Predicting Chinese Preschoolers’ Acquisition of Aspect Markers: A Corpus-Based Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Grammatical Aspect and Lexical Aspect in Mandarin Chinese
3. Acquisition of Chinese Aspect Markers in Early Years
- AH1, children tend to acquire past or perfective markings first and use them in association with achievements and accomplishments. Subsequently, this association extends to states and activities;
- AH2, in languages that differentiate imperfective and perfective aspects, children acquire the perfective past before the imperfective past;
- AH3, in languages that have a progressive aspect, children first use progressive aspect marking most frequently with activity verbs, then extend its use to achievements and accomplishments;
- AH4, children are less likely to use progressive aspect markings with states.
4. Development of Chinese Temporal Adverbs in Early Childhood
5. Predictors of Aspectual Development
6. The Aim of this Study
- RQ1:
- What are the developmental patterns of aspect markers in Chinese preschool children?
- RQ2:
- What interactional pattern can be observed between grammatical aspect markers and lexical aspect subclasses?
- RQ3:
- Do Chinese preschoolers produce grammatical aspect markers with temporal adverbs?
- RQ4:
- What are the predictors of aspectual development in Chinese preschoolers?
7. Method
7.1. Corpus
7.2. The Communication Task
7.3. Transcription of Data
1. | Hai shi tang -ZHE ba. |
Let’s lie down-ZHE here. |
2. | Ta ZAI zuo yi-zi. |
He is sitting down on the chair. |
7.4. Coding Scheme
7.5. Parental Report
8. Results
8.1. Early Development of Aspect Markers
8.2. Interaction Pattern of Grammatical Aspect Markers and Lexical Aspect Subclasses
3. | Na-ge xiao qi-che huai (‘broken’, achievement) -LE. |
That small car is broken. |
4. | Wo chao-cai (‘frying the food’, activity) -LE. |
I have started frying the food. |
5. | Wo chi-wan (‘finished eating’, achievement) fan -LE. |
I have finished eating. |
6. | Wo zhao-dao (‘found’, achievement)-LE zhe-ge. |
I have found this. |
7. | Wo zhao (‘looking for’, activity)-LE ban-tian. |
I have been looking for it for quite some time. |
8. | Yi en zhe-ge jiu xiang (‘making sound’, activity)-LE. |
It starts making a sound when you push this. |
9. | Dou fei-zou (‘fly away’, achievement)-LE. |
All have flown away. |
10. | Wo chao (‘fry’, activity) -GUO -LE. |
I have already fried it. |
11. | Wo ZAI da-dian-hua (‘making phone call’, activity). |
I am making phone calls. |
12. | Wo na (‘hold’, activity) -ZHE liang-ge bing-ji-ling. |
I am holding two ice-creams in my hands. |
8.3. Testing the Aspect Hypothesis (AH)
8.4. Co-Occurrence of Grammatical Aspect Markers and Temporal Adverbs
13. | wo zao-jiu (temporal adverb, ‘long ago’) ban-jia (move) -LE. |
I have moved long ago. |
14. | wo yi-jing (temporal adverb, ‘already’) zhun-bei-hao (get ready) -LE. |
I have gotten ready already. |
15. | wo gang (temporal adverb, ‘just now’) shi (test)-GUO sai-che. |
I tested the race car just now. |
8.5. Predicting Early Development of Aspect Markers
9. Discussion
9.1. The Developmental Pattern of Chinese Aspect Markers in Early Years
9.2. Interaction Patterns within and beyond the Aspectual System
9.3. Testing the Aspect Hypothesis
9.4. Interactions between Grammatical Aspect Markers and Temporal Adverbs
9.5. Predicting Chinese Aspectual Development
10. Conclusions, Limitations, and Implications
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Lexical Aspect Subclasses | Semantic Features | ||
---|---|---|---|
[±Dynamic] | [±Telicity] | [±Punctuality] | |
STA | − | − | − |
ACT | + | − | − |
ACC | + | + | − |
ACH (RVC) | + | + | + |
SMF | + | − | + |
MST-ZAI | + | + | − |
MST-ZHE/-LE | − | − | − |
Appendix B
References
- Bohnemeyer, Jürgen, and Mary Swift. 2004. Event realization and default aspect. Linguistics and Philosophy 27: 263–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cao, Dao-gen, and Ling-chun Xu. 2017. The study of genuine tenseless languages. Contemporary Linguistics 19: 93–121. [Google Scholar]
- Chang, Chien-Ju. 2004. Telling stories of experiences: Narrative development of young Chinese children. Applied Psycholinguistics 25: 83–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, Hsiang-hua. 2002. Child Acquisition of the Aspect Marker -Le in Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished Master’s dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Chang, Hsiang-hua. 2013. The aspect marker -le in early child Mandarin. Journal of Universal Language 14: 7–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chen, Ji-dong. 2017. When transparency doesn’t mean ease: Learning the meaning of resultative verb compounds in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language 44: 695–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Ji-dong, and Yasuhiro Shirai. 2010. The development of aspectual marking in child Mandarin Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics 31: 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chen, Qian-rui, and Ya Hu. 2016. The Multifunctional Model of Verbal-final Le and Sentence-final Le. Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies 180: 66–74. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Colliver, Yeshe, Amael Arguel, and Rauno Parilla. 2021. Formal literacy practices through play: Exposure to adult literacy practices increases child-led learning and interest. International Journal of Early Years Education 29: 6–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Comrie, Bernard. 1976. Aspect: An Introduction to the Study of Verbal Aspect and Related Problems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, vol. 2. [Google Scholar]
- Dale, Philip S., Maria Grazia Tosto, Marianna E. Hayiou-Thomas, and Robert Plomin. 2015. Why does parental language input style predict child language development? A twin study of gene–environment correlation. Journal of Communication Disorders 57: 106–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Deng, Xiang-jun. 2019. The Acquisition of Resultative Verb Compounds in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 47: 42–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickey, Stephen. 2016. Lexical and grammatical aspect. In The Routledge Handbook of Semantics. Edited by Nick Riemer. New York: Routledge, pp. 338–53. [Google Scholar]
- Droit-Volet, Sylvie. 2016. Development of time. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 8: 102–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erbaugh, Mary. 1978. Acquisition of temporal and aspectual distinctions in Mandarin. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 15: 30–37. Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED200006 (accessed on 18 December 2021).
- Erbaugh, Mary. 1982. Coming to Order: Natural Selection and the Origin of Syntax in the Mandarin-Speaking Child. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, The University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Erbaugh, Mary. 1992. The acquisition of Mandarin. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition. Edited by Dan Isaac Slobin. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, vol. 3, pp. 373–455. [Google Scholar]
- Field, Andy. 2013. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics. London: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Fu, Man-yi. 2002. Adverbs of Child Language. Master’s dissertation, Anhui Normal University, Wuhu, China. [Google Scholar]
- Grant, Janie Busby, and Thomas Suddendorf. 2011. Production of temporal terms by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26: 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, Chiung-chin. 2000. Temporal reference in Chinese mother–child conversation: Morphosyntactic, semantic and discourse-pragmatic resources. Journal of Child Language 27: 421–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, Chiung-chin. 2003. Mandarin temporality inference in child, maternal and adult speech. First Language 23: 147–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, Li-hong. 2018. Corpus-Based Study of the Grammaticalization and Semantic Networks of Chinese Guo and Le. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Jin, Li-min, and Henriette Hendriks. 2005. The development of aspect marking in L1 and L2 Chinese. Working Papers in English and Applied Linguistics 9: 69–99. Available online: http://arslangulthese.free.fr/page_perso_telech/jin2c_hendriks_the_development_of_aspect_marking_in_l1_and_l2_chinese.pdf (accessed on 18 December 2021).
- Justice, Laura M., Hui Jiang, and Katherine Strasser. 2018. Linguistic environment of preschool classrooms: What dimensions support children’s language growth? Early Childhood Research Quarterly 42: 79–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Hae-Young, and Eun-Hee Lee. 2006. The development of tense and aspect morphology in L2 Korean. In Frontiers of Korean Language Acquisition. Edited by Jae Jung Song. London: Saffron Books, pp. 91–126. [Google Scholar]
- Klein, Wolfgang, Ping Li, and Henriette Hendriks. 2000. Aspect and assertion in Mandarin Chinese. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 18: 723–70. [Google Scholar]
- Lau, Eva Y., Hui Li, and Nirmala Rao. 2011. Parental involvement and children’s readiness for school in China. Educational Research 53: 95–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Charles N., and Sandra A. Thompson. 1989. Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Hui, and Nirmala Rao. 2000. Parental influences on Chinese literacy development: A comparison of preschoolers in Beijing, Hong Kong, and Singapore. International Journal of Behavioral Development 24: 82–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Hui, and Shek Kam Tse. 2011. The Early Child Mandarin Corpus. Hong Kong: The University of Hong Kong. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Hui, Dandan Wu, Luyao Liang, and Mengguo Jing. 2022. Predicting Chinese and English interrogative development in a multilingual context: A corpus-based study of Singapore preschoolers. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 25: 241–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Hui, Loraine F. Corrie, and Betty Kit Mei Wong. 2008. Early teaching of Chinese literacy skills and later literacy outcomes. Early Child Development and Care 178: 441–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Ping. 1990. Aspect and Aktionsart in Child Mandarin. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Leiden, Leiden, The Netherlands. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Ping, and Melissa Bowerman. 1998. 才. First Language 18: 311–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Li, Ping, and Yasuhiro Shirai. 2011. The Acquisition of Lexical and Grammatical Aspect. New York: de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Y. 2016. Mandarin-speaking preschoolers’ early acquisition of ‘Zai’ and ‘Le’ 普通话早期儿童体标记词“在”与“了”的习得研究. Master’s thesis, Hunan University, Chashang, China. (In Chinese). [Google Scholar]
- Liang, Luyao, Dandan Wu, and Hui Li. 2019. Chinese preschoolers’ acquisition of temporal adverbs indicating past, present, and future: A corpus-based study. Journal of Child Language 46: 760–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, Jowang. 2003. Temporal reference in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 12: 259–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, Jowang. 2012. Tenselessness. In The Oxford Handbook of Tense and Aspect. Edited by R. I. Binnick. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 669–95. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, Hai-yong. 2009. The acquisition of Mandarin aspects and modals: Evidence from the acquisition of negation. Language and Linguistics 10: 133–60. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/239544939_The_Acquisition_of_Mandarin_Aspects_and_Modals_Evidence_from_the_Acquisition_of_Negation (accessed on 18 December 2021).
- Liu, Mei-chun. 2015. Tense and aspect in Mandarin Chinese. In The Oxford Handbook of Chinese Linguistics. Edited by Willian S. Y. Wang and Chaofen Sun. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 274–89. [Google Scholar]
- Ljungqvist, Marita. 2003. Aspect, Tense and Mood: Context Dependency and the Marker LE in Mandarin Chinese. Lund: Department of East Asian Languages, Lund University. [Google Scholar]
- Luk, Zoe Pei-Sui, and Yasuhiro Shirai. 2018. The development of aspectual marking in Cantonese-English bilingual children. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 56: 137–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, Jing. 2010. The Research of Time Adverbs and ‘Time Adverbs + (de) + X’ Construction in Modern Chinese 现代汉语时间副词界定及其定语用法研究. Master’s thesis, Jinan University, Guangzhou, China. (In Chinese). [Google Scholar]
- Ma, Li, Gita Martohardjono, and Willian McClure. 2019. The functional roles of lexical devices in second language learners’ encoding of temporality: A study of Mandarin Chinese-speaking ESL learners. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCormack, Teresa, and Christoph Hoerl. 2008. Temporal decentering and the development of temporal concepts. Language Learning 58: 89–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nicoladis, Elena, Yuehan Yang, and Zixia Jiang. 2020. Why jumped is so difficult: Tense/aspect marking in Mandarin-English bilingual children. Journal of Child Language 47: 1073–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niklas, Frank, and Wolfgang Schneider. 2017. Home learning environment and development of child competencies from kindergarten until the end of elementary school. Contemporary Educational Psychology 49: 263–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pawlak, Aleksandra, Jessica Oehlrich, and Richard Weist. 2006. Reference time in child English and Polish. First Language 26: 281–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, Xiaohong, and Juhong Li. 2018. Case study on Chinese children’s acquisition of sentences led by Le and Guo 说汉语儿童“了”字句与“过”字句的习得个案研究. Journal of Xingtai University 33: 166–68. (In Chinese). [Google Scholar]
- Qiu, Yinchen, and Xiaolin Zhou. 2012. Processing temporal agreement in a tenseless language: An ERP study of Mandarin Chinese. Brain Research 1446: 91–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rosemberg, Celia Renata, Florencia Alam, and Alejandra Stein. 2014. Factors reflecting children’s use of temporal terms as a function of social group. Language, Interaction and Acquisition 5: 38–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ryu, Ju Yeon, Kaoru Horie, and Yasuhiro Shirai. 2015. Acquisition of the Korean Imperfective Aspect Markers–ko iss–and–a iss–by Japanese Learners: A Multiple-Factor Account. Language Learning 65: 791–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seefeldt, Carol. 1997. Social studies in the developmentally appropriate curriculum. In Integrated Curriculum and Developmentally Appropriate Practice: Birth to Age 8. Albany: State University of New York, pp. 171–99. [Google Scholar]
- Shirai, Yasuhiro. 1998. Where the progressive and the resultative meet: Imperfective aspect in Japanese, Chinese, Korean, and English. Studies in Language. International Journal sponsored by the Foundation “Foundations of Language” 22: 661–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shirai, Yasuhiro. 2004. A multi-factor account for form-meaning connections in the acquisition of tense-aspect morphology. In Form-Meaning Connections in Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Bill VanPatten, Jessica Williams, Susanne Rott and Mark Overstreet. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 97–122. [Google Scholar]
- Shirai, Yasuhiro. 2009. Temporality in first and second language acquisition. In The Expression of Time. Edited by Wolfgang Klein and Ping Li. New York: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 167–93. [Google Scholar]
- Shirai, Yasuhiro, and Roger W. Andersen. 1995. The acquisition of tense-aspect morphology: A prototype account. Language 71: 743–62. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/415743?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents (accessed on 18 December 2021). [CrossRef]
- Slabakova, Roumyana. 2015. Acquiring temporal meanings without tense morphology: The case of L2 Mandarin Chinese. The Modern Language Journal 99: 283–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smith, Carlota S. 1991. The Parameter of Aspect. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, Carlota S. 1994. Aspectual viewpoint and situation type in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 3: 107–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stites, Lauren J., and Seyda Özçalıskan. 2013. Developmental changes in children’s comprehension and explanation of spatial metaphors for time. Journal of Child Language 40: 1123–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sun, Juan, and Cristina Grisot. 2020. Expressing Temporal Reference in Mandarin: A Quantitative Study Using Translation Corpora. Corpus Pragmatics 4: 207–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, Linlin, and Kasper Boye. 2019. The grammatical-lexical distinction in Chinese aspectual markers. Studies in Language. International Journal Sponsored by the Foundation “Foundations of Language” 43: 668–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tai, James H. Y. 1984. Verbs and times in Chinese: Vendler’s four categories. Parasession on Lexical Semantics 20: 289–96. [Google Scholar]
- Tillman, Katharine A., Tyler Marghetis, David Barner, and Mahesh Srinivasan. 2017. Today is tomorrow’s yesterday: Children’s acquisition of deictic time words. Cognitive Psychology 92: 87–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tse, Shek Kam, Hui Li, and Shing On Leung. 2012. Tense and temporality: How young children express time in Cantonese. Chinese Language and Discourse 3: 35–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vendler, Zeno. 1957. Verbs and times. The Philosophical Review 66: 143–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wang, Kun. 2017. The summary review of the studies of Chinese adverbs 汉语副词研究综述. Journal of Xihua University (Philosophy & Social Sciences) 哲学社会科学版 36: 43–51. (In Chinese). [Google Scholar]
- Weist, Richard M. 1986. Tense and aspect. In Language Acquisition. Edited by Peter Fletcher and Michael Garman. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 356–74. [Google Scholar]
- Weist, Richard M., Aleksandra Pawlak, and Jenell Carapella. 2004. Syntactic–semantic interface in the acquisition of verb morphology. Journal of Child Language 31: 31–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weist, Richard M., Paula Lyytinen, Jolanta Wysocka, and Marja Atanassova. 1997. The interaction of language and thought in children’s language acquisition: A crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language 24: 81–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wen, Xiao-Hong. 1995. Second language acquisition of the Chinese particle le. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 5: 45–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, Chi In. 2009. The Role of Adult Input on the Usage of Cantonese Aspect Markers in Young Cantonese-Speaking Children. Unpublished Bachelor’s thesis, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China. [Google Scholar]
- Wu, Ruey-Jiuan Regina. 2002. Discourse-pragmatic principles for temporal reference in Mandarin Chinese conversation. Studies in Language. International Journal Sponsored by the Foundation “Foundations of Language” 26: 513–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xiao, Zhonghua, and Anthony McEnery. 2004. A corpus-based two-level model of situation aspect. Journal of Linguistics 40: 325–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Xu, Hong-Zhi. 2019. The experiential aspect of Mandarin Chinese (-guo): Semantics and pragmatics. Lingua 229: 102714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, Hai, Xiaofei Lu, and Vaclav Brezina. 2019. Acquisition of the Chinese particle le by L2 learners: A corpus-based approach. In Computational and Corpus Approaches to Chinese Language Earning. Edited by Xiaofei Lu and Berlin Chen. Singapore: Springer, pp. 197–216. [Google Scholar]
- Xu, Yi. 2020. Perfective-le use and consciousness-raising among beginner-level Chinese learners. Languages 5: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yang, Xiaolu, Rushen Shi, and Kailin Xu. 2018. Grammatical Aspect in Early Child Mandarin: Evidence from a Preferential Looking Experiment. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 47: 1301–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yeung, S. Susanna, and Ronnel B. King. 2016. Home literacy environment and English language and literacy skills among Chinese young children who learn English as a second language. Reading Psychology 37: 92–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Anqi. 2018. On Non-Culminating Accomplishments in Mandarin. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, Guoguang. 2004. An investigation of the acquisition of Chinese time system. Applied Linguistics 4: 33–40. [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, Peng, Stephen Crain, and Likan Zhan. 2014. Grammatical aspect and event recognition in children’s online sentence comprehension. Cognition 133: 262–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Grammatical Aspect Markers | Meaning | Examples | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
ZAI | Indicates that an action or event is in progress. | 狗在吃饭. go ZAI chi-fan. dog ZAI eat. The dog is eating. | ||
ZHE | Describes situations that are viewed as enduring or continuous. | 他听着音乐. ta ting ZHE yin-yue. he listen to ZHE music. He is listening to the music. | ||
GUO | Implies the situation described was experienced at some time point prior to the speech time, and the resultative state of the event no longer obtains at the time of speech. | 他去过长城. ta qu GUO chang-cheng. he go to GUO the Great Wall. He has been to the Great Wall. | ||
LE | VF-LE | VF1 | Completion or termination of an action/event (Chen and Shirai 2010) | 他跌断了腿. ta die duan -LE tui. he fall break -LE the leg. He broke his leg. |
VF2 | Inchoative reading, indicates the beginning of a situation (Chang 2013) | 他知道了这件事. ta zhi-dao -LE zhe jian shi. he know about -LE this. He knows about this now. | ||
VF3 | Indicates the result of an anterior event/action (Chen and Hu 2016; Ljungqvist 2003) | 我洗了澡. wo xi -LE zao. I wash -LE shower. I have taken a shower. | ||
VF4 | Emphasizing the temporal relationship of two or more events (Chen and Hu 2016) | 下课了来我办公室. xia ke -LE lai wo ban-gong-shi. after class -LE come to my office. Come to my office after class. | ||
SF-LE | SF1 | Indicates change of state and signals its current relevance state (Chen and Shirai 2010). | 他已经起床了. ta yi-jing qi chuang -LE. he already get up bed -LE. He has already gotten up. | |
SF2 | Denotes an inchoative reading, highlighting the beginning of a situation (Jin and Hendriks 2005). | 系统运行了. xi tong yun-xing -LE. system operate LE. The system is now operating. | ||
VSF-LE | VSF1 | When used with ACH, RVC or ACC, it indicates the resultative state after the attainment of the goals (Chen and Shirai 2010). | 窗帘挂好了. chuang-lian gua hao -LE. the curtain hang well -LE. The curtain is now properly hung. | |
VSF2 | When used with STA or ACT, it indicates inchoative reading (Chen and Shirai 2010). | 他吃饭了. ta chi-fan -LE. he eat LE. Now he starts eating. | ||
VSF3 | Indicates a perfective reading (Chang 2013) | 刚才轮子转了. gang-cai lun-zi zhuan -LE. just now tyre turn -LE. The tyre just turned. |
Aspect Markers | Age Groups (N = 157) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
2;6 M (SD) | 3;6 M (SD) | 4;6 M (SD) | 5;6 M (SD) | |
GAs | 6.48 (1.41) | 7.15 (1.19) | 10.79 (1.22) | 18.98 (2.11) |
-LE | 5.52 (1.28) | 5.82 (1.06) | 9.33 (1.00) | 15.71 (1.73) |
-GUO | 0.15 (0.09) | 0.41 (0.18) | 0.12 (0.06) | 0.33 (0.08) |
-ZHE | 0.52 (0.20) | 0.65 (0.17) | 0.83 (0.27) | 1.94 (0.38) |
ZAI | 0.30 (0.09) | 0.26 (0.10) | 0.50 (0.17) | 1.00 (0.24) |
LAs | 6.48 (1.41) | 7.15 (1.19) | 10.79 (1.22) | 18.98 (2.11) |
States | 0.15 (0.08) | 0.26 (0.11) | 0.93 (0.21) | 1.52 (0.26) |
Activities | 1.18 (0.27) | 1.56 (0.32) | 2.86 (0.51) | 4.52 (0.63) |
Accomplishments | 0.09 (0.05) | 0.18 (0.10) | 0.31 (0.10) | 0.69 (0.19) |
Achievements | 3.18 (0.87) | 2.91 (0.53) | 3.88 (0.46) | 6.27 (0.87) |
Resultative compounds | 1.21 (0.31) | 1.59 (0.42) | 2.31 (0.31) | 4.67 (0.48) |
Semelfactives | 0.15 (0.10) | 0.00 (0.00) | 0.10 (0.08) | 0.04 (0.03) |
Mixed stative-telic verbs | 0.52 (0.19) | 0.65 (0.21) | 0.40 (0.10) | 1.27 (0.22) |
Aspect Markers | Age Groups (N = 157) | TJT a | z b | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2;6 | 3;6 | 4;6 | 5;6 | |||
GAs | 53.08 a | 59.91 a/b | 82.42 b | 107.35 c | 6491.00 *** | 6.01 |
-LE | 53.48 a | 58.49 a | 83.99 b | 106.71 c | 6497.50 *** | 6.04 |
-GUO | 4963.00 | 1.80 | ||||
-ZHE | 63.68 a | 73.32 a | 71.94 a | 99.73 b | 5710.50 *** | 3.94 |
ZAI | 73.08 | 69.19 a | 74.87 | 93.64 b | 5313.50 *** | 2.80 |
LAs | 53.08 a | 56.91 a/b | 82.42 b | 107.35 c | 6491.00 *** | 6.01 |
States | 55.45 a | 60.00 a | 85.29 b | 103.15 b | 6282.50 *** | 6.05 |
Activities | 56.15 a | 63.35 a/b | 81.37 b/c | 103.72 c | 6209.00 *** | 5.20 |
Accomplishments | 70.05 a | 72.50 | 80.12 | 88.78 b | 5232.00 *** | 2.94 |
Achievements | 60.82 a | 65.24 a | 83.42 | 97.39 b | 5878.50 *** | 4.11 |
Resultative compounds | 53.95 a | 59.60 a/b | 80.07 b | 109.02 c | 6533.50 *** | 6.24 |
Semelfactives | 4524.50 | −0.524 | ||||
Mixed stative-telic verbs | 69.86 a | 73.22 a | 69.60 a | 97.60 b | 5469.50 *** | 3.16 |
Lexical Aspect | VF | Effect Size of Pairwise Comparison | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
VF1 b | VF2 | VF3 | VF4 | χ2 | 1v3 | 2v3 | 4v3 | |
Activities | 2.52 | 2.28 | 2.82 | 2.38 | 33.08 *** | - c | −0.21 * | - |
Achievements | 2.24 | 2.02 | 3.46 | 2.28 | 118.10 *** | −0.48 *** | −0.56 *** | 0.46 *** |
VSF | Effect Size of Pairwise Comparison | |||||||
VSF1 | VSF2 | VSF3 | χ2 | 3v2 | 3v1 | 2v1 | ||
Activities | 2.06 | 2.17 | 1.77 | 35.93 *** | 0.20 ** | - | - | |
Achievements | 2.98 | 1.53 | 1.49 | 259.60 *** | - | 0.75 *** | 0.73 *** |
GA–TA Combinations | Age Groups (N = 157) | χ2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
2;6 (n = 33) | 3;6 (n = 34) | 4;6 (n = 42) | 5;6 (n = 48) | ||
Yes n(%) | 8 b (24.2) | 11 a (32.4) | 17 a (40.5) | 32 b (66.7) | 17.35 *** df = 3 Cramer’s V = 0.33 *** |
No n(%) | 25 a (75.8) | 23 a (67.6) | 25 a (59.5) | 16 a (33.3) |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Frequency of AM | 1 | ||||||
2 | Age | 0.509 ** | 1 | |||||
3 | Gender | −0.157 * | 0.007 | 1 | ||||
4 | Years of preschooling | 0.552 ** | 0.798 ** | −0.012 | 1 | |||
5 | Early learning center attendance | 0.178 * | 0.284 ** | −0.032 | 0.237 ** | 1 | ||
6 | Age of learning to read in Mandarin | 0.195 * | 0.395 * | 0.039 | 0.270 ** | 0.035 | 1 | |
7 | Age of learning to write in Mandarin | 0.188 * | 0.401 ** | 0.019 | 0.235 ** | 0.117 | 0.499 ** | 1 |
Steps | Predictors | β | R2 | ΔR2 | F |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | 0.274 | - | 25.131 *** | ||
Age | 0.498 *** | ||||
Gender | −0.156 * | ||||
Step2 | 0.276 | 0.002 | 12.501 *** | ||
Age of learning to read in Mandarin | −0.046 | ||||
Age of learning to write in Mandarin | −0.006 | ||||
Step 3 | 0.345 | 0.068 | 11.310 *** | ||
Early learning center attendance | 0.027 | ||||
Years of preschooling | 0.430 *** |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Li, H.; Liang, L.; Wu, D. Predicting Chinese Preschoolers’ Acquisition of Aspect Markers: A Corpus-Based Study. Languages 2022, 7, 133. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020133
Li H, Liang L, Wu D. Predicting Chinese Preschoolers’ Acquisition of Aspect Markers: A Corpus-Based Study. Languages. 2022; 7(2):133. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020133
Chicago/Turabian StyleLi, Hui, Luyao Liang, and Dandan Wu. 2022. "Predicting Chinese Preschoolers’ Acquisition of Aspect Markers: A Corpus-Based Study" Languages 7, no. 2: 133. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020133
APA StyleLi, H., Liang, L., & Wu, D. (2022). Predicting Chinese Preschoolers’ Acquisition of Aspect Markers: A Corpus-Based Study. Languages, 7(2), 133. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020133