Nonverbal Switching Ability of Monolingual and Bilingual Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Development and Assessment of Switching Ability
1.2. Nonverbal Switching in Bilingual Children
1.3. Nonverbal Switching in Children with DLD
1.4. The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Instruments
2.2.1. Nonverbal Switching
2.2.2. Response Inhibition and Sustained Attention
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Data Preparation
2.4.1. Nonverbal Switching
2.4.2. Response Inhibition and Sustained Attention
2.5. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Mixing Costs: Effects of DLD and Bilingualism
3.1.1. Accuracy
3.1.2. Reaction Times
3.2. Switching Costs: Effects of DLD and Bilingualism
3.2.1. Accuracy
3.2.2. Reaction Times
3.3. Mediation Effects: Mixing Costs
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | We explored the possibility of comparing the first single-task block with the second single-task block in wave 2. This still required the exclusion of 10 children. The results showed that the groups of children were not differently affected by the shift from the first to the second single-task block. |
References
- Adesope, Olusola O., Tracy Lavin, Terri Thompson, and Charles Ungerleider. 2010. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research 80: 207–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Aljahlan, Yara, and Tammie J. Spaulding. 2021. Attentional shifting in children with developmental language disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders 91: 106105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Antoniou, Kyriakos, Kleanthes K. Grohmann, Maria Kambanaros, and Napoleon Katsos. 2016. The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition 149: 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Archibald, Lisa M. D., and Susan E. Gathercole. 2006. Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 41: 675–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barac, Raluca, and Ellen Bialystok. 2012. Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development 83: 413–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barkley, Russell A. 2012. Executive Functions: What They Are, How They Work, and Why They Evolved. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Best, John R., and Patricia H. Miller. 2010. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development 81: 1641–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bialystok, Ellen, and Fergus I. M. Craik. 2010. Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Blom, Elma, and Tessel Boerma. 2016. Why do children with language impairment have difficulties with narrative macrostructure? Research in Developmental Disabilities 55: 301–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blom, Elma, Roni Berke, Nehama Shaya, and Esther Adi-Japha. 2021. Cognitive flexibility in children with Developmental Language Disorder: Drawing of nonexistent objects. Journal of Communication Disorders 93: 106137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boerma, Tessel, and Elma Blom. 2020. Effects of developmental language disorder and bilingualism on children’s executive functioning: A longitudinal study. Research in Developmental Disabilities 107: 103782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boerma, Tessel, Paul Leseman, Frank Wijnen, and Elma Blom. 2017. Language proficiency and sustained attention in monolingual and bilingual children with and without language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Borella, Erika, Barbara Carretti, and Santiago Pelegrina. 2010. The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning Disabilities 43: 541–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Botting, Nicola, and Chloë Marshall. 2017. Domain-specific and domain-general approaches to developmental disorders. In The Wiley Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology. Edited by Luna C. Centifanti and David M. Williams. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 139–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Braver, Todd S. 2012. The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 106–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Braver, Todd S., Jeremy R. Reynolds, and David I. Donaldson. 2003. Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron 39: 713–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brown, Thomas E., and Jeanne M. Landgraf. 2010. Improvements in executive function correlate with enhanced performance and functioning and health-related quality of life: Evidence from 2 large, double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trials in ADHD. Postgraduate Medicine 122: 42–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. 2021. Bevolking; Generatie, Geslacht, Leeftijd en Herkomstgroepering. Available online: www.cbs.nl (accessed on 12 April 2022).
- Conti-Ramsden, Gina, and Nicola Botting. 1999. Classification of children with specific language impairment: Longitudinal considerations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42: 1195–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cragg, Lucy, and Nicolas Chevalier. 2012. The processes underlying flexibility in childhood. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65: 209–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dajani, Dina R., and Lucina Q. Uddin. 2015. Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends in Neurosciences 38: 571–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- de Bruin, Angela, Barbara Treccani, and Sergio Della Sala. 2015. Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science 26: 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Diamond, Adele. 2013. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Druey, Michel D., and Ronald Hübner. 2008. Response inhibition under task switching: Its strength depends on the amount of task-irrelevant response activation. Psychological Research 72: 515–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Duñabeitia, Jon Andoni, Juan Andrés Hernández, Eneko Antón, Pedro Macizo, Adelina Estévez, Luis J. Fuentes, and Manuel Carreiras. 2014. The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental Psychology 61: 234–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ebert, Kerry Danahy, and Kathryn Kohnert. 2011. Sustained attention in children with primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 54: 1372–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ebert, Kerry Danahy, Diane Rak, Caitlyn M. Slawny, and Louis Fogg. 2019. Attention in bilingual children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62: 979–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Farrant, Brad M., Murray T. Maybery, and Janet Fletcher. 2012. Language, cognitive flexibility, and explicit false belief understanding: Longitudinal analysis in typical development and specific language impairment. Child Development 83: 223–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garon, Nancy, Susan E. Bryson, and Isabel M. Smith. 2008. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin 134: 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller, Enlli M. Thomas, Ivan Kennedy, Cynog Prys, Nia Young, Nestor Viñas Guasch, Emily J. Roberts, Emma K. Hughes, and Leah Jones. 2014. Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gonzalez-Barrero, Ana Maria, and Aparna S. Nadig. 2019. Can bilingualism mitigate set-shifting difficulties in children with autism spectrum disorders? Child Development 90: 1043–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, David W. 1998. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1: 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, David W., and Jubin Abutalebi. 2013. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25: 515–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunnerud, Hilde Lowell, Dieuwer ten Braak, Elin Kirsti Lie Reikerås, Enrica Donolato, and Monica Melby-Lervåg. 2020. Is bilingualism related to a cognitive advantage in children? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 146: 1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hartanto, Andree, and Hwajin Yang. 2020. The role of bilingual interactional contexts in predicting interindividual variability in executive functions: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 149: 609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayes, Andrew F. 2017. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York: Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Henry, Lucy A., David J. Messer, and Gilly Nash. 2012. Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of child Psychology and Psychiatry 53: 37–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hilchey, Matthew D., and Raymond M. Klein. 2011. Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18: 625–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huizinga, Mariëtte, and Maurits W. van der Molen. 2011. Task switching and shifting between stopping and going: Developmental change in between-trial control adjustments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108: 484–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- IBM Corp. 2016. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Version 24.0. Armonk: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
- Iluz-Cohen, Peri, and Sharon Armon-Lotem. 2013. Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 884–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Im-Bolter, Nancie. 2003. Executive Processes and Mental Attention in Children with Language Impairments. Ph.D. dissertation, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Im-Bolter, Nancie, Janice Johnson, and Juan Pascual-Leone. 2006. Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development 77: 1822–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kapa, Leah L., and Elena Plante. 2015. Executive function in SLI: Recent advances and future directions. Current Developmental Disorders Reports 2: 245–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kapa, Leah L., Elena Plante, and Kevin Doubleday. 2017. Applying an integrative framework of executive function to preschoolers with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 60: 2170–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuzyk, Olivia, Margaret Friend, Vivianne Severdija, Pascal Zesiger, and Diane Poulin-Dubois. 2020. Are there cognitive benefits of code-switching in bilingual children? A longitudinal study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 542–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Legare, Cristine H., Michael T. Dale, Sarah Y. Kim, and Gedeon O. Deák. 2018. Cultural variation in cognitive flexibility reveals diversity in the development of executive functions. Scientific Reports 8: 16326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leonard, Laurence B. 2014. Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lubin, Amélie, Emmanuelle Regrin, Laetitia Boulc’h, Sébastien Pacton, and Céline Lanoë. 2016. Executive functions differentially contribute to fourth graders’ mathematics, reading, and spelling skills. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology 15: 444–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lukács, Ágnes, Enikő Ladányi, Kata Fazekas, and Ferenc Kemény. 2016. Executive functions and the contribution of short-term memory span in children with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology 30: 296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martin-Rhee, Michelle M., and Ellen Bialystok. 2008. The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11: 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marton, Klara. 2016. Executive control in bilingual children: Factors that influence the outcomes. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6: 575–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Noterdaeme, Michele, Hedwig Amorosa, Katrin Mildenberger, Stefan Sitter, and Falk Minow. 2001. Evaluation of attention problems in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 10: 58–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, Kenneth R., Hunter A. Myuz, Regina T. Anders, Morgan F. Bockelman, Roman Mikulinsky, and Oliver M. Sawi. 2017. No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29: 89–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, Kenneth R., Regina Anders-Jefferson, Lauren Mason, Katerinne Alvarado, and Brandon Zimiga. 2018. Bilingual advantages in inhibition or selective attention: More challenges. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, Jisook, Carol A. Miller, Teenu Sanjeevan, Janet G. van Hell, Daniel J. Weiss, and Elina Mainela-Arnold. 2019. Bilingualism and attention in typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62: 4105–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pauls, Laura J., and Lisa M. D. Archibald. 2016. Executive Functions in Children With Specific Language Impairment: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 59: 1074–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peristeri, Eleni, Eleni Baldimtsi, Margreet Vogelzang, Ianthi Maria Tsimpli, and Stephanie Durrleman. 2021. The cognitive benefits of bilingualism in autism spectrum disorder: Is theory of mind boosted and by which underlying factors? Autism Research 14: 1695–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poarch, Gregory J., and Andrea Krott. 2019. A bilingual advantage? An appeal for a change in perspective and recommendations for future research. Behavioral Sciences 9: 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Prior, Anat, and Brian MacWhinney. 2010. A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13: 253–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rice, Mabel L., and Kenneth Wexler. 1996. Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 39: 1239–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rothbart, Mary K., and Michael I. Posner. 2001. Mechanism and variation in the development of attentional networks. In Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Edited by Charles A. Nelson and Monica Luciana. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 353–363. [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough, Hollis S., and Wanda Dobrich. 1990. Development of children with early language delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 33: 70–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schlichting, L. 2005. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL. Dutch version. Amsterdam: Harcourt Assessment B.V. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, Walter, Amy Eschman, and Anthony Zuccolotto. 2002. E-Prime User’s Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Smolak, Erin, Karla K. McGregor, Tim Arbisi-Kelm, and Nichole Eden. 2020. Sustained attention in developmental language disorder and its relation to working memory and language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 63: 4096–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stichting Siméa. 2014. Indicatiecriteria: Auditief en/of Communicatief Beperkte Leerlingen. Available online: http://www.simea.nl/dossiers/passend-onderwijs/brochures-po/simea-brochure-indicatiecriteria-juni-2014.pdf (accessed on 3 December 2021).
- Stichting Siméa. 2016. Handreiking Meertaligheid TOS. Available online: http://www.simea.nl/dossiers/si166289-simea-handreiking-meertaligheid-tos.pdf (accessed on 3 December 2021).
- Timmermeister, Mona, Paul Leseman, Frank Wijnen, and Elma Blom. 2020. No bilingual benefits despite relations between language switching and task switching. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 1832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomblin, J. Bruce, Nancy L. Records, Paula Buckwalter, Xuyang Zhang, Elaine Smith, and Marlea O’Brien. 1997. Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 40: 1245–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tuller, Laurice. 2015. Clinical use of parental questionnaires in multilingual contexts. In Methods for Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment. Edited by Sharon Armon-Lotem, John H. A. L. de Jong and Natalia Meir. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 301–30. [Google Scholar]
- van den Noort, Maurits, Esli Struys, Peggy Bosch, Lars Jaswetz, Benoît Perriard, Sujung Yeo, Pia Barisch, Katrien Vermeire, Sook-Hyun Lee, and Sabina Lim. 2019. Does the bilingual advantage in cognitive control exist and if so, what are its modulating factors? A systematic review. Behavioral Sciences 9: 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van der Lely, Heather K. J. 2005. Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from Grammatical-SLI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 53–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vandierendonck, André, Baptist Liefooghe, and Frederick Verbruggen. 2010. Task switching: Interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin 136: 601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verhagen, Josje, Elise de Bree, and Sharon Unsworth. 2020. Effects of bilingual language use and language proficiency on 24-month-olds’ cognitive control. Journal of Cognition and Development 21: 46–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verhoeven, Ludo, and Anne Vermeer. 2001. Taaltoets Alle Kinderen (TAK). Arnhem: Cito. [Google Scholar]
- Vissers, Constance, Sophieke Koolen, Daan Hermans, Annette Scheper, and Harry Knoors. 2015. Executive functioning in preschoolers with specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ware, Anna T., Melissa Kirkovski, and Jarrad A. G. Lum. 2020. Meta-analysis reveals a bilingual advantage that is dependent on task and age. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 1458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, David, and Jack A. Naglieri. 2008. Wechsler Nonverbal Scale of Ability. Available online: http://www.pearsonclinical.nl/wnv-nl-wechsler-non-verbal (accessed on 3 December 2021).
- Yeniad, Nihal, Maike Malda, Judi Mesman, Marinus H. Van IJzendoorn, and Suzanne Pieper. 2013. Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences 23: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelazo, Philip David. 2006. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols 1: 297–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Chronological Age in Months | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | Wave 1 | Wave 2 | Wave 3 | Nonverbal Intelligence | Socio-Economic Status b | ||
N a | Girls/Boys | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Median (Range) | |
MOTD | 32 | 14/18 | 70.9 (7.0) | 82.5 (6.9) | 94.1 (6.9) | 100.4 (11.9) | 6.8 (2–9) |
MODLD | 32 | 8/24 | 71.4 (6.3) | 82.8 (6.5) | 94.6 (6.6) | 97.5 (12.9) | 5.0 (2–8.5) |
BITD | 32 | 17/15 | 71.3 (7.3) | 83.0 (7.1) | 94.8 (7.1) | 95.8 (15.0) | 5.4 (1–9) |
BIDLD | 32 | 10/22 | 72.4 (8.6) | 83.0 (8.9) | 94.7 (8.8) | 94.7 (15.3) | 5.8 (2–9) |
PPVT | TAK Sentence Repetition | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wave 1 | Wave 2 | Wave 3 | Wave 1 | Wave 2 | Wave 3 | |
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | |
MOTD | 86.6 (10.4) | 98.5 (9.6) | 103.8 (8.9) | 28.7 (5.9) | 32.0 (5.5) | 34.3 (3.7) |
MODLD | 76.5 (9.2) | 86.6 (11.9) | 95.9 (10.1) | 10.2 (5.7) | 14.9 (6.1) | 19.9 (7.2) |
BITD | 75.8 (10.5) | 87.8 (11.5) | 96.2 (12.4) | 21.8 (7.0) | 27.7 (7.1) | 31.0 (6.3) |
BIDLD | 62.9 (13.3) | 77.8 (15.5) | 86.3 (14.0) | 9.5 (5.8) | 14.8 (7.5) | 19.0 (8.7) |
Task-Switching Block | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Single-Task Blocks | Repeat Trials | Switch Trials | |||||
N | Mean (SD) | Range | Mean (SD) | Range | Mean (SD) | Range | |
MOTD | 31 | 94.5 (4.8) | 84–100 | 85.8 (10.0) | 53–100 | 77.8 (16.8) | 31–100 |
MODLD | 31 | 93.3 (5.7) | 80–100 | 77.3 (16.2) | 43–100 | 70.8 (14.2) | 38–100 |
BITD | 31 | 91.6 (6.0) | 79–100 | 85.1 (10.0) | 58–100 | 72.4 (11.7) | 44–94 |
BIDLD | 31 | 93.4 (6.8) | 71–100 | 78.5 (17.4) | 38–100 | 69.2 (15.7) | 38–94 |
Task-Switching Block | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Single-Task Blocks | Repeat Trials | Switch Trials | |||||
N | Mean (SD) | Range | Mean (SD) | Range | Mean (SD) | Range | |
MOTD | 31 | 805.7 (354.4) | 555–1651 | 1179.7 (374.8) | 614–2719 | 1330.6 (354.4) | 836–2484 |
MODLD | 31 | 822.8 (210.9) | 543–1514 | 1233.4 (332.8) | 693–1904 | 1572.7 (435.1) | 649–2805 |
BITD | 31 | 784.2 (207.5) | 432–1170 | 1244.3 (367.1) | 547–2043 | 1560.5 (525.6) | 744–2884 |
BIDLD | 31 | 881.1 (241.7) | 544–1463 | 1361.5 (395.5) | 756–2143 | 1705.8 (498.2) | 597–2954 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Boerma, T.; van Witteloostuijn, M.; Blom, E. Nonverbal Switching Ability of Monolingual and Bilingual Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. Languages 2022, 7, 108. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020108
Boerma T, van Witteloostuijn M, Blom E. Nonverbal Switching Ability of Monolingual and Bilingual Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. Languages. 2022; 7(2):108. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020108
Chicago/Turabian StyleBoerma, Tessel, Merel van Witteloostuijn, and Elma Blom. 2022. "Nonverbal Switching Ability of Monolingual and Bilingual Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder" Languages 7, no. 2: 108. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020108
APA StyleBoerma, T., van Witteloostuijn, M., & Blom, E. (2022). Nonverbal Switching Ability of Monolingual and Bilingual Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. Languages, 7(2), 108. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020108