explain the wide variability in findings. Hence, possible confounding variables such as an individual’s cultural affiliation and level of multiculturalism should be carefully considered. While culture’s exact implications and complex effects on EF in bilingual populations are still being examined, growing evidence suggests that cultural variables may mediate bilingual individuals’ cognitive abilities. We believe that greater emphasis needs to be placed on understanding the effects of broad, macro-level cultures, the influences of bicultural switching, and the broader impacts of biculturalism in future studies of a bilingual person’s cognitive advantage. Only by first considering the individual differences and sociolinguistic factors and broadening the current boundaries of what constitutes bilingual people’s interaction context will we be able to arrive at a better understanding of the unique contribution of bilingualism to EF.
In this paper, we argue that cultural variation and biculturalism deserve greater attention from scholars interested in the cognitive effects of bilingualism. Here, we suggest that a more nuanced view of culture needs to be considered, as cultural effects are seldom accounted for or explicitly manipulated in studies of bilingualism. As recent studies comparing important demographic and sociolinguistic factors are making headway in unravelling the puzzle that is bilingual people’s cognition, we hope that this paper takes a small step towards a clearer picture of the exact role that language plays in relation to EF.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. List of key publications explicitly discussing culture in studies on bilingual EF. Asterisk (*) indicates a note about the participant sample.
Table A1. List of key publications explicitly discussing culture in studies on bilingual EF. Asterisk (*) indicates a note about the participant sample.
PublicationEF Task(s)Type of Participants (Country or Nationality, Mage)Presence of Bilingual AdvantageCultural Effects
BilingualsMonolinguals
(Sabbagh et al. 2006)Stroop (day/night and grass/snow), bear/dragon, Dimension Change Cart Sort Task (DCCS), tower building, whisper, and Kansas Reflection-Impulsivity Scale for Preschoolers (KRISP)-109 Chinese (China, 4 y.o.)
107 English (USA, 4 y.o.)
-Among the monolingual children tested, Eastern preschoolers (China) outperformed Western preschoolers (USA) in all measures of executive functioning
(Morton and Harper 2007)Simon Task17 English-French (Canada, 6.8 y.o.)17 English (Canada, 6.8 y.o.)No BL advantage or disadvantage in either congruent or incongruent trials Authors suggested that matching by ethnicity (cultural background) and socioeconomic variables mitigated bilingual advantage
(Bialystok and Viswanathan 2009)Anti-saccade task (faces task)30 English-combination (Canada, 8.5 y.o)
30 English-Tamil or Telugu (India, 8.6 y.o.)
30 English (Canada, 8.5 y.o.)BL advantage in conditions based on inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, but there was no significant difference between groups in response suppressionNo group differences between bilingual groups (Canadians vs. Indians)
(Bialystok et al. 2010)Luria’s tapping task, opposite worlds, reverse categorisation, ANT (Flanker), and mutual exclusivity27 English-combination (Canada, 3.5 y.o.)40 English (Canada, 3 y.o)
20 French (France, 3.5 y.o.)
BL advantage in all executive control (Conflict) tasksNo significant cultural effects reported
29 English-combination (Canada, 4.5 y.o.)29 English (Canada, 4.5 y.o)
17 French (France, 4.5 y.o.)
(Yang et al. 2011)Attention Network Task (ANT)15 Korean-English (USA, 4 y.o.)15 English (USA, 4 y.o.)
13 Korean (USA, 4 y.o.)
13 Korean (Korea, 4 y.o.)
Bilingual advantage over monolingual groups in accuracy and RT, as well as in conflict resolution (Executive control)Overall accuracy of Korean monolinguals from Korea higher than Korean or English monolinguals in the USA, though RT was slower. (Korean MLs in the USA performed similarly to English MLs in the USA.)
Barac and Bialystok (2012)Colour-shape task switching30 Chinese-English (5.9 y.o.)
28 French-English (6.2 y.o.)
20 Spanish-English (6.2 y.o.)
26 English (5.9 y.o.)Bilingual advantage with smaller global task-switching costsNo significant cultural effects reported
* Note: Participant location not specified. Only “large multicultural city” was mentioned (p. 416).
(Tran et al. 2015)ANT13 Spanish-English (USA)
15 Vietnamese-English (USA)
16 Vietnamese-Cantonese (Vietnam)
14 English (USA)
19 Spanish (Argentina)
20 Vietnamese (Vietnam)
BL advantage in accuracy and RT over monolingualsEastern children have faster RT and greater accuracy than Western or Latin American children.
Significant culture and ANT network interaction. Main effect of culture on task performance. Cultural background plays a vital role in the development of the alerting and executive control networks.
* Longitudinal study. Children were initially Mage 38.8 months old (3 y.o.) and tested at 5 time points 6 months apart.
Yang and Yang (2016)ANT32 Korean-English BL (USA, second-generation immigrants, 5–6 y.o.)31 English (USA, 5–6 y.o.)BL advantage in global performance, accuracy, and RT
ANT: No BL effects on network efficiency scores
No specific cultural effects seen. Cultural differences controlled by studying culturally homogenous children and adults.
20 Korean-English (Korean undergraduates who arrived at 10 y.o., currently 19.9 y.o)19 English (USA, 20.7 y.o.)BL advantage in global performance
+ reaction time
ANT: BL advantage in
+ orienting
+ executive control
(Ye et al. 2017)Flanker Task18 Mandarin-English (China, 21 y.o.,)18 Mandarin (China, 21 y.o.)
* Not a completely monolingual: English exposure present, but without passing college English tests
Some evidence that high-proficiency bilinguals outperformed low-proficiency bilingualsMore demanding mixed cultural context cues bring out advantage in incongruent trials for high-proficiency bilingual participants.
(Samuel et al. 2018)Simon Task78 British (21 y.o.)
64 Korean (23 y.o.)
69 mixed nationalities (23 y.o.)
* Level of bilingualism taken as continuous variables based on L2 proficiency, dominance, and code-switching frequency
No BL advantageEast Asian (Korean) participants outperformed Western (British) participants on RT and accuracy regardless of monolingual or bilingual status.
(Tran et al. 2018)DCCS, day/night Stroop, bear dragon, and gift delay13 Spanish-English (USA)
15 Vietnamese-English (USA)
20 Vietnamese-Cantonese (Vietnam)
13 English (USA)
19 Spanish (Argentina)
20 Vietnamese (Vietnam)
BL advantage in DCCS, day/night, and gift delay task and advantage in inhibition and shiftingEastern children (Vietnamese) outperformed Western and Argentinian children in the day/night task.
Cultural effect in response inhibition.
* Longitudinal study. Children were initially Mage 38.7 months old and tested every 6 months.
(Treffers-Daller et al. 2020)Flanker Task29 Turkish-English (Turkey, 32.5 y.o.)
28 Turkish-English (Cyprus, 25.25 y.o.)
* All BL participants were immigrants
30 English (UK, 32.3 y.o.)BL advantage in inhibition (reduced conflict effect)Among BLs, multicultural switching style (alternating or hybrid) was the key explanatory variable for variance in EF performance.
(Cho et al. 2021)Colour and Word Stroop Task33 Korean-English (Canada, 4.7 y.o.)36 English (Canada, 4.4 y.o)
43 Korean (Korea, 4.3 y.o)
No BL advantage (no disadvantage)BL East-Asians show higher accuracy on inhibitory control than ML Canadian children.

References

  1. Abutalebi, Jubin, Lucia Guidi, Virginia Borsa, Matteo Canini, Pasquale A. Della Rosa, Ben A. Parris, and Brendan S. Weekes. 2015. Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia 69: 201–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Altarriba, Jeanette. 2008. Expressions of emotion as mediated by context. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11: 165–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Altarriba, Jeanette, and Dana Basnight-Brown. 2022. The psychology of communication: The interplay between language and culture through time. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 53: 860–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Antón, Eneko, Jon A. Duñabeitia, Adelina Estévez, Juan A. Hernández, Alejandro Castillo, Luis J. Fuentes, Douglas J. Davidson, and Manuel Carreiras. 2014. Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Antoniou, Mark. 2019. The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annual Review of Linguistics 5: 395–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Antoniou, Mark, and Sarah M. Wright. 2017. Uncovering the mechanisms Responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 2217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Antoniou, Kyriakos, Kleanthes K. Grohmann, Maria Kambanaros, and Napoleon Katsos. 2016. The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition 149: 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Arshad, Memoona, and Joanne M. Chung. 2022. Practical recommendations for considering culture, race, and ethnicity in personality psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 16: e12656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bak, Thomas H. 2016. The impact of bilingualism on cognitive ageing and dementia. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6: 205–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bak, Thomas H., Jack J. Nissan, Michael M. Allerhand, and Ian J. Deary. 2014. Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology 75: 959–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Barac, Raluca, and Ellen Bialystok. 2012. Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development 83: 413–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Beatty-Martínez, Anne L., Christian A. Navarro-Torres, Paola E. Dussias, María Teresa Bajo, Rosa E. Guzzardo Tamargo, and Judith F. Kroll. 2020. Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46: 1022–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Benet-Martínez, Verónica, Fiona Lee, and Janxin Leu. 2006. Biculturalism and cognitive complexity: Expertise in cultural representations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 37: 386–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Benet-Martínez, Verónica, Janxin Leu, Fiona Lee, and Michael W. Morris. 2002. Negotiating biculturalism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 33: 492–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Berry, John W. 1997. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology 46: 5–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Berry, John W. 2003. Conceptual approaches to acculturation. In Acculturation: Advances in Theory, Measurement, and Applied Research. Edited by Kevin Chun, Pamela Balls Organista and Gerardo Marín. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 17–37. [Google Scholar]
  17. Best, John R., Patricia H. Miller, and Jack A. Naglieri. 2011. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences 21: 327–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bezin, Emeline, and Fabien Moizeau. 2017. Cultural dynamics, social mobility, and urban segregation. Journal of Urban Economics 99: 173–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bialystok, Ellen. 1999. Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development 70: 636–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bialystok, Ellen. 2011. Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale 65: 229–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Bialystok, Ellen. 2015. Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives 9: 117–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Bialystok, Ellen. 2017. The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143: 233–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Bialystok, Ellen. 2021. Bilingualism: Pathway to cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 25: 355–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Bialystok, Ellen, and Fergus I. M. Craik. 2022. How does bilingualism modify cognitive function? Attention to the mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 29: 1246–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Bialystok, Ellen, and Mythili Viswanathan. 2009. Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition 112: 494–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, and Gigi Luk. 2012. Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 240–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, and Morris Freedman. 2007. Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 45: 459–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, Raymond Klein, and Mythili Viswanathan. 2004. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the simon task. Psychology and Aging 19: 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bialystok, Ellen, Raluca Barac, Agnes Blaye, and Diane Poulin-Dubois. 2010. Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development 11: 485–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Blom, Elma, Tessel Boerma, Evelyn Bosma, Leonie Cornips, and Emma Everaert. 2017. Cognitive advantages of bilingual children in different sociolinguistic contexts. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Bornstein, Marc H. 2013. Parenting and child mental health: A cross-cultural perspective. World Psychiatry 12: 258–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Byers-Heinlein, Krista, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2013. Bilingualism in the early years: What the science says. Learning Landscapes 7: 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Calvo, Noelia, Adolfo M. García, Laura Manoiloff, and Agustín Ibáñez. 2016. Bilingualism and cognitive reserve: A critical overview and a plea for methodological innovations. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 7: 249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Chen, Sylvia Xiaohua, and Michael Harris Bond. 2010. Two languages, two personalities? Examining language effects on the expression of personality in a bilingual context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36: 1514–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Chen, Xinyin, Paul D. Hastings, Kenneth H. Rubin, Huichang Chen, Guozhen Cen, and Shannon L. Stewart. 1998. Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology 34: 677–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Cheng, Chi-Ying, Fiona Lee, Verónica Benet-Martínez, and Que-Lam Huynh. 2014. Variations in multicultural experience: Influence of bicultural identity integration on socio-cognitive processes and outcomes. In The Oxford Handbook of Multicultural Identity. Edited by Veronica Benet-Martínez and Ying-Yi Hong. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 276–99. [Google Scholar]
  37. Cho, Isu, Jewan Park, Hyun-joo Song, and J. Bruce Morton. 2021. Disentangling language status and country-of-origin explanations of the bilingual advantage in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 212: 105235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Collins, Anne, and Etienne Koechlin. 2012. Reasoning, learning, and creativity: Frontal lobe function and human decision-making. PLoS Biology 10: e1001293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Colzato, Lorenza S., Maria Teresa Bajo, Wery Van den Wildenberg, Daniela Paolieri, Sander Nieuwenhuis, Wido La Heij, and Bernhard Hommel. 2008. How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34: 302–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Costa, Albert, Michele Miozzo, and Alfonso Caramazza. 1999. Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual’s two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language 41: 365–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Costa, Albert, Mireia Hernández, Jordi Costa-Faidella, and Núria Sebastián-Gallés. 2009. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 113: 135–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Crisp, Richard J., and Rhiannon N. Turner. 2011. Cognitive adaptation to the experience of social and cultural diversity. Psychological Bulletin 137: 242–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. de Baene, Wouter, Wouter Duyck, Marcel Brass, and Manuel Carreiras. 2015. Brain circuit for cognitive control Is shared by task and language switching. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27: 1752–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. de Bruin, Angela. 2019. Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences 9: 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. de Bruin, Angela, Barbara Treccani, and Sergio Della Sala. 2015. Cognitive advantage in bilingualism. Psychological Science 26: 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Diamond, Adele. 2013. Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Dick, Anthony Steven, Nelcida L. Garcia, Shannon M. Pruden, Wesley K. Thompson, Samuel W. Hawes, Matthew T. Sutherland, Michael C. Riedel, Angela R. Laird, and Raul Gonzalez. 2019. No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour 3: 692–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Dijkstra, Ton, and Judith F. Kroll. 2005. Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Edited by Judith F. Kroll and Annette. M. B. de Groot. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 179–201. [Google Scholar]
  49. Fuller-Thomson, Esme, and Diana Kuh. 2014. The healthy migrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 52: 128–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Garcia, Fredi, Diana Mendez, Chris Ellis, and Casey Gautney. 2014. Cross-cultural, values and ethics differences and similarities between the US and Asian countries. Journal of Technology Management in China 9: 303–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller, Enlli M. Thomas, Ivan Kennedy, Cynog Prys, Nia Young, Nestor Viñas Guasch, Emily J. Roberts, Emma K. Hughes, and Leah Jones. 2014. Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Goriot, Claire, Mirjam Broersma, James M. McQueen, Sharon Unsworth, and Roeland Van Hout. 2018. Language balance and switching ability in children acquiring English as a second language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 173: 168–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Green, David W. 1998. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1: 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Green, David W., and Jubin Abutalebi. 2013. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25: 515–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Grosjean, François. 1992. Another view of bilingualism. Cognitive Processing in Bilinguals 83: 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Grosjean, François. 2015. Bicultural bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 19: 572–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Harkness, Sara, Charles M. Super, and Nathalie Van Tijen. 2000. Individualism and the “Western mind” reconsidered: American and Dutch parents’ ethnotheories of the child. In Variability in the Social Construction of the Child. Edited by Sara Harkness, Catherine Raeff and Charles M. Super. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, pp. 23–39. [Google Scholar]
  58. Hartanto, Andree, and Hwajin Yang. 2016. Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 150: 10–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hernández, Mireia, Clara D. Martin, Francisco Barceló, and Albert Costa. 2013. Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? Journal of Memory and Language 69: 257–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Hofstede, Geert. 1980. Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills and London: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  61. Hofstede, Geert. 2001. Culture’s Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions, and Organizations across Nations. Thousand Oaks: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  62. Hofstede, Geert. 2011. Dimensionalizing cultures: The hofstede model in context. Readings in Psychology and Culture 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hong, Ying-yi, Michael W. Morris, Chi-yue Chiu, and Veronica Benet-Martinez. 2000. Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist 55: 709–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Imada, Toshie, Stephanie M. Carlson, and Shoji Itakura. 2012. East-West cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Developmental Science 16: 198–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Jaramillo, Jorge M., María I. Rendón, Lorena Muñoz, Mirjam Weis, and Gisela Trommsdorff. 2017. Children’s self-Regulation in cultural contexts: The role of parental socialization theories, goals, and practices. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Jia, Fanli, and Tobias Krettenauer. 2017. Recognizing moral identity as a cultural construct. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Kelkar, Amit S., Monica S. Hough, and Xiangming Fang. 2013. Do we think alike? A cross-cultural study of executive functioning. Culture and Brain 1: 118–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kim, Young-Hoon, Chi-Yue Chiu, Siqing Peng, Huajian Cai, and William Tov. 2009. Explaining east-west differences in the likelihood of making favorable self-evaluations: The role of evaluation apprehension and directness of expression. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 41: 62–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Kitayama, Shinobu, Batja Mesquita, and Mayumi Karasawa. 2006. Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 890–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Kiyokawa, Sachiko, Zoltán Dienes, Daisuke Tanaka, and Ayumi Yamada. 2010. Cross cultural differences in implicit learning. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Portland, OR, USA, August 11–14; Available online: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0c98t8p9 (accessed on 1 September 2022).
  71. Kiyokawa, Sachiko, Zoltán Dienes, Daisuke Tanaka, Ayumi Yamada, and Louise Crowe. 2012. Cross cultural differences in unconscious knowledge. Cognition 124: 16–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Kousaie, Shanna, and Natalie A. Phillips. 2012. Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65: 356–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Kovács, Ágnes Melinda, and Jacques Mehler. 2009. Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 6556–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Krassner, Ariye M., Maria A. Gartstein, Curie Park, Wojciech Ł. Dragan, Felipe Lecannelier, and Samuel P. Putnam. 2016. East–west, collectivist-individualist: A cross-cultural examination of temperament in toddlers from Chile, Poland, South Korea, and the U.S. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 14: 449–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Kroll, Judith F., Susan C. Bobb, and Zofia Wodniecka. 2006. Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9: 119–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Kroll, Judith F., Susan C. Bobb, Maya Misra, and Taomei Guo. 2008. Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica 128: 416–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Kroll, Judith F., Paola E. Dussias, Kinsey Bice, and Lauren Perrotti. 2015. Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics 1: 377–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Kuwabara, Megumi, and Linda B. Smith. 2012. Cross-cultural differences in cognitive development: Attention to relations and objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 113: 20–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. LaFromboise, Teresa, Hardin LK Coleman, and Jennifer Gerton. 1993. Psychological impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin 114: 395–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Lan, Xuezhao, Cristine H. Legare, Claire Cameron Ponitz, Su Li, and Frederick J. Morrison. 2011. Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: A cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108: 677–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Lee, Hyo-Seon, and Seok-Ki Kim. 2014. A qualitative study on the bicultural experience of second-generation Korean immigrants in Germany. Pacific Science Review 16: 97–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Legare, Cristine H., Michael T. Dale, Sarah Y. Kim, and Gedeon O. Deák. 2018. Cultural variation in cognitive flexibility reveals diversity in the development of executive functions. Scientific Reports 8: 16326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Lehtonen, Minna, Anna Soveri, Aini Laine, Janica Järvenpää, Angela De Bruin, and Jan Antfolk. 2018. Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 144: 394–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Ling, Xiaoli, Li Zheng, Xiuyan Guo, Shouxin Li, Shiyu Song, Lining Sun, and Zoltan Dienes. 2018. Cross-cultural differences in implicit learning of chunks versus symmetries. Royal Society Open Science 5: 180469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Lowe, Cassandra J., Isu Cho, Samantha F. Goldsmith, and J. Bruce Morton. 2021. The bilingual advantage in children’s executive functioning is not related to language status: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Science 32: 1115–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Luk, Gigi, and Ellen Bialystok. 2013. Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25: 605–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Luna, David, Torsten Ringberg, and Laura A. Peracchio. 2008. One individual, two identities: Frame switching among biculturals. Journal of Consumer Research 35: 279–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Lunt, Laura, Jessica Bramham, Robin G. Morris, Peter R. Bullock, Richard P. Selway, Kiriakos Xenitidis, and Anthony S. David. 2012. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and ‘jumping to conclusions’: Bias or deficit? Journal of Neuropsychology 6: 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Luo, Lin, Gigi Luk, and Ellen Bialystok. 2010. Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition 114: 29–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Marian, Viorica, and Michael Spivey. 2003. Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 173–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Markus, Hazel Rose, and Shinobu Kitayama. 1991. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review 98: 224–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Markus, Hazel Rose, and Shinobu Kitayama. 2010. Cultures and Selves. Perspectives on Psychological Science 5: 420–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Martin, Clara D., Kristof Strijkers, Mikel Santesteban, Carles Escera, Robert J. Hartsuiker, and Albert Costa. 2013. The impact of early bilingualism on controlling a language learned late: An ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Martin-Rhee, Michelle M., and Ellen Bialystok. 2008. The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11: 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Masuda, Takahiko, and Richard E. Nisbett. 2001. Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 922–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Masuda, Takahiko, Phoebe C. Ellsworth, Batja Mesquita, Janxin Leu, Shigehito Tanida, and Ellen Van de Veerdonk. 2008. Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94: 365–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Meca, Alan, Raha F. Sabet, Colleen M. Farrelly, Cynthia G. Benitez, Seth J. Schwartz, Melinda Gonzales-Backen, and Elma I. Lorenzo-Blanco. 2017. Personal and cultural identity development in recently immigrated Hispanic adolescents: Links with psychosocial functioning. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 23: 348–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Mezzacappa, Enrico. 2004. Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development 75: 1373–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Miyake, Akira, and Naomi P. Friedman. 2012. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21: 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” Tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Miyamoto, Yuri, Richard E. Nisbett, and Takahiko Masuda. 2006. Culture and the physical environment: Holistic versus analytic perceptual affordances. Psychological Science 17: 113–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Morton, J. Bruce, and Sarah N. Harper. 2007. What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science 10: 719–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Nayak, Srishti, Hiba Z. Salem, and Amanda R. Tarullo. 2020. Neural mechanisms of response-preparation and inhibition in bilingual and monolingual children: Lateralized Readiness Potentials (LRPs) during a nonverbal Stroop task. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 41: 100740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Nguyen, Angela-MinhTu D., and Verónica Benet-Martínez. 2007. Biculturalism unpacked: Components, measurement, individual differences, and outcomes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 1: 101–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Nguyen, Angela-MinhTu D., and Verónica Benet-Martínez. 2012. Biculturalism and adjustment. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44: 122–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Nickels, Lyndsey, Solène Hameau, Vishnu KK Nair, Polly Barr, and Britta Biedermann. 2019. Ageing with bilingualism: Benefits and challenges. Speech, Language and Hearing 22: 32–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Nisbett, Richard E., and Takahiko Masuda. 2003. Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy Of Sciences 100: 11163–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Nisbett, Richard E., and Takahiko Masuda. 2006. Culture and Point of View. In Biological and Cultural Bases of Human Inference. Edited by Riccardo Viale, Daniel Andler and Lawrence A. Hirschfeld. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 49–70. [Google Scholar]
  109. Nisbett, Richard E., and Yuri Miyamoto. 2005. The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 467–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Nisbett, Richard E., Kaiping Peng, Incheol Choi, and Ara Norenzayan. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108: 291–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Norenzayan, Ara, Edward E. Smith, Beom Jun Kim, and Richard E. Nisbett. 2002. Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science 26: 653–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Oh, Seungmi, and Charlie Lewis. 2008. Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Development 79: 80–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Ooi, Seok Hui, Winston D. Goh, Antonella Sorace, and Thomas H. Bak. 2018. From Bilingualism to bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21: 867–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Oyserman, Daphna, and Spike WS Lee. 2008. Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134: 311–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Paap, Kenneth R. 2019. Bilingualism in cognitive science: The characteristics and consequences of bilingual language control. In The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism. Edited by Annick De Houwer and Lourdes Ortega. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 435–65. [Google Scholar]
  116. Paap, Kenneth R., and Zachary I. Greenberg. 2013. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66: 232–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Paap, Kenneth R., and Oliver Sawi. 2014. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Paap, Kenneth R., Hunter A. Johnson, and Oliver Sawi. 2015. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 69: 265–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Paap, Kenneth R., Regina Anders-Jefferson, Lauren Mason, Katerinne Alvarado, and Brandon Zimiga. 2018. Bilingual advantages in inhibition or selective attention: More challenges. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Paap, Kenneth R., Regina Anders-Jefferson, Roman Mikulinsky, Shigeaki Masuda, and Lauren Mason. 2019. On the encapsulation of bilingual language control. Journal of Memory and Language 105: 76–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Padilla, Amado M. 2006. Bicultural social development. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 28: 467–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Pavlenko, Aneta. 2005. Emotions and Multilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  123. Peal, Elizabeth, and Wallace E. Lambert. 1962. The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 76: 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Pelham, Sabra D., and Lise Abrams. 2014. Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40: 313–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Poulin-Dubois, Diane, Agnes Blaye, Julie Coutya, and Ellen Bialystok. 2011. The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108: 567–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Prior, Anat, and Brian MacWhinney. 2009. A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13: 253–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Ramírez-Esparza, Nairán, Samuel D. Gosling, Verónica Benet-Martínez, Jeffrey P. Potter, and James W. Pennebaker. 2006. Do bilinguals have two personalities? A special case of cultural frame switching. Journal of Research in Personality 40: 99–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Sabbagh, Mark A., Fen Xu, Stephanie M. Carlson, Louis J. Moses, and Kang Lee. 2006. The development of executive functioning and theory of mind. A comparison of Chinese and U.S. Preschoolers. Psychological Science 17: 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Samuel, Steven, Karen Roehr-Brackin, Hyensou Pak, and Hyunji Kim. 2018. Cultural effects rather than a bilingual advantage in cognition: A review and an empirical study. Cognitive Science 42: 2313–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Sarma, U. Arathi, and Tissy Mariam Thomas. 2020. Breaking the limits of executive functions: Towards a sociocultural perspective. Culture & Psychology 26: 358–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Schwartz, Seth J., Dina Birman, Verónica Benet-Martínez, and Jennifer B. Unger. 2017. Biculturalism: Negotiating multiple cultural streams. In The Oxford Handbook of Acculturation and Health. Edited by Seth J. Schwartz and Jennifer B. Unger. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 29–47. [Google Scholar]
  132. Shih, Margaret, and Diana T. Sanchez. 2005. Perspectives and research on the positive and negative implications of having multiple racial identities. Psychological Bulletin 131: 569–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Soffietti, James P. 1960. Bilingualism and biculturalism. The Modern Language Journal 44: 275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Spiegler, Olivia, and Birgit Leyendecker. 2017. Balanced cultural identities promote cognitive flexibility among immigrant children. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Spivey, Michael J., and Viorica Marian. 1999. Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science 10: 281–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Stroink, Mirella L., and Richard N. Lalonde. 2009. Bicultural identity conflict in second-generation Asian Canadians. The Journal of Social Psychology 149: 44–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Suárez-Orozco, Carola, Marcelo M. Suárez-Orozco, and Irina Todorova. 2008. Learning a New Land: Immigrant Students in American Society. Cambridge: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  138. Tadmor, Carmit T., and Philip E. Tetlock. 2006. Biculturalism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 37: 173–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Thierry, Guillaume, and Yan Jing Wu. 2007. Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 12530–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Tomoschuk, Brendan, Victor S. Ferreira, and Tamar H. Gollan. 2018. When a seven is not a seven: Self-ratings of bilingual language proficiency differ between and within language populations. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22: 516–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Tran, Crystal D., Maria M. Arredondo, and Hanako Yoshida. 2015. Differential effects of bilingualism and culture on early attention: A longitudinal study in the U.S., Argentina, and Vietnam. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Tran, Crystal D., Maria M. Arredondo, and Hanako Yoshida. 2018. Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22: 714–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Treffers-Daller, Jeanine, Zehra Ongun, Julia Hofweber, and Michal Korenar. 2020. Explaining individual differences in executive functions performance in multilinguals: The impact of code-switching and alternating between multicultural identity styles. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 561088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Triandis, Harry C. 1989. The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review 96: 506–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Triandis, Harry C. 2001. Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality 69: 907–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Tse, Chi-Shing, and Jeanette Altarriba. 2012. The effects of first- and second-language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: Evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15: 663–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Tse, Chi-Shing, and Jeanette Altarriba. 2014. The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Valian, Virginia. 2014. Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18: 3–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Varnum, Michael EW, Igor Grossmann, Shinobu Kitayama, and Richard E. Nisbett. 2010. The origin of cultural differences in cognition: Evidence for the social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Von Bastian, Claudia C., Alessandra S. Souza, and Miriam Gade. 2016. No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145: 246–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Ward, Colleen, Caroline Ng Tseung-Wong, Agnes Szabo, Tamara Qumseya, and Uma Bhowon. 2018. Hybrid and alternating identity styles as strategies for managing multicultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 49: 1402–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Weissberger, Gali H., Tamar H. Gollan, Mark W. Bondi, Daniel A. Nation, Lawrence A. Hansen, Douglas Galasko, and David P. Salmon. 2019. Neuropsychological deficit profiles, vascular risk factors, and neuropathological findings in hispanic Older adults with autopsy-confirmed Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 67: 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Wiseheart, Melody, Mythili Viswanathan, and Ellen Bialystok. 2014. Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19: 141–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Yang, Hwajin, Andree Hartanto, and Sujin Yang. 2016. The complex nature of Bilinguals’ language usage modulates task-switching outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Yang, Sujin, and Hwajin Yang. 2016. Bilingual effects on deployment of the attention system in linguistically and culturally homogeneous children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 146: 121–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Yang, Sujin, Hwajin Yang, and Barbara Lust. 2011. Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14: 412–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Ye, Yanyan, Lei Mo, and Qihan Wu. 2017. Mixed cultural context brings out bilingual advantage on executive function. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 844–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  158. Yow, W. Quin, and Xiaoqian Li. 2015. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Table 1. Some generalised traits of “Eastern” compared with “Western” cultures.
Table 1. Some generalised traits of “Eastern” compared with “Western” cultures.
“Eastern”“Western”
Languages 07 00247 i001
Power distanceLargeSmall
Uncertainty avoidanceHighLow
Individualism-collectivismCollectivismIndividualism
OrientationLong termShort term
Indugence-restraintRestrainedIndulgence
Table 2. Four possible outcomes from Berry’s Acculturation Model (Berry 1997, 2003).
Table 2. Four possible outcomes from Berry’s Acculturation Model (Berry 1997, 2003).
+ ← Maintenance of heritage culture → −
− ← Cultural Adaptation → +IntegrationAssimilation
SeparationMarginalisation
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Xie, W.; Altarriba, J.; Ng, B.C. Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship? Languages 2022, 7, 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247

AMA Style

Xie W, Altarriba J, Ng BC. Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship? Languages. 2022; 7(4):247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247

Chicago/Turabian Style

Xie, Wenhan, Jeanette Altarriba, and Bee Chin Ng. 2022. "Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship?" Languages 7, no. 4: 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247

APA Style

Xie, W., Altarriba, J., & Ng, B. C. (2022). Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship? Languages, 7(4), 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop