Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. The Bilingual Advantage in EF Debate
3. Conflicting Findings in EF Performance
3.1. Methodological Concerns
3.2. Hidden Confounds
4. Culture: An Often-Overlooked Factor
4.1. Studies in Young Children
4.2. Studies in Adults
5. Is There a Need to Disambiguate Cultural Effects from Language Effects in Studies on Executive Functions?
5.1. Macro-Scale Cultural Differences and EF
5.2. Micro-Level Cultural Factors: Individual Acculturation and Bilingualism
6. Future Directions
7. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Publication | EF Task(s) | Type of Participants (Country or Nationality, Mage) | Presence of Bilingual Advantage | Cultural Effects | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bilinguals | Monolinguals | ||||
(Sabbagh et al. 2006) | Stroop (day/night and grass/snow), bear/dragon, Dimension Change Cart Sort Task (DCCS), tower building, whisper, and Kansas Reflection-Impulsivity Scale for Preschoolers (KRISP) | - | 109 Chinese (China, 4 y.o.) 107 English (USA, 4 y.o.) | - | Among the monolingual children tested, Eastern preschoolers (China) outperformed Western preschoolers (USA) in all measures of executive functioning |
(Morton and Harper 2007) | Simon Task | 17 English-French (Canada, 6.8 y.o.) | 17 English (Canada, 6.8 y.o.) | No BL advantage or disadvantage in either congruent or incongruent trials | Authors suggested that matching by ethnicity (cultural background) and socioeconomic variables mitigated bilingual advantage |
(Bialystok and Viswanathan 2009) | Anti-saccade task (faces task) | 30 English-combination (Canada, 8.5 y.o) 30 English-Tamil or Telugu (India, 8.6 y.o.) | 30 English (Canada, 8.5 y.o.) | BL advantage in conditions based on inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, but there was no significant difference between groups in response suppression | No group differences between bilingual groups (Canadians vs. Indians) |
(Bialystok et al. 2010) | Luria’s tapping task, opposite worlds, reverse categorisation, ANT (Flanker), and mutual exclusivity | 27 English-combination (Canada, 3.5 y.o.) | 40 English (Canada, 3 y.o) 20 French (France, 3.5 y.o.) | BL advantage in all executive control (Conflict) tasks | No significant cultural effects reported |
29 English-combination (Canada, 4.5 y.o.) | 29 English (Canada, 4.5 y.o) 17 French (France, 4.5 y.o.) | ||||
(Yang et al. 2011) | Attention Network Task (ANT) | 15 Korean-English (USA, 4 y.o.) | 15 English (USA, 4 y.o.) 13 Korean (USA, 4 y.o.) 13 Korean (Korea, 4 y.o.) | Bilingual advantage over monolingual groups in accuracy and RT, as well as in conflict resolution (Executive control) | Overall accuracy of Korean monolinguals from Korea higher than Korean or English monolinguals in the USA, though RT was slower. (Korean MLs in the USA performed similarly to English MLs in the USA.) |
Barac and Bialystok (2012) | Colour-shape task switching | 30 Chinese-English (5.9 y.o.) 28 French-English (6.2 y.o.) 20 Spanish-English (6.2 y.o.) | 26 English (5.9 y.o.) | Bilingual advantage with smaller global task-switching costs | No significant cultural effects reported |
* Note: Participant location not specified. Only “large multicultural city” was mentioned (p. 416). | |||||
(Tran et al. 2015) | ANT | 13 Spanish-English (USA) 15 Vietnamese-English (USA) 16 Vietnamese-Cantonese (Vietnam) | 14 English (USA) 19 Spanish (Argentina) 20 Vietnamese (Vietnam) | BL advantage in accuracy and RT over monolinguals | Eastern children have faster RT and greater accuracy than Western or Latin American children. Significant culture and ANT network interaction. Main effect of culture on task performance. Cultural background plays a vital role in the development of the alerting and executive control networks. |
* Longitudinal study. Children were initially Mage 38.8 months old (3 y.o.) and tested at 5 time points 6 months apart. | |||||
Yang and Yang (2016) | ANT | 32 Korean-English BL (USA, second-generation immigrants, 5–6 y.o.) | 31 English (USA, 5–6 y.o.) | BL advantage in global performance, accuracy, and RT ANT: No BL effects on network efficiency scores | No specific cultural effects seen. Cultural differences controlled by studying culturally homogenous children and adults. |
20 Korean-English (Korean undergraduates who arrived at 10 y.o., currently 19.9 y.o) | 19 English (USA, 20.7 y.o.) | BL advantage in global performance + reaction time ANT: BL advantage in + orienting + executive control | |||
(Ye et al. 2017) | Flanker Task | 18 Mandarin-English (China, 21 y.o.,) | 18 Mandarin (China, 21 y.o.) * Not a completely monolingual: English exposure present, but without passing college English tests | Some evidence that high-proficiency bilinguals outperformed low-proficiency bilinguals | More demanding mixed cultural context cues bring out advantage in incongruent trials for high-proficiency bilingual participants. |
(Samuel et al. 2018) | Simon Task | 78 British (21 y.o.) 64 Korean (23 y.o.) 69 mixed nationalities (23 y.o.) * Level of bilingualism taken as continuous variables based on L2 proficiency, dominance, and code-switching frequency | No BL advantage | East Asian (Korean) participants outperformed Western (British) participants on RT and accuracy regardless of monolingual or bilingual status. | |
(Tran et al. 2018) | DCCS, day/night Stroop, bear dragon, and gift delay | 13 Spanish-English (USA) 15 Vietnamese-English (USA) 20 Vietnamese-Cantonese (Vietnam) | 13 English (USA) 19 Spanish (Argentina) 20 Vietnamese (Vietnam) | BL advantage in DCCS, day/night, and gift delay task and advantage in inhibition and shifting | Eastern children (Vietnamese) outperformed Western and Argentinian children in the day/night task. Cultural effect in response inhibition. |
* Longitudinal study. Children were initially Mage 38.7 months old and tested every 6 months. | |||||
(Treffers-Daller et al. 2020) | Flanker Task | 29 Turkish-English (Turkey, 32.5 y.o.) 28 Turkish-English (Cyprus, 25.25 y.o.) * All BL participants were immigrants | 30 English (UK, 32.3 y.o.) | BL advantage in inhibition (reduced conflict effect) | Among BLs, multicultural switching style (alternating or hybrid) was the key explanatory variable for variance in EF performance. |
(Cho et al. 2021) | Colour and Word Stroop Task | 33 Korean-English (Canada, 4.7 y.o.) | 36 English (Canada, 4.4 y.o) 43 Korean (Korea, 4.3 y.o) | No BL advantage (no disadvantage) | BL East-Asians show higher accuracy on inhibitory control than ML Canadian children. |
References
- Abutalebi, Jubin, Lucia Guidi, Virginia Borsa, Matteo Canini, Pasquale A. Della Rosa, Ben A. Parris, and Brendan S. Weekes. 2015. Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia 69: 201–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Altarriba, Jeanette. 2008. Expressions of emotion as mediated by context. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11: 165–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Altarriba, Jeanette, and Dana Basnight-Brown. 2022. The psychology of communication: The interplay between language and culture through time. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 53: 860–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antón, Eneko, Jon A. Duñabeitia, Adelina Estévez, Juan A. Hernández, Alejandro Castillo, Luis J. Fuentes, Douglas J. Davidson, and Manuel Carreiras. 2014. Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antoniou, Mark. 2019. The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annual Review of Linguistics 5: 395–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antoniou, Mark, and Sarah M. Wright. 2017. Uncovering the mechanisms Responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 2217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Antoniou, Kyriakos, Kleanthes K. Grohmann, Maria Kambanaros, and Napoleon Katsos. 2016. The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition 149: 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arshad, Memoona, and Joanne M. Chung. 2022. Practical recommendations for considering culture, race, and ethnicity in personality psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 16: e12656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bak, Thomas H. 2016. The impact of bilingualism on cognitive ageing and dementia. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6: 205–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bak, Thomas H., Jack J. Nissan, Michael M. Allerhand, and Ian J. Deary. 2014. Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology 75: 959–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barac, Raluca, and Ellen Bialystok. 2012. Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development 83: 413–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beatty-Martínez, Anne L., Christian A. Navarro-Torres, Paola E. Dussias, María Teresa Bajo, Rosa E. Guzzardo Tamargo, and Judith F. Kroll. 2020. Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46: 1022–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benet-Martínez, Verónica, Fiona Lee, and Janxin Leu. 2006. Biculturalism and cognitive complexity: Expertise in cultural representations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 37: 386–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benet-Martínez, Verónica, Janxin Leu, Fiona Lee, and Michael W. Morris. 2002. Negotiating biculturalism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 33: 492–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berry, John W. 1997. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology 46: 5–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berry, John W. 2003. Conceptual approaches to acculturation. In Acculturation: Advances in Theory, Measurement, and Applied Research. Edited by Kevin Chun, Pamela Balls Organista and Gerardo Marín. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 17–37. [Google Scholar]
- Best, John R., Patricia H. Miller, and Jack A. Naglieri. 2011. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences 21: 327–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bezin, Emeline, and Fabien Moizeau. 2017. Cultural dynamics, social mobility, and urban segregation. Journal of Urban Economics 99: 173–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 1999. Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development 70: 636–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2011. Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale 65: 229–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2015. Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives 9: 117–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2017. The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143: 233–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2021. Bilingualism: Pathway to cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 25: 355–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, Ellen, and Fergus I. M. Craik. 2022. How does bilingualism modify cognitive function? Attention to the mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 29: 1246–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen, and Mythili Viswanathan. 2009. Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition 112: 494–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, and Gigi Luk. 2012. Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 240–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, and Morris Freedman. 2007. Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 45: 459–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, Raymond Klein, and Mythili Viswanathan. 2004. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the simon task. Psychology and Aging 19: 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Raluca Barac, Agnes Blaye, and Diane Poulin-Dubois. 2010. Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development 11: 485–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blom, Elma, Tessel Boerma, Evelyn Bosma, Leonie Cornips, and Emma Everaert. 2017. Cognitive advantages of bilingual children in different sociolinguistic contexts. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bornstein, Marc H. 2013. Parenting and child mental health: A cross-cultural perspective. World Psychiatry 12: 258–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Byers-Heinlein, Krista, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2013. Bilingualism in the early years: What the science says. Learning Landscapes 7: 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Calvo, Noelia, Adolfo M. García, Laura Manoiloff, and Agustín Ibáñez. 2016. Bilingualism and cognitive reserve: A critical overview and a plea for methodological innovations. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 7: 249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chen, Sylvia Xiaohua, and Michael Harris Bond. 2010. Two languages, two personalities? Examining language effects on the expression of personality in a bilingual context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36: 1514–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chen, Xinyin, Paul D. Hastings, Kenneth H. Rubin, Huichang Chen, Guozhen Cen, and Shannon L. Stewart. 1998. Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology 34: 677–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, Chi-Ying, Fiona Lee, Verónica Benet-Martínez, and Que-Lam Huynh. 2014. Variations in multicultural experience: Influence of bicultural identity integration on socio-cognitive processes and outcomes. In The Oxford Handbook of Multicultural Identity. Edited by Veronica Benet-Martínez and Ying-Yi Hong. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 276–99. [Google Scholar]
- Cho, Isu, Jewan Park, Hyun-joo Song, and J. Bruce Morton. 2021. Disentangling language status and country-of-origin explanations of the bilingual advantage in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 212: 105235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collins, Anne, and Etienne Koechlin. 2012. Reasoning, learning, and creativity: Frontal lobe function and human decision-making. PLoS Biology 10: e1001293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colzato, Lorenza S., Maria Teresa Bajo, Wery Van den Wildenberg, Daniela Paolieri, Sander Nieuwenhuis, Wido La Heij, and Bernhard Hommel. 2008. How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34: 302–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, Albert, Michele Miozzo, and Alfonso Caramazza. 1999. Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual’s two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language 41: 365–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, Albert, Mireia Hernández, Jordi Costa-Faidella, and Núria Sebastián-Gallés. 2009. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 113: 135–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crisp, Richard J., and Rhiannon N. Turner. 2011. Cognitive adaptation to the experience of social and cultural diversity. Psychological Bulletin 137: 242–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Baene, Wouter, Wouter Duyck, Marcel Brass, and Manuel Carreiras. 2015. Brain circuit for cognitive control Is shared by task and language switching. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27: 1752–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- de Bruin, Angela. 2019. Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences 9: 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- de Bruin, Angela, Barbara Treccani, and Sergio Della Sala. 2015. Cognitive advantage in bilingualism. Psychological Science 26: 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, Adele. 2013. Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dick, Anthony Steven, Nelcida L. Garcia, Shannon M. Pruden, Wesley K. Thompson, Samuel W. Hawes, Matthew T. Sutherland, Michael C. Riedel, Angela R. Laird, and Raul Gonzalez. 2019. No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour 3: 692–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dijkstra, Ton, and Judith F. Kroll. 2005. Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Edited by Judith F. Kroll and Annette. M. B. de Groot. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 179–201. [Google Scholar]
- Fuller-Thomson, Esme, and Diana Kuh. 2014. The healthy migrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 52: 128–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garcia, Fredi, Diana Mendez, Chris Ellis, and Casey Gautney. 2014. Cross-cultural, values and ethics differences and similarities between the US and Asian countries. Journal of Technology Management in China 9: 303–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller, Enlli M. Thomas, Ivan Kennedy, Cynog Prys, Nia Young, Nestor Viñas Guasch, Emily J. Roberts, Emma K. Hughes, and Leah Jones. 2014. Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goriot, Claire, Mirjam Broersma, James M. McQueen, Sharon Unsworth, and Roeland Van Hout. 2018. Language balance and switching ability in children acquiring English as a second language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 173: 168–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, David W. 1998. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1: 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, David W., and Jubin Abutalebi. 2013. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25: 515–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grosjean, François. 1992. Another view of bilingualism. Cognitive Processing in Bilinguals 83: 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, François. 2015. Bicultural bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 19: 572–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harkness, Sara, Charles M. Super, and Nathalie Van Tijen. 2000. Individualism and the “Western mind” reconsidered: American and Dutch parents’ ethnotheories of the child. In Variability in the Social Construction of the Child. Edited by Sara Harkness, Catherine Raeff and Charles M. Super. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, pp. 23–39. [Google Scholar]
- Hartanto, Andree, and Hwajin Yang. 2016. Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 150: 10–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hernández, Mireia, Clara D. Martin, Francisco Barceló, and Albert Costa. 2013. Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? Journal of Memory and Language 69: 257–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofstede, Geert. 1980. Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills and London: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede, Geert. 2001. Culture’s Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions, and Organizations across Nations. Thousand Oaks: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Hofstede, Geert. 2011. Dimensionalizing cultures: The hofstede model in context. Readings in Psychology and Culture 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hong, Ying-yi, Michael W. Morris, Chi-yue Chiu, and Veronica Benet-Martinez. 2000. Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist 55: 709–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Imada, Toshie, Stephanie M. Carlson, and Shoji Itakura. 2012. East-West cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Developmental Science 16: 198–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaramillo, Jorge M., María I. Rendón, Lorena Muñoz, Mirjam Weis, and Gisela Trommsdorff. 2017. Children’s self-Regulation in cultural contexts: The role of parental socialization theories, goals, and practices. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jia, Fanli, and Tobias Krettenauer. 2017. Recognizing moral identity as a cultural construct. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kelkar, Amit S., Monica S. Hough, and Xiangming Fang. 2013. Do we think alike? A cross-cultural study of executive functioning. Culture and Brain 1: 118–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Young-Hoon, Chi-Yue Chiu, Siqing Peng, Huajian Cai, and William Tov. 2009. Explaining east-west differences in the likelihood of making favorable self-evaluations: The role of evaluation apprehension and directness of expression. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 41: 62–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kitayama, Shinobu, Batja Mesquita, and Mayumi Karasawa. 2006. Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 890–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kiyokawa, Sachiko, Zoltán Dienes, Daisuke Tanaka, and Ayumi Yamada. 2010. Cross cultural differences in implicit learning. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Portland, OR, USA, August 11–14; Available online: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0c98t8p9 (accessed on 1 September 2022).
- Kiyokawa, Sachiko, Zoltán Dienes, Daisuke Tanaka, Ayumi Yamada, and Louise Crowe. 2012. Cross cultural differences in unconscious knowledge. Cognition 124: 16–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kousaie, Shanna, and Natalie A. Phillips. 2012. Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65: 356–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kovács, Ágnes Melinda, and Jacques Mehler. 2009. Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 6556–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Krassner, Ariye M., Maria A. Gartstein, Curie Park, Wojciech Ł. Dragan, Felipe Lecannelier, and Samuel P. Putnam. 2016. East–west, collectivist-individualist: A cross-cultural examination of temperament in toddlers from Chile, Poland, South Korea, and the U.S. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 14: 449–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroll, Judith F., Susan C. Bobb, and Zofia Wodniecka. 2006. Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9: 119–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroll, Judith F., Susan C. Bobb, Maya Misra, and Taomei Guo. 2008. Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica 128: 416–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroll, Judith F., Paola E. Dussias, Kinsey Bice, and Lauren Perrotti. 2015. Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics 1: 377–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kuwabara, Megumi, and Linda B. Smith. 2012. Cross-cultural differences in cognitive development: Attention to relations and objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 113: 20–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- LaFromboise, Teresa, Hardin LK Coleman, and Jennifer Gerton. 1993. Psychological impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin 114: 395–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lan, Xuezhao, Cristine H. Legare, Claire Cameron Ponitz, Su Li, and Frederick J. Morrison. 2011. Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: A cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108: 677–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, Hyo-Seon, and Seok-Ki Kim. 2014. A qualitative study on the bicultural experience of second-generation Korean immigrants in Germany. Pacific Science Review 16: 97–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Legare, Cristine H., Michael T. Dale, Sarah Y. Kim, and Gedeon O. Deák. 2018. Cultural variation in cognitive flexibility reveals diversity in the development of executive functions. Scientific Reports 8: 16326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lehtonen, Minna, Anna Soveri, Aini Laine, Janica Järvenpää, Angela De Bruin, and Jan Antfolk. 2018. Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 144: 394–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ling, Xiaoli, Li Zheng, Xiuyan Guo, Shouxin Li, Shiyu Song, Lining Sun, and Zoltan Dienes. 2018. Cross-cultural differences in implicit learning of chunks versus symmetries. Royal Society Open Science 5: 180469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lowe, Cassandra J., Isu Cho, Samantha F. Goldsmith, and J. Bruce Morton. 2021. The bilingual advantage in children’s executive functioning is not related to language status: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Science 32: 1115–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luk, Gigi, and Ellen Bialystok. 2013. Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25: 605–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luna, David, Torsten Ringberg, and Laura A. Peracchio. 2008. One individual, two identities: Frame switching among biculturals. Journal of Consumer Research 35: 279–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lunt, Laura, Jessica Bramham, Robin G. Morris, Peter R. Bullock, Richard P. Selway, Kiriakos Xenitidis, and Anthony S. David. 2012. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and ‘jumping to conclusions’: Bias or deficit? Journal of Neuropsychology 6: 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luo, Lin, Gigi Luk, and Ellen Bialystok. 2010. Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition 114: 29–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marian, Viorica, and Michael Spivey. 2003. Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 173–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markus, Hazel Rose, and Shinobu Kitayama. 1991. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review 98: 224–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markus, Hazel Rose, and Shinobu Kitayama. 2010. Cultures and Selves. Perspectives on Psychological Science 5: 420–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, Clara D., Kristof Strijkers, Mikel Santesteban, Carles Escera, Robert J. Hartsuiker, and Albert Costa. 2013. The impact of early bilingualism on controlling a language learned late: An ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin-Rhee, Michelle M., and Ellen Bialystok. 2008. The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11: 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masuda, Takahiko, and Richard E. Nisbett. 2001. Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 922–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masuda, Takahiko, Phoebe C. Ellsworth, Batja Mesquita, Janxin Leu, Shigehito Tanida, and Ellen Van de Veerdonk. 2008. Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94: 365–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meca, Alan, Raha F. Sabet, Colleen M. Farrelly, Cynthia G. Benitez, Seth J. Schwartz, Melinda Gonzales-Backen, and Elma I. Lorenzo-Blanco. 2017. Personal and cultural identity development in recently immigrated Hispanic adolescents: Links with psychosocial functioning. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 23: 348–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mezzacappa, Enrico. 2004. Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development 75: 1373–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miyake, Akira, and Naomi P. Friedman. 2012. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21: 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” Tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miyamoto, Yuri, Richard E. Nisbett, and Takahiko Masuda. 2006. Culture and the physical environment: Holistic versus analytic perceptual affordances. Psychological Science 17: 113–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morton, J. Bruce, and Sarah N. Harper. 2007. What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science 10: 719–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nayak, Srishti, Hiba Z. Salem, and Amanda R. Tarullo. 2020. Neural mechanisms of response-preparation and inhibition in bilingual and monolingual children: Lateralized Readiness Potentials (LRPs) during a nonverbal Stroop task. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 41: 100740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nguyen, Angela-MinhTu D., and Verónica Benet-Martínez. 2007. Biculturalism unpacked: Components, measurement, individual differences, and outcomes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 1: 101–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nguyen, Angela-MinhTu D., and Verónica Benet-Martínez. 2012. Biculturalism and adjustment. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44: 122–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nickels, Lyndsey, Solène Hameau, Vishnu KK Nair, Polly Barr, and Britta Biedermann. 2019. Ageing with bilingualism: Benefits and challenges. Speech, Language and Hearing 22: 32–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nisbett, Richard E., and Takahiko Masuda. 2003. Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy Of Sciences 100: 11163–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nisbett, Richard E., and Takahiko Masuda. 2006. Culture and Point of View. In Biological and Cultural Bases of Human Inference. Edited by Riccardo Viale, Daniel Andler and Lawrence A. Hirschfeld. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 49–70. [Google Scholar]
- Nisbett, Richard E., and Yuri Miyamoto. 2005. The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 467–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nisbett, Richard E., Kaiping Peng, Incheol Choi, and Ara Norenzayan. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108: 291–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Norenzayan, Ara, Edward E. Smith, Beom Jun Kim, and Richard E. Nisbett. 2002. Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science 26: 653–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oh, Seungmi, and Charlie Lewis. 2008. Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Development 79: 80–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ooi, Seok Hui, Winston D. Goh, Antonella Sorace, and Thomas H. Bak. 2018. From Bilingualism to bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21: 867–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oyserman, Daphna, and Spike WS Lee. 2008. Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134: 311–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, Kenneth R. 2019. Bilingualism in cognitive science: The characteristics and consequences of bilingual language control. In The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism. Edited by Annick De Houwer and Lourdes Ortega. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 435–65. [Google Scholar]
- Paap, Kenneth R., and Zachary I. Greenberg. 2013. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66: 232–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, Kenneth R., and Oliver Sawi. 2014. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, Kenneth R., Hunter A. Johnson, and Oliver Sawi. 2015. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 69: 265–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, Kenneth R., Regina Anders-Jefferson, Lauren Mason, Katerinne Alvarado, and Brandon Zimiga. 2018. Bilingual advantages in inhibition or selective attention: More challenges. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, Kenneth R., Regina Anders-Jefferson, Roman Mikulinsky, Shigeaki Masuda, and Lauren Mason. 2019. On the encapsulation of bilingual language control. Journal of Memory and Language 105: 76–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Padilla, Amado M. 2006. Bicultural social development. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 28: 467–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pavlenko, Aneta. 2005. Emotions and Multilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Peal, Elizabeth, and Wallace E. Lambert. 1962. The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 76: 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pelham, Sabra D., and Lise Abrams. 2014. Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40: 313–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poulin-Dubois, Diane, Agnes Blaye, Julie Coutya, and Ellen Bialystok. 2011. The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108: 567–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prior, Anat, and Brian MacWhinney. 2009. A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13: 253–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramírez-Esparza, Nairán, Samuel D. Gosling, Verónica Benet-Martínez, Jeffrey P. Potter, and James W. Pennebaker. 2006. Do bilinguals have two personalities? A special case of cultural frame switching. Journal of Research in Personality 40: 99–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sabbagh, Mark A., Fen Xu, Stephanie M. Carlson, Louis J. Moses, and Kang Lee. 2006. The development of executive functioning and theory of mind. A comparison of Chinese and U.S. Preschoolers. Psychological Science 17: 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samuel, Steven, Karen Roehr-Brackin, Hyensou Pak, and Hyunji Kim. 2018. Cultural effects rather than a bilingual advantage in cognition: A review and an empirical study. Cognitive Science 42: 2313–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sarma, U. Arathi, and Tissy Mariam Thomas. 2020. Breaking the limits of executive functions: Towards a sociocultural perspective. Culture & Psychology 26: 358–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, Seth J., Dina Birman, Verónica Benet-Martínez, and Jennifer B. Unger. 2017. Biculturalism: Negotiating multiple cultural streams. In The Oxford Handbook of Acculturation and Health. Edited by Seth J. Schwartz and Jennifer B. Unger. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 29–47. [Google Scholar]
- Shih, Margaret, and Diana T. Sanchez. 2005. Perspectives and research on the positive and negative implications of having multiple racial identities. Psychological Bulletin 131: 569–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soffietti, James P. 1960. Bilingualism and biculturalism. The Modern Language Journal 44: 275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spiegler, Olivia, and Birgit Leyendecker. 2017. Balanced cultural identities promote cognitive flexibility among immigrant children. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spivey, Michael J., and Viorica Marian. 1999. Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science 10: 281–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stroink, Mirella L., and Richard N. Lalonde. 2009. Bicultural identity conflict in second-generation Asian Canadians. The Journal of Social Psychology 149: 44–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suárez-Orozco, Carola, Marcelo M. Suárez-Orozco, and Irina Todorova. 2008. Learning a New Land: Immigrant Students in American Society. Cambridge: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tadmor, Carmit T., and Philip E. Tetlock. 2006. Biculturalism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 37: 173–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thierry, Guillaume, and Yan Jing Wu. 2007. Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 12530–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomoschuk, Brendan, Victor S. Ferreira, and Tamar H. Gollan. 2018. When a seven is not a seven: Self-ratings of bilingual language proficiency differ between and within language populations. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22: 516–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tran, Crystal D., Maria M. Arredondo, and Hanako Yoshida. 2015. Differential effects of bilingualism and culture on early attention: A longitudinal study in the U.S., Argentina, and Vietnam. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tran, Crystal D., Maria M. Arredondo, and Hanako Yoshida. 2018. Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22: 714–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treffers-Daller, Jeanine, Zehra Ongun, Julia Hofweber, and Michal Korenar. 2020. Explaining individual differences in executive functions performance in multilinguals: The impact of code-switching and alternating between multicultural identity styles. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 561088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Triandis, Harry C. 1989. The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review 96: 506–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Triandis, Harry C. 2001. Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality 69: 907–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tse, Chi-Shing, and Jeanette Altarriba. 2012. The effects of first- and second-language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: Evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15: 663–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tse, Chi-Shing, and Jeanette Altarriba. 2014. The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Valian, Virginia. 2014. Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18: 3–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varnum, Michael EW, Igor Grossmann, Shinobu Kitayama, and Richard E. Nisbett. 2010. The origin of cultural differences in cognition: Evidence for the social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Bastian, Claudia C., Alessandra S. Souza, and Miriam Gade. 2016. No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145: 246–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ward, Colleen, Caroline Ng Tseung-Wong, Agnes Szabo, Tamara Qumseya, and Uma Bhowon. 2018. Hybrid and alternating identity styles as strategies for managing multicultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 49: 1402–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weissberger, Gali H., Tamar H. Gollan, Mark W. Bondi, Daniel A. Nation, Lawrence A. Hansen, Douglas Galasko, and David P. Salmon. 2019. Neuropsychological deficit profiles, vascular risk factors, and neuropathological findings in hispanic Older adults with autopsy-confirmed Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 67: 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wiseheart, Melody, Mythili Viswanathan, and Ellen Bialystok. 2014. Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19: 141–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, Hwajin, Andree Hartanto, and Sujin Yang. 2016. The complex nature of Bilinguals’ language usage modulates task-switching outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, Sujin, and Hwajin Yang. 2016. Bilingual effects on deployment of the attention system in linguistically and culturally homogeneous children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 146: 121–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, Sujin, Hwajin Yang, and Barbara Lust. 2011. Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14: 412–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ye, Yanyan, Lei Mo, and Qihan Wu. 2017. Mixed cultural context brings out bilingual advantage on executive function. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 844–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yow, W. Quin, and Xiaoqian Li. 2015. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
“Eastern” | “Western” | |
---|---|---|
Power distance | Large | Small |
Uncertainty avoidance | High | Low |
Individualism-collectivism | Collectivism | Individualism |
Orientation | Long term | Short term |
Indugence-restraint | Restrained | Indulgence |
+ ← Maintenance of heritage culture → − | ||
− ← Cultural Adaptation → + | Integration | Assimilation |
Separation | Marginalisation |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Xie, W.; Altarriba, J.; Ng, B.C. Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship? Languages 2022, 7, 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247
Xie W, Altarriba J, Ng BC. Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship? Languages. 2022; 7(4):247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247
Chicago/Turabian StyleXie, Wenhan, Jeanette Altarriba, and Bee Chin Ng. 2022. "Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship?" Languages 7, no. 4: 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247
APA StyleXie, W., Altarriba, J., & Ng, B. C. (2022). Bilingualism, Culture, and Executive Functions: Is There a Relationship? Languages, 7(4), 247. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040247