Next Article in Journal
The Effect of Language Contact on /tʃ/ Deaffrication in Spanish from the US–Mexico Borderland
Next Article in Special Issue
Testing the Bilingual Cognitive Advantage in Toddlers Using the Early Executive Functions Questionnaire
Previous Article in Journal
Optionality in the Expression of Indefiniteness: A Pilot Study on Piacentine
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Is Early Bilingual Experience Associated with Greater Fluid Intelligence in Adults?

1
Department of Psychology, City, University of London, London EC1V 0HB, UK
2
School of Psychology and Sport Science, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge CB1 1PT, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Languages 2022, 7(2), 100; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020100
Submission received: 13 November 2021 / Revised: 31 March 2022 / Accepted: 6 April 2022 / Published: 19 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue New Perspectives on Bilingual Cognition in Children)

Abstract

:
Emerging evidence suggests that early bilingual experience constrains the development of attentional processes in infants, and that some of these early bilingual adaptations could last into adulthood. However, it is not known whether the early adaptations in the attentional domain alter more general cognitive abilities. If they do, then we would expect that bilingual adults who learned their second language early in life would score more highly across cognitive tasks than bilingual adults who learned their second language later in life. To test this hypothesis, 170 adult participants were administered a well-established (non-verbal) measure of fluid intelligence: Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM). Fluid intelligence (the ability to solve novel reasoning problems, independent of acquired knowledge) is highly correlated with numerous cognitive abilities across development. Performance on the RAPM was greater in bilinguals than monolinguals, and greater in ‘early bilinguals’ (adults who learned their second language between 0–6 years) than ‘late bilinguals’ (adults who learned their second language after age 6 years). The groups did not significantly differ on a proxy of socioeconomic status. These results suggest that the difference in fluid intelligence between bilinguals and monolinguals is not a consequence of bilingualism per se, but of early adaptive processes. However, the finding may depend on how bilingualism is operationalized, and thus needs to be replicated with a larger sample and more detailed measures.

1. Introduction

Recent studies suggest that mere exposure to a bilingual environment can affect an infant’s attentional development. Specifically, 6-month-old infants from bilingual homes looked longer at a novel visual stimulus than infants from monolingual homes (Singh et al. 2015) and 7- to 9-month-old infants from bilingual homes redirected their visual attention faster and more frequently than infants from monolingual homes (Dal Ben et al. 2021; D’Souza et al. 2020; Kalashnikova et al. 2021). These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that infants’ attentional processes adapt to the complexity of their language environment through greater exploration of the visual environment (D’Souza and D’Souza 2021). Furthermore, by comparing adults who learned their second language early in development with adults who learned their second language later in development, D’Souza et al. (2021) found that the early bilingual adaptations in the attentional domain appear to last into adulthood. Specifically, the ‘early bilingual’ adults redirected attention faster than the ‘late bilingual’ adults, a finding which parallels the observation that infants from bilingual homes redirect attention faster than infants from monolingual homes. These data hint at the possibility that bilingual environments constrain the early development of the attentional system with long term consequences.
However, it is not known whether these early adaptations to bilingual environments in the attentional domain constrain cognitive development more generally. This is because most investigations into the relationship between bilingual language experience and cognitive abilities compared bilinguals with monolinguals1, rather than early bilinguals with late bilinguals. Moreover, the few studies that did compare early bilinguals with late bilinguals report mixed results. The first of these studies compared the two groups on inhibitory control: the ability to inhibit dominant, automatic, or prepotent responses (Luk et al. 2011). This measure was selected because it is believed that when words in one language are activated in the bilingual brain during language production, the activation of words in the other language(s) is suppressed, which means that over developmental time, the process that suppresses activation in the language domain is strengthened in the bilingual brain from regular use (Green 1998). Researchers (including Luk et al. 2011) have postulated a link between this change in activation levels during language production and the ability to consciously and deliberately override the tendency to produce a dominant or automatic response, such as the name of a colour word (Stroop 1935). Luk et al. (2011) found that early bilinguals (43 university students who acquired their second language (L2) before age 10 years) demonstrated greater inhibitory control than late bilinguals (43 university students who acquired L2 after age 10 years). This hints at a relationship between early bilingual experience and cognitive development. However, subsequent studies have either failed to replicate this finding (Humphrey and Valian 2012; Kalia et al. 2014; Kapa and Colombo 2013; Paap et al. 2014; Pelham and Abrams 2014; Tao et al. 2011) or only partially replicated it (Hartanto and Yang 2019). For example, the largest and most recent study to date found that early bilinguals (142 university students who acquired L2 before age 10 years) demonstrated greater inhibitory control than late bilinguals (54 university students who acquired L2 after age 10 years)—but only in one of several measures of inhibitory control (Hartanto and Yang 2019). The evidence in favour of a relationship between early language experience and inhibitory control is therefore limited.
Although researchers have mostly focused on group differences in inhibitory control, some have examined differences in another cognitive control process: monitoring and updating working memory representations. These studies have also yielded mixed results. For example, Paap et al. (2014) found that 91 bilinguals who had been exposed to L2 since birth performed better on a measure of monitoring than 55 bilinguals who had been exposed to L2 after age 6 years, but other studies have not found this effect (Hartanto and Yang 2019; Luk et al. 2011; Pelham and Abrams 2014; Yow and Li 2015); and Paap et al. (2014) concluded that their finding could be spurious because a group difference was only observed in one of five measures of monitoring.
Thus, the few studies that directly compared early bilinguals with late bilinguals across cognitive tasks yield inconclusive results. Yet, no study has hitherto directly compared early bilinguals with late bilinguals on a measure that may underpin many of these cognitive processes: fluid intelligence. Fluid intelligence is the ability to solve novel reasoning problems, independent of acquired knowledge. It is strongly correlated with a large number of apparently diverse cognitive variables (Salthouse et al. 2008), though it is not clear why. For some researchers (e.g., Salthouse et al. 2008), there is a single psychological capacity underpinning controlled or effortful processes, which has a lot of different labels: cognitive control, executive function, fluid intelligence, working memory. For other researchers (e.g., Friedman et al. 2011), the labels describe different—albeit similar—psychological capacities. To further complicate the issue, various theoretical frameworks exist and vie with each other to explain the same empirical data: EF unity/diversity (Friedman et al. 2008); the multiple demand system (Duncan 2010); executive/controlled attention (Engle et al. 1999a; Kane and Engle 2003); proactive control (Braver 2012; Braver et al. 2007); the supervisory attention system (Norman and Shallice 1986; Shallice and Burgess 1996).
Although there is no consensus on what the relationship is between fluid intelligence and various cognitive abilities, we suggest the relationship is not something that exists between separable functions, but rather that each function is the emerging property of dynamic interactions among diverse, interdependent, neural subsystems, not unlike how aerodynamic stability, maneuverability, and performance are the emerging properties of interactions between the concrete properties of an aeroplane (the number of blades in its turbine, the aspect and taper ratios of its wings, the shape of its fuselage, etc.) and current weather conditions. In other words, we conceptualize fluid intelligence and cognitive abilities as emerging (and sometimes overlapping) properties that arise from the collective action of the components at the level below, and which may constrain the arrangements of the components below to some purpose or function, but cannot themselves be linked to any separable concrete component. To adapt to its physical and social environment, a system (individual) must internally represent parts of the external world and actively select, maintain, and implement goals (common executive function or common EF). Some tasks may also require the individual to continuously update their goal representations in working memory. We suggest that performing these goal-directed tasks is likely to require the coordinated activity of components involved in solving novel reasoning problems (Figure 1). For example, a latent variable (common EF) was found to predict performance across nine different cognitive tasks (Friedman et al. 2008). It was also found to be related to a measure of intelligence (r = .51), as was another latent variable: updating working memory (r = .49) (Friedman et al. 2011). Fluid intelligence is important because it is not only related to adaptive processes such as learning, but also to educational, occupational, and social outcomes (Deary et al. 2007; Neisser et al. 1996).
How might early bilingual experience constrain the development of such a general cognitive ability as fluid intelligence? One possibility is that changes in fluid intelligence reflect early experience-driven adaptations to environmental complexity. There is evidence that bilingual environments are more complex than monolingual environments. For example, some bilingual parents produce single utterances that contain phonemes from different languages (Bail et al. 2015), and some produce more pronunciation errors in their non-native language (Bosch and Ramon-Casas 2011). It has been proposed that infants adapt to these more complex (language) environments by exploring (gathering more information from) their visual environment to facilitate learning (D’Souza and D’Souza 2021). These adaptations (which include redirecting visual attention faster and more frequently; D’Souza et al. 2020) may co-develop with domain-general neurocognitive structures or networks such as the ‘multiple-demand cortex’ (MDC; Duncan 2010). The MDC is not a single cortical region but rather a network of co-activating frontoparietal regions that stretch from intraparietal sulcus to middle frontal gyrus, and which are involved in functions as diverse as controlling attentional processes, maintaining goals, and selecting strategies. The MDC is especially active during tasks that require learning (Duncan and Owen 2000; Ruge and Wolfensteller 2016). For example, stimulation of MDC regions (using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation) substantially enhances word learning (Fiori et al. 2018; Sliwinska et al. 2021; Sliwinska et al. 2017). It has been argued that MDC facilitates learning through the orchestration of domain-specific networks (Chein and Schneider 2005, 2012; Uddin 2015) while also serving as an attentional modulator of tasks (Walsh et al. 1998). We therefore propose that early adaptations that engage and strengthen parts of the MDC may have indirect consequences on learning in general, and thus on tasks that measure the ability to solve novel problems, independent of acquired knowledge: i.e., fluid intelligence.
Moreover, a study found that research participants demonstrate greater performance on a widely used test of fluid intelligence (Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices; Raven et al. 2003) when the test is modified to force participants to attend to each part of the puzzle separately (Duncan et al. 2017). This hints at the possibility that adaptive visuo-attentional motor behaviour, such as redirecting attention and visual scanning (exploration), may scaffold the development of learning strategies as well as strengthen frontoparietal connectivity. This would fit with evidence that early bilingual adults are quicker at detecting change than late bilingual adults (D’Souza et al. 2021). In other words, early adaptations to complex language environments might have cascading effects on neural connectivity that scaffold a range of cognitive functions and behavioural strategies, possibly even resulting in individuals being better at solving problems by attending more to the parts of a problem. Indeed, measures of attentional scope and control during task performance are associated with measures of fluid intelligence in children and adults (Cowan et al. 2006). This raises the possibility that performance on cognitive tasks is contingent on the participant’s attentional ability to zoom in to maintain a goal in the presence of interference (e.g., when naming the colour of a colour word rather than reading the word aloud), zoom out to apprehend and retain multiple items simultaneously (e.g., when maintaining a specific configuration of visual stimuli in working memory), and to switch back and forth to identify, represent, and contrast the parts of a problem (see Cowan 1995, for discussion).
If early bilingual experience results in increased exploration and attentional control, and if attentional control co-develops with the neural structures (e.g., the MDC) through which fluid intelligence emerges, then we would expect early bilingual adults to score more highly on a fluid intelligence task than late bilingual adults. To test this hypothesis, we directly compared early bilingual adults with late bilingual adults on the well-established (non-verbal) measure of fluid intelligence: Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 212 English-speaking adult participants (18+ years) were recruited online through the university and social media, of whom 42 were excluded from analysis because they provided incomplete information regarding their linguistic status. Of the remaining 170 participants, 49% (n = 83) were women and 51% (n = 87) were men. Participants were organized by linguistic status based on commonly used criteria: individuals who acquired two or more languages by 6 years of age (early bilinguals); individuals who acquired two or more languages after 6 years of age (late bilinguals); and individuals who acquired no more than one language (monolinguals) (Hull and Vaid 2006, 2007). See Table 1 for participant characteristics by group.
The groups did not significantly differ on gender, χ 2 (2, N = 170) = 1.91, p = .385, or parental education, F2,167 = 3.04, p = .051, ω 2 = 0.02. However, because the group difference in parental education was trending, we decided to carry out post-hoc Bonferroni-adjusted t-tests to aid interpretation. Parental education was not significantly higher among early bilinguals than late bilinguals (t = 2.15, p = .100) or monolinguals (t = 2.20, p = .088). Nor was parental education significantly higher among late bilinguals than monolinguals (t = 0.11, p > .999).
Both bilingual groups were highly heterogeneous and represented a wide range of languages (n = 38), as well as a wide range of proficiency in each language, from basic to native or native-like (Table 2). Most were advanced/fluent in English (84% (n = 95)), French (48% (n = 54)), and/or Arabic (40% (n = 45)). Fifty (44%) were advanced/fluent in three or more languages. The early bilinguals had marginally more languages than the late bilinguals, t(111) = 1.93, p = .056, d = 0.37. The early bilinguals could also converse in more languages than the late bilinguals, t(111) = 2.57, p = .011, d = 0.49. The early bilinguals also had more languages they were advanced or fluent/native in than the late bilinguals, t(111) = 4.32, p < .001, d = 0.82.
As expected, the bilingual groups did not differ on first language (L1) proficiency, U = 1543.50, Z = −0.88 p = .382. This is because 99.1% of participants reported having a native-level grasp of their first language; the one exception being a late bilingual who reported having an ‘advanced’ but not ‘native-level’ grasp of the language. However, second language (L2) proficiency was higher in the early bilingual group than in the late bilingual group, U = 1056.50, Z = −3.30, p < .001. L2 proficiency in relation to L1 proficiency (i.e., L2 proficiency minus L1 proficiency) was closer to 0 in the early bilingual group than in the late bilingual group, U = 1080.00, Z = −3.16 p = .002, indicating that early bilinguals were more likely to be equally proficient in their first two languages than late bilinguals. (Mann-Whitney U tests were used to analyse ordinal data.)

2.2. Materials

Materials comprised an online background questionnaire and an online version of Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices—Set I (Raven et al. 1988). These were presented to participants via the online Gorilla platform (www.gorilla.sc; Anwyl-Irvine et al. 2020).

2.2.1. Background Questionnaire

Participants were asked questions about their demographic, parents’ education, and language background. Specifically, participants were asked to provide the following information: age; gender; parents’ level of education (rated from 1 (no qualifications) to 6 (doctoral level qualifications)); the languages they speak; and for each language, whether they acquired the language early (first six years of life) or late (6–18 years or adulthood), and whether they were fluent in the language or not (rated from 1 (basic) to 4 (fluent)).
The answer to one of these questions (parents’ level of education) was used as a proxy of socioeconomic background. Socioeconomic status (SES) is a multifaceted, complex construct (Farah 2017). It is often measured as a composite of parents’ education, occupation, and income (e.g., Li et al. 2020) and is associated with academic achievement (e.g., Gottfried et al. 2003; Sirin 2005) and intelligence (e.g., Von Stumm and Plomin 2015; White 1982). Parental education is a particularly powerful predictor of academic achievement, intelligence, and socioeconomic success. For example, a structured equation modelling study of 8- to 12-year-old children (N = 868) found that SES was related indirectly to children’s academic achievement through parental education (rather than income), specifically through the parents’ beliefs and behaviours (Davis-Kean 2005). Because there are such strong correlations between parental education, other measures of family SES, and academic achievement and intelligence, and because parental education is a greater predicator of educational achievement than other measures of SES (Liu et al. 2020), to keep the online design as short as possible, a single measure (parental education) was adopted as a proxy of SES.

2.2.2. Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices—Set I

Participants completed Set I of Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM; Raven et al. 1988). The RAPM was selected because it is a commonly used nonverbal test of fluid intelligence and proxy for general cognitive ability (Raven et al. 2003). The items comprise a series of black visual geometric designs on a white background, in the form of a 2 × 2, 3 × 3, 4 × 4, or 6 × 6 matrix. Each matrix has a single missing piece, and the participant is instructed to identify the missing piece from six to eight options. The RAPM contains 48 items, presented as one set of 12 (Set I) and another of 36 (Set II). Only Set I of the RAPM was administered to participants in the current study, because evidence suggests that Set I is a reliable and valid short form of the RAPM in the assessment of fluid intelligence (Chiesi et al. 2012) and a short online procedure helps to avoid collecting data from only persistent and determined participants. That is, a participant may be more likely to withdraw from an online study (which they can do by simply closing their browser) than a traditional lab-based one.

2.3. Procedure

Participants were provided with a link to access this online study. After providing informed consent, they were asked to fill in the background questionnaire. The participants then received instructions on how to complete the RAPM—Set I and were provided with three practice items. Once they had completed the practice items, they were allowed to proceed and complete the 12 items in Set I. Participants had unlimited time to complete each item, but were informed that their response times would be recorded.

2.4. Ethical Approval

The study received ethical approval from the School Research Ethics Panel (No. PSYPGT20_135), which was ratified by the Faculty Research Ethics Panel under the terms of Anglia Ruskin University’s Policy and Code of Practice for the Conduct of Research with Human Participants.

3. Results

3.1. Comparing Early Bilinguals with Late Bilinguals and Monolinguals

Six outliers (with scores less than 2 SD from the group mean) were identified and excluded from analysis (two from each group). A further seven outliers (with response times greater than 2 SD from the group mean) were excluded from analysis (one early bilingual, three late bilinguals, and three monolinguals). As predicted, fluid intelligence significantly differed across groups, F2,154 = 9.91, p < .001, ω 2 = 0.10 (see Figure 2). Planned contrasts were used to break down the variation due to the experimental manipulation into its component parts. The planned contrasts revealed that fluid intelligence was higher in (early and late) bilinguals (M = 9.77, SD = 1.79) than monolinguals (M = 8.33, SD = 2.92), t(154) = 4.02, p < .001, d = 0.60, and higher in early bilinguals (M = 10.30, SD = 1.36) than late bilinguals (M = 9.36, SD = 1.97), t(154) = 2.19, p = .030, d = 0.56.

3.2. Comparing Early and Late Bilinguals: A Bayesian Analysis

To further evaluate the evidence, a Bayesian analysis was carried out (van Doorn et al. 2021). We focus on the critical difference reported in the main document between early bilinguals and late bilinguals. The null hypothesis (H0) postulates that there is no difference in fluid intelligence between early and late bilinguals and therefore δ should equal 0. The one-sided alternative hypothesis (H+) states that only positive values of δ are possible and assigns more prior mass to values closer to 0 than extreme values. Specifically, δ was assigned a Cauchy prior distribution with r = 1/ 2 , trunctuated to allow only positive effect size values. Figure A1 (the prior and posterior plot) shows that the Bayes factor (BF+0) indicates evidence for H+; specifically, BF+0 = 12.15, which means that the data are approximately 12 times more likely to occur under H+ than H0. This result indicates “moderate” to “strong” evidence in favour of H+ (Kass and Raftery 1995). The error percentage is < 0.001%, which indicates great stability of the numerical algorithm that was used to obtain the result. In order to assess the robustness of the Bayes factor to our prior specification, Figure A2 shows BF+0 as a function of the prior width r. Across a range of widths, the Bayes factor appears to be relatively stable, ranging from 8.26 to 13.21.
We also report the results for parameter estimation. Of interest is the posterior distribution of the standardized effect size δ (i.e., the population version of Cohen’s d, the standardized difference in mean fluid intelligence). For parameter estimation, δ was assigned a Cauchy prior distribution with r = 1/ 2 . Figure A1 shows that the median of the resulting posterior distribution of δ equals 0.50—albeit with a central 95% credible interval for δ that ranges from 0.13 to 0.89.
In summary, we hypothesised that fluid intelligence would be higher in early bilinguals than late bilinguals. A Bayesian version of the independent t-test suggests that the evidence provided by the data are about 12:1 in favour of our hypothesis, BF+0 = 12.15, error = ~6.62 × 10−6. According to Jeffreys (1961), Kass and Raftery (1995), and Raftery (1995), these data provide “moderate” to “strong” evidence in favour of the alternative hypothesis.

3.3. Exploratory Analyses

To probe further, we carried out an additional set of (exploratory) analyses. Because these analyses are exploratory, they should be interpreted with extra caution. First, we directly compared late bilinguals with monolinguals. Fluid intelligence was greater in late bilinguals than monolinguals, t(109) = 2.20, p = .030, d = 0.42 (two-tailed).
Second, because the early bilinguals had more languages than the late bilinguals, we decided to see whether the number or proficiency of languages the bilinguals could speak correlates with fluid intelligence. Neither the number of languages, nor the number of advanced/fluent/native languages, nor second language proficiency, nor second language proficiency in relation to first language proficiency correlated with fluid intelligence, r = .02, p = .821, r = −.11, p = .269, τ = −.08, p = .333, τ = −.10, p = .238, respectively (Kendall’s tau was used for ordinal data).
Finally, because bilingual experience was so variable, we decided to see what would happen if we used a different definition of bilingualism. If we re-categorize bilinguals as individuals who are advanced or fluent/native in 2 or more languages, then, excluding 10 outliers (3 early bilinguals, 1 late bilingual, and 6 monolinguals) for having scores less than 2 SD from their group mean, and 5 outliers (2 early bilinguals, 1 late bilingual, 2 monolinguals) for taking too long to provide their answers (+2 SD), we no longer find a significant main effect of group, F2,152 = 2.49, p = .086. If the scores of all participants are included (excluding the five participants who took too long), then there is an effect of group, F2,162 = 4.73, p = .010, but only between bilinguals and monolinguals (p = .003) and not between early bilinguals and late bilinguals (p = .305). We obtain a similar result if we include the five participants who were too slow: F2,167 = 5.95, p = .003; bilinguals vs. monolinguals, p = .002; early bilinguals vs. late bilinguals, p = .155. Therefore, the difference between early and late bilinguals varies according to how they are categorized.

4. Discussion

This study was the first to directly compare early bilinguals with late bilinguals on a commonly used measure of fluid intelligence. We found that fluid intelligence was greater in early bilinguals (adults who learned their second language between 0 and 6 years) than late bilinguals (adults who learned their second language after age 6 years). The groups did not significantly differ on a proxy of socioeconomic status (parental education). These data suggest that the difference in fluid intelligence between the bilinguals and monolinguals was not a result of bilingualism per se, but of early adaptive processes. However, this result must be interpreted with caution because it could not be replicated when bilingualism was operationalized differently. A large replication study is needed.
If the main result is replicated in a larger study, it will be important to ask the following question: How could early cognitive development be constrained by bilingual experience? One suggestion is that bilingual language processing requires (and thus strengthens) domain-general cognitive control processes. For example, 20-month-old bilingual toddlers demonstrate a processing cost when they hear a language switch within a sentence (e.g., “Look! Find the chien!”) (Byers-Heinlein et al. 2017). The researchers interpreted this switch cost to mean that the toddlers were monitoring and controlling their languages during language listening, and that monitoring, controlling, and switching between two languages during comprehension “trains information processing beyond the domain of language” (Byers-Heinlein et al. 2017, p. 9035). However, there are different ways of interpreting the data—and most importantly, there remains a large explanatory gap between what may be a language-specific mechanism and a domain-general cognitive control process. For example, switching between languages in the temporal domain may require different mechanisms from those necessary for switching between visual stimuli in the spatial domain. In addition, the processing cost was only observed when the switch occurred within a single sentence, not between sentences. For this reason, the data can only suggest a link between cognitive control and a language environment where switching within sentences (intra-sentential code switching) is common. However, a study of 10- and 18-month-olds in Montreal, Canada, found low rates of infant-directed code-switching (0.6–2.8 times per 100 words), and most of these (77–83%) were instances of inter-sentential code switching, not intra-sentential code switching (Kremin et al. 2021). A more parsimonious explanation for why there is a switch cost in 20-month-old toddlers is that the brain actively adapts to its environment in a cost-efficient way by automatically minimizing the gap between prior expectations and sensory input (Friston 2009, 2010). Involvement of a higher-level (domain-general) cognitive control process is possible but not necessary.
Alternatively, because infants raised in bilingual environments are likely to receive fewer words from each language, and more varied, noisier, and less predictable speech sounds, they may sample (explore) more of their environment than infants exposed to just one language (D’Souza and D’Souza 2021). For example, visual information (such as lip movements) is known to facilitate the discrimination of speech sounds, so infants in more complex (language) environments may use visual information for a longer period of developmental time than those in less complex environments, which may result in them rapidly and more frequently switching attention between the objects they are exploring and speakers’ lip movements. This kind of activity would be supported by the mobilisation—and thus refinement—of neurocognitive mechanisms that enable rapid disengaging and attention switching. Indeed, this is what was found in another study: infants raised in bilingual homes were faster at disengaging attention from one visual stimulus in order to shift attention to another visual stimulus, and they switched attention more frequently between visual stimuli, than infants raised in monolingual homes (D’Souza et al. 2020).
How might these early bilingual adaptations constrain the development of fluid intelligence specifically? The neurocognitive mechanisms that enable rapid disengaging and attention switching comprise long-range white matter tracts that connect and facilitate coordinated activity among multiple neural systems in the parietal lobe (e.g., intraparietal cortex, superior parietal lobe, and precuneus) and frontal lobe (e.g., anterior cingulate cortex, presupplementary and supplementary motor areas, and middle frontal gyrus) (Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Corbetta et al. 2008). Most of these neural systems have also been associated with fluid intelligence (Cocchi et al. 2014; Cole et al. 2015; Duncan 1995; Fedorenko et al. 2013; Gray et al. 2003; Tamnes et al. 2010; Ullén et al. 2008; Wendelken et al. 2017; see Basten et al. 2015, for meta-analysis). For example, spontaneous activity in the right middle frontal gyrus correlates with two functionally segregated attentional systems: the dorsal and ventral frontoparietal networks (Corbetta et al. 2008). The dorsal network appears to bias or filter activity in the ventral network, ensuring that the ventral network processes only behaviourally-important information, while the ventral network reorients the dorsal network to unexpected but behaviourally-important stimuli. These interactions happen via the right middle frontal gyrus (Fox et al. 2006). Damage to this region disrupts performance on tasks that require participants to reorient attention (Japee et al. 2015). This suggests that the right middle frontal gyrus plays an important role in attention. However, it also plays an important role in fluid intelligence. In one Magnetic Resonance Imaging study, 104 young adults were administered 21 different cognitive tasks, including Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (Colom et al. 2013). Confirmatory factor analysis found significant overlap between the right middle frontal gyrus (cortical grey matter volume, cortical surface area) and fluid intelligence.
The connection between attention and fluid intelligence is not surprising, since both involve building, monitoring, and maintaining behaviourally-important goal representations in the prefrontal cortex (Duncan 2010). Early adaptations in the attentional system may therefore shape the emergence and development of fluid intelligence through increased long-range frontoparietal connectivity. This would explain why fractional anisotropy in white matter fibre tracts that link parieto-occipital regions with premotor and prefrontal areas is greater in children who were exposed to two languages before age 3 years than age 3–5 years (Mohades et al. 2012).
Furthermore, fluid intelligence is believed to reflect processing speed (Salthouse 1996) and/or working memory capacity (Conway et al. 2002; Engle et al. 1999b; Kyllonen and Christal 1990). Performance on tests of fluid intelligence and processing speed is correlated with prefrontal white matter volume (Ullén et al. 2008), which in turn appears important for the synchronization of cortical neural activity (Traub et al. 2004) and a wide range of cognitive functions, including attention and working memory (e.g., Fries 2005; Singer 1999; Uhlhaas and Singer 2006). Adaptations in attention may therefore lead to greater prefrontal connectivity that results in more temporally stable neural activity, which may affect other cognitive functions dependent on neural synchrony in the millisecond range. This is consistent with evidence of greater structural and functional connectivity in the frontoparietal control network in bilingual adults than monolingual adults (Grady et al. 2015), and more importantly, greater in early bilingual adults than late bilingual adults (Berken et al. 2015, 2016).
Greater attentional control may also lead to improvements in fluid intelligence via working memory. Although working memory has often been described as a system that comprises a supervisory subsystem that directs attention to relevant information, suppresses irrelevant information, and controls three subordinate subsystems which maintain representations (Baddeley and Hitch 1974), more recent models suggest that working memory refers to representations in long-term memory that have become activated through attentional processes, up to four of which may be called the ‘focus of attention’ (Cowan 1995, 1999, 2010). In other words, working memory and attention are not necessarily separate subsystems but different aspects (states) of the same underlying neurocognitive system; we select and maintain items in working memory by actively attending to them. If early bilingual adaptations lead to greater connectivity within attentional networks, it would explain why bilinguals perform particularly well on working memory tasks that require greater attentional control, such as complex span tasks (Linck et al. 2014).
Fluid intelligence may also reflect the ability to direct attention to the simpler parts of a complex task (Duncan et al. 2017). If this is the case, then it is possible that early bilingual experience biases the visuo-attentional system in a way that supports the development of learning strategies as well as strengthens frontoparietal connectivity. That is, performance on tasks of fluid intelligence may rely on the ability to zoom in and out or switch back and forth between the different aspects of a single representation or problem. This attentional flexibility could mean that bilinguals are more likely to find the solution to a task of fluid intelligence, and may explain how early bilingual adults were better at detecting minute gradual changes in a visual stimulus than late bilingual adults (D’Souza et al. 2021).
Assuming that early bilingual experience is related to fluid intelligence, then given that fluid intelligence is also correlated with general cognitive ability, why do comparisons between early bilinguals and late bilinguals (or even between bilinguals and monolinguals) on cognitive performance report mixed results (e.g., Donnelly et al. 2019)? Although attention, fluid intelligence, and cognitive abilities overlap, they are still functionally separate. Attention is the ability to select goal-relevant sensory information, monitor information, and redirect action; fluid intelligence is the ability to recognize patterns, engage in abstract thinking, and solve novel problems; and while cognitive abilities include problem solving, they also include planning, controlling, and regulating the flow of information processing more generally to support goal-oriented behaviour, including the capacity to draw on past experience and knowledge to comprehend situations, figure out what is needed, decide on and plan new courses of action, and override automatic thoughts and behaviours. Although all three functional clusters rely on many of the same neural regions, adaptive demands differ across time and place, and thus, the neurocognitive processes adapt to the environment not by activating the same neural regions but through the timely coordination of different neural populations. It is therefore possible that some cognitive tasks require long-range connectivity between parietal and frontal regions, while others rely more on specific neural systems in prefrontal cortex.
Moreover, there is evidence of a gradual differentiation of cognitive ability, with a transition from a single non-specific function early in life to diverse and increasingly specialized functions by adulthood. For example, confirmatory factor analyses often identify a ‘mental set-shifting’ factor in adults, but not in young children (see Karr et al. 2018, for review). These findings are consistent with the observation that grey matter in regions associated with mental set-shifting (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) is pruned after grey matter in regions more associated with general cognitive ability (e.g., ventral frontal areas) (Gogtay et al. 2004). This suggests that early adaptations in frontoparietal connectivity may have broad implications for fluid intelligence but not necessarily for specific cognitive abilities such as mental set-shifting.
In summary, we propose that early bilingual adaptations involve mechanisms that enable rapid disengaging and attention switching and that these mechanisms are likely to comprise white matter tracts and functional connectivity among diverse frontal and parietal cortical regions. This ‘attentional network’ is unlikely to be an independent system. It is more likely to involve interdependent processes that overlap with other neurocognitive processes and which have cascading effects on how fast information is processed, how representations are selected, maintained, and updated, and how complex tasks are solved.

Limitations and Future Research

Although our data fit with findings in the wider literature, our study needs to be replicated by larger studies that include larger samples and more detailed measures of fluid intelligence, language experience, and socioeconomic background. For example, rather than ask participants to tick whether they acquired the language early (first six years of life) or late (6–18 years or adulthood), we could ask them to state when they acquired each language. The need to replicate our results with more detailed measures of language experience is particularly evident given the fact that the effect was not observed when we re-analysed the data using a different criterion for bilingualism. This raises the question of precisely what kind of bilingual experience leads to greater fluid intelligence? A larger sample would also permit us to analyse typological similarity and other potentially important factors (Antoniou and Wright 2017). A larger study could also factor in the time dimension by longitudinally relating infant and home environment data to later measures of language experience, home/school environment, and fluid intelligence. This could pave the way to studies that elucidate causal mechanisms.
If the current study is replicated on larger scales, with larger samples and more detailed measures, and fluid intelligence is found to be greater in early bilinguals than late bilinguals (as the current study hints at), then it will pave the way to further studies on the relationship between early bilingual experience and fluid intelligence. Why has bilingual research not hitherto focused on fluid intelligence? We suspect that the reason is due to the popularity of the inhibitory control hypothesis (Green 1998) as an explanation for findings in the ‘bilingual advantage’ literature. As mentioned in the Introduction, the automatic process through which one language is selected for use during language production has been linked, via an inhibitory control mechanism, to the process through which a research participant controls their motor behaviour in tasks that require them to consciously and deliberately override an automatic, dominant, or prepotent response to an external stimulus. This argument requires more detail, however, because even if an inhibitory control mechanism is used2, there exists a wide range of inhibitory control mechanisms across multiple spatial and temporal scales, from the inhibition of dendritic growth to the regulation of neuronal oscillatory activity. They also serve a wide range of functions, from generating complex properties (e.g., non-linearity) in cortical circuits to preventing involuntary motor movements (Buzsaki 2006). A diverse range of inhibitory mechanisms may even be observed within the same domain and modality. For example, ‘inhibition of return’ and the ‘inhibitory surround’ mechanism are two very different processes in the low-level domain of visual attention, and ‘prepotent response inhibition’, ‘resistance to distractor interference’, and ‘resistance to proactive interference’ appear to be dissociable functions in the higher-level domain of cognitive control (Friedman and Miyake 2004). Even within the same cortical regions, different inhibitory neural networks coexist with each other: lateral inhibition divides, isolates, and suppresses neural activity; negative (inhibitory) feedback provides stability; and feedforward inhibition filters afferent excitation or increases the temporal precision of neural firing (Buzsaki 2006). Moreover, even seemingly single inhibitory processes may be subserved by different coalitions of neural activity. For example, performance across four similar inhibitory control (Stroop-like interference) tasks was found to be uncorrelated (Shilling et al. 2002; see also Borragan et al. 2018; Cipolotti et al. 2016). This diversity in inhibitory mechanisms precludes the possibility of any straightforward link between the automatic modulation of activation levels in the language domain and deliberate motor control. The two processes are conceptually separate. More importantly, the inhibitory control hypothesis fails to explain why mere exposure to two or more languages leads preverbal infants (who do not select and produce language) to redirect visual attention faster and more frequently than infants exposed to just one language (see D’Souza and D’Souza 2016, 2021, for discussion). Future research should therefore seek to elucidate the putative link between the process through which one language is selected for use during language production and the process through which a participant controls their motor behaviour.
Future research should also establish when in development the inhibitory mechanism constrains cognition and behaviour. Indeed, there is evidence that greater inhibitory control is more likely to be found in late bilinguals than early bilinguals (Donnelly et al. 2019). In addition, whereas inhibitory control is often greater in late bilinguals than monolinguals, processing speed is often greater in early bilinguals than monolinguals (Donnelly et al. 2019). This is consistent with our hypothesis that early bilingual experience strengthens long range connectivity within the attentional network, while inhibitory control may play a role later in development.

5. Conclusions

Fluid intelligence was greater in bilinguals than monolinguals, and greater in early bilinguals than late bilinguals. This suggests that the difference in fluid intelligence was not a result of bilingualism per se, but of early adaptive processes. We hypothesise that early bilingual experience leads to adaptations that enable rapid disengaging and attention switching, and that these adaptations are likely to comprise increased functional connectivity among diverse frontal and parietal cortical regions that are implicated in a wide range of higher-level functions including fluid intelligence. However, our finding needs to be replicated with a larger sample and more detailed measures, because early and late bilinguals did not differ when bilingualism was operationalized differently.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.D.; Formal analysis, D.D.; Investigation, Y.D.; Methodology, Y.D.; Supervision, D.D.; Visualization, D.D.; Writing—original draft, D.D.; Writing—review & editing, D.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the School Research Ethics Committee of Anglia Ruskin University (protocol code No. PSYPGT20_135) on 16 December 2020.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are openly available in OSF at osf.io/7m8bk.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Hana D’Souza (Newnham College, University of Cambridge) for her comments on the first draft.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Figure A1. A Bayes factor of 12.15 suggests that the evidence provided by the data are about 12:1 in favour of the alternative hypothesis (the red part of the pie chart) that early bilinguals would demonstrate greater fluid intelligence than late bilinguals. A Bayes factor of 1 indicates that both hypotheses (alternative and null) predicted the data equally well. This prior posterior plot shows the prior (dashed line) and posterior (full line). The two gray dots indicate the prior and posterior density at the test value. The median and 95% central credible interval of the posterior distribution are shown in the top right corner. The pie chart (“probability wheel”) at the top visualises the evidence that the data provide for the two rival hypotheses (red = alternative hypothesis; white = null hypothesis).
Figure A1. A Bayes factor of 12.15 suggests that the evidence provided by the data are about 12:1 in favour of the alternative hypothesis (the red part of the pie chart) that early bilinguals would demonstrate greater fluid intelligence than late bilinguals. A Bayes factor of 1 indicates that both hypotheses (alternative and null) predicted the data equally well. This prior posterior plot shows the prior (dashed line) and posterior (full line). The two gray dots indicate the prior and posterior density at the test value. The median and 95% central credible interval of the posterior distribution are shown in the top right corner. The pie chart (“probability wheel”) at the top visualises the evidence that the data provide for the two rival hypotheses (red = alternative hypothesis; white = null hypothesis).
Languages 07 00100 g0a1
Figure A2. The Bayes factor robustness check. The maximum Bayes factor (BF+0) is attained when setting the prior width r to 0.43. The plot indicates BF+0 for the user specified prior (r = 1/ 2 ), wide prior (r = 1), and ultrawide prior (r = 2 ). The evidence for the alternative hypothesis is somewhat stable across a wide range of prior distributions, suggesting that the analysis is robust.
Figure A2. The Bayes factor robustness check. The maximum Bayes factor (BF+0) is attained when setting the prior width r to 0.43. The plot indicates BF+0 for the user specified prior (r = 1/ 2 ), wide prior (r = 1), and ultrawide prior (r = 2 ). The evidence for the alternative hypothesis is somewhat stable across a wide range of prior distributions, suggesting that the analysis is robust.
Languages 07 00100 g0a2

Notes

1
A recent review and meta-analysis of 1194 effect sizes from studies that included 10,937 bilinguals and 12,477 monolinguals between 3 and 17 years of age found no effect of language status (bilingual vs. monolingual) on cognitive ability, after accounting for moderating factors and publication bias (Lowe et al. 2021). The meta-analysis did not compare early bilinguals with late bilinguals.
2
Although it is possible that a dedicated control system inhibits the activation of words in the non-target language (Green 1998), such a system is not necessary. It is possible that greater activation of task-relevant words attenuates activation of task-irrelevant words. This could be achieved through simple lateral inhibition mechanisms, whereby the synchronous activation of one neural population automatically disrupts the synchronous activation of nearby populations (Buzsaki 2006). These lateral inhibition mechanisms are ubiquitous in the human brain and may help to prevent task-irrelevant interference (Buzsaki 2006). In other words, control processes and attentional processes may not be two independent systems but two aspects of one complex process involving highly diverse, interconnected, and interdependent neurons.

References

  1. Antoniou, Mark, and Sarah M. Wright. 2017. Uncovering the mechanisms responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Frontiers in Psychology 15: 2217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Anwyl-Irvine, Alexander L., Jessica Massonnié, Adam Flitton, Natasha Kirkham, and Jo K. Evershed. 2020. Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods 52: 388–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Baddeley, Alan D., and Graham Hitch. 1974. Working memory. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. New York: Academic Press, vol. 8, pp. 47–89. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bail, Amelie, Giovanna Morini, and Rochelle S. Newma. 2015. Look at the gato! Code-switching in speech to toddlers. Journal of Child Language 42: 1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Basten, Ulrike, Kirsten Hilger, and Christian J. Fiebach. 2015. Where smart brains are different: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional and structural brain imaging studies on intelligence. Intelligence 51: 10–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Berken, Jonathan A., Vincent L. Gracco, Jen-Kai Chen, and Denise Klein. 2015. The timing of language learning shapes brain structure associated with articulation. Brain Structure and Function 221: 3591–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Berken, Jonathan A., Xiaoqian Chai, Jen-Kai Chen, Vincent L. Gracco, and Denise Klein. 2016. Effects of early and late bilingualism on resting-state functional connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience 36: 1165–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Borragan, Maria, Clara D. Martin, Angela De Bruin, and Jon Andoni Duñabeitia. 2018. Exploring different types of inhibition during bilingual language production. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 2256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bosch, Laura, and Marta Ramon-Casas. 2011. Variability in vowel production by bilingual speakers: Can input properties hinder the early stabilization of contrastive categories? Journal of Phonetics 39: 514–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Braver, Todd S. 2012. The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 106–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Braver, Todd S., Jeremy R. Gray, and Gregory C. Burgess. 2007. Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. Variation in Working Memory 75: 106. [Google Scholar]
  12. Buzsaki, Gyorgy. 2006. Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  13. Byers-Heinlein, Krista, Elizabeth Morin-Lessard, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2017. Bilingual infants control their languages as they listen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114: 9032–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Chein, Jason M., and Walter Schneider. 2005. Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. Cognitive Brain Research 25: 607–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Chein, Jason M., and Walter Schneider. 2012. The brain’s learning and control architecture. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21: 78–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Chiesi, Francesca, Matteo Ciancaleoni, Silvia Galli, and Caterina Primi. 2012. Using the Advanced Progressive Matrices (Set I) to assess fluid ability in a short time frame: An item response theory–based analysis. Psychological Assessment 24: 892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Cipolotti, Lisa, Barbara Spanò, Colm Healy, Carina Tudor-Sfetea, Edgar Chan, Mark White, Francesca Biondo, John Duncan, Tim Shallice, and Marco Bozzali. 2016. Inhibition processes are dissociable and lateralized in human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 93: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Cocchi, Luca, Graeme S. Halford, Andrew Zalesky, Ian H. Harding, Brentyn J. Ramm, Tim Cutmore, David HK Shum, and Jason B. Mattingley. 2014. Complexity in relational processing predicts changes in functional brain network dynamics. Cerebral Cortex 24: 2283–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Cole, Michael W., Takuya Ito, and Todd S. Braver. 2015. Lateral prefrontal cortex contributes to fluid intelligence through multinetwork connectivity. Brain Connectivity 5: 497–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Colom, Roberto, Miguel Burgaleta, Francisco J. Román, Sherif Karama, Juan Álvarez-Linera, Francisco J. Abad, Kenia Martínez, Ma Ángeles Quiroga, and Richard J. Haier. 2013. Neuroanatomic overlap between intelligence and cognitive factors: Morphometry methods provide support for the key role of the frontal lobes. Neuroimage 72: 143–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Conway, Andrew R. A., Nelson Cowan, Michael F. Bunting, David J. Therriault, and Scott R. B. Minkoff. 2002. A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence 30: 163–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Corbetta, Maurizio, and Gordon L. Shulman. 2002. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3: 201–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Corbetta, Maurizio, Gaurav Patel, and Gordon L. Shulman. 2008. The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58: 306–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Cowan, Nelson. 1995. Attention and Memory: An Integrative Framework. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  25. Cowan, Nelson. 1999. An embedded-processes model of working memory. In Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. Edited by Akira Miyake and Priti Shah. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 62–101. [Google Scholar]
  26. Cowan, Nelson. 2010. The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Cowan, Nelson, Nathanael M. Fristoe, Emily M. Elliott, Ryan P. Brunner, and J. Scott Saults. 2006. Scope of attention, control of attention, and intelligence in children and adults. Memory & Cognition 34: 1754–68. [Google Scholar]
  28. D’Souza, Dean, and Hana D’Souza. 2016. Bilingual language control mechanisms in anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A developmental perspective. Journal of Neuroscience 36: 5434–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. D’Souza, Dean, and Hana D’Souza. 2021. Bilingual adaptations in early development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 25: 727–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. D’Souza, Dean, Dan Brady, Jennifer X. Haensel, and Hana D’Souza. 2020. Is mere exposure enough? The effects of bilingual environments on infant cognitive development. Royal Society Open Science 7: 180191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. D’Souza, Dean, Dan Brady, Jennifer X. Haensel, and Hana D’Souza. 2021. Early bilingual experience is associated with change detection ability in adults. Scientific Reports 11: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Dal Ben, Rodrigo, Hilary Killam, Sadaf Pour Iliaei, and Krista Byers-Heinlein. 2021. Bilingualism Affects Infant Cognition: Insights from New and Open Data. PsyArXiv, December 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Davis-Kean, Pamela E. 2005. The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: The indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment. Journal of Family Psychology 19: 294–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Deary, Ian J., Steve Strand, Pauline Smith, and Cres Fernandes. 2007. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence 35: 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Donnelly, Seamus, Patricia J. Brooks, and Bruce D. Homer. 2019. Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26: 1122–47. [Google Scholar]
  36. Duncan, John. 1995. Attention, intelligence and the frontal lobes. In The Cognitive Neuroscience. Edited by Michael S. Gazzaniga. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 721–33. [Google Scholar]
  37. Duncan, John. 2010. The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: Mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 172–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Duncan, John, and Adrian M. Owen. 2000. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences 23: 475–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Duncan, John, Daphne Chylinski, Daniel J. Mitchell, and Apoorva Bhandari. 2017. Complexity and compositionality in fluid intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114: 5295–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Engle, Randall W., Michael J. Kane, and Stephen W. Tuholski. 1999a. Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control 4: 102–34. [Google Scholar]
  41. Engle, Randall W., Stephen W. Tuholski, James E. Laughlin, and Andrew RA Conway. 1999b. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128: 309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Farah, Martha J. 2017. The neuroscience of socioeconomic status: Correlates, causes, and consequences. Neuron 96: 56–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Fedorenko, Evelina, John Duncan, and Nancy Kanwisher. 2013. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110: 16616–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Fiori, Valentina, Lisa Kunz, Philipp Kuhnke, Paola Marangolo, and Gesa Hartwigsen. 2018. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) facilitates verb learning by altering effective connectivity in the healthy brain. NeuroImage 181: 550–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Fox, Michael D., Maurizio Corbetta, Abraham Z. Snyder, Justin L. Vincent, and Marcus E. Raichle. 2006. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103: 10046–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Friedman, Naomi P., and Akira Miyake. 2004. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133: 101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Friedman, Naomi P., Akira Miyake, Susan E. Young, John C. DeFries, Robin P. Corley, and John K. Hewitt. 2008. Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137: 201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Friedman, Naomi P., Akira Miyake, JoAnn L. Robinson, and John K. Hewitt. 2011. Developmental trajectories in toddlers’ self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: A behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology 47: 1410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Fries, Pascal. 2005. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 474–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Friston, Karl. 2009. The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 293–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Friston, Karl. 2010. The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 127–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Gogtay, Nitin, Jay N. Giedd, Leslie Lusk, Kiralee M. Hayashi, Deanna Greenstein, A. Catherine Vaituzis, Tom F. Nugent, David H. Herman, Liv S. Clasen, Arthur W. Toga, and et al. 2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101: 8174–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Gottfried, Allen W., Adele E. Gottfried, Kay Bathurst, Diana Wright Guerin, and Makeba M. Parramore. 2003. Socioeconomic Status in Children’s Development and Family Environment: Infancy through Adolescence. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  54. Grady, Cheryl L., Gigi Luk, Fergus I. M. Craik, and Ellen Bialystok. 2015. Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia 66: 170–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Gray, Jeremy R., Christopher F. Chabris, and Todd S. Braver. 2003. Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience 6: 316–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Green, David W. 1998. Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1: 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Hartanto, Andree, and Hwajin Yang. 2019. Does early active bilingualism enhance inhibitory control and monitoring? A propensity-matching analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 45: 360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Hull, Rachel, and Jyotsna Vaid. 2006. Laterality and language experience. Laterality 11: 436–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Hull, Rachel, and Jyotsna Vaid. 2007. Bilingual language lateralization: A meta-analytic tale of two hemispheres. Neuropsychologia 45: 1987–2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Humphrey, A. D., and Virginia Valian. 2012. Multilingualism and cognitive control: Simon and flanker task performance in monolingual and multilingual young adults. Paper presented at the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Minneapolis, MN, USA, November 15–18. [Google Scholar]
  61. Japee, Shruti, Kelsey Holiday, Maureen D. Satyshur, Ikuko Mukai, and Leslie G. Ungerleider. 2015. A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: A case study. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 9: 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Jeffreys, Harrold. 1961. Theory of Probability, 3rd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  63. Kalashnikova, Marina, Jovana Pejovic, and Manuel Carreiras. 2021. The effects of bilingualism on attentional processes in the first year of life. Developmental Science 24: e13011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Kalia, Vrinda, Makeba Parramore Wilbourn, and Kathleen Ghio. 2014. Better early or late? Examining the influence of age of exposure and language proficiency on executive function in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 26: 699–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kane, Michael J., and Randall W. Engle. 2003. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132: 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Kapa, Leah L., and John Colombo. 2013. Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development 28: 233–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Karr, Justin E., Corson N. Areshenkoff, Philippe Rast, Scott M. Hofer, Grant L. Iverson, and Mauricio A. Garcia-Barrera. 2018. The unity and diversity of executive functions: A systematic review and re-analysis of latent variable studies. Psychological Bulletin 144: 1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Kass, Robert E., and Adrian E. Raftery. 1995. Bayes factors. Journal of the American Statistical Association 90: 773–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Kremin, Lena V., Julia Alves, Adriel John Orena, Linda Polka, and Krista Byers-Heinlein. 2021. Code-switching in parents’ everyday speech to bilingual infants. Journal of Child Language, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Kyllonen, Patrick C., and Raymond E. Christal. 1990. Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence 14: 389–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Li, Shifeng, Qiongying Xu, and Ruixue Xia. 2020. Relationship between SES and academic achievement of junior high school students in China: The mediating effect of self-concept. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 2513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Linck, Jared A., Peter Osthus, Joel T. Koeth, and Michael F. Bunting. 2014. Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21: 861–83. [Google Scholar]
  73. Liu, Juan, Peng Peng, and Liang Luo. 2020. The relation between family socioeconomic status and academic achievement in China: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review 32: 49–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lowe, Cassandra J., Isu Cho, Samantha F. Goldsmith, and J. Bruce Morton. 2021. The bilingual advantage in children’s executive functioning is not related to language status: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Science 32: 1115–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Luk, Gigi, Eric De Sa, and Ellen Bialystok. 2011. Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14: 588–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Mohades, Seyede Ghazal, Esli Struys, Peter Van Schuerbeek, Katrien Mondt, Piet Van De Craen, and Robert Luypaert. 2012. DTI reveals structural differences in white matter tracts between bilingual and monolingual children. Brain Research 1435: 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Neisser, Ulric, Gwyneth Boodoo, Thomas J. Bouchard Jr., A. Wade Boykin, Nathan Brody, Stephen J. Ceci, Diane F. Halpern, John C. Loehlin, Robert Perloff, Robert J. Sternberg, and et al. 1996. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist 51: 77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Norman, Donald A., and Tim Shallice. 1986. Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In Consciousness and Self-Regulation. Edited by Richard J. Davidson, Gary E. Schwartz and David Shapi. Boston: Springer, pp. 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  79. Paap, Kenneth R., Hunter A. Johnson, and Oliver Sawi. 2014. Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology 26: 615–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Pelham, Sabra D., and Lise Abrams. 2014. Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40: 313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Raftery, Adrian E. 1995. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology 25: 111–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Raven, John C., John H. Court, and Jean Raven. 1988. Advanced Progressive Matrices. Raven Manual. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
  83. Raven, Jean, John C. Raven, and John H. Court. 2003. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment. [Google Scholar]
  84. Ruge, Hannes, and Uta Wolfensteller. 2016. Distinct contributions of lateral orbito-frontal cortex, striatum, and fronto-parietal network regions for rule encoding and control of memory-based implementation during instructed reversal learning. NeuroImage 125: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Salthouse, Timothy A. 1996. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review 103: 403–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Salthouse, Timothy A., Jeffrey E. Pink, and Elliot M. Tucker-Drob. 2008. Contextual analysis of fluid intelligence. Intelligence 36: 464–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. Shallice, Timothy, and Paul Burgess. 1996. The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 351: 1405–12. [Google Scholar]
  88. Shilling, V. M., A. Chetwynd, and P. M. A. Rabbitt. 2002. Individual inconsistency across measures of inhibition: An investigation of the construct validity of inhibition in older adults. Neuropsychologia 40: 605–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Singer, Wolf. 1999. Neuronal synchrony: A versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 24: 49–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Singh, Leher, Charlene S. L. Fu, Aishah A. Rahman, Waseem B. Hameed, Shamini Sanmugam, Pratibha Agarwal, Binyan Jiang, Yap Seng Chong, Michael J. Meaney, Anne Rifkin-Graboi, and et al. 2015. Back to basics: A bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development 86: 294–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Sirin, Selcuk R. 2005. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research 75: 417–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Sliwinska, Magdalena W., Inês R. Violante, Richard J. S. Wise, Robert Leech, Joseph T. Devlin, Fatemeh Geranmayeh, and Adam Hampshire. 2017. Stimulating multiple-demand cortex enhances vocabulary learning. Journal of Neuroscience 37: 7606–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Sliwinska, Magdalena W., Ryan Elson, and David Pitcher. 2021. Stimulating parietal regions of the multiple-demand cortex impairs novel vocabulary learning. Neuropsychologia 162: 108047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Stroop, J. Ridley. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18: 643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Tamnes, Christian K., Ylva Østby, Kristine B. Walhovd, Lars T. Westlye, Paulina Due-Tønnessen, and Anders M. Fjell. 2010. Intellectual abilities and white matter microstructure in development: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Human Brain Mapping 31: 1609–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Tao, Lily, Anna Marzecová, Marcus Taft, Dariusz Asanowicz, and Zofia Wodniecka. 2011. The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Traub, Roger D., Andrea Bibbig, Fiona EN LeBeau, Eberhard H. Buhl, and Miles A. Whittington. 2004. Cellular mechanisms of neuronal population oscillations in the hippocampus in vitro. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27: 247–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  98. Uddin, Lucina Q. 2015. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16: 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Uhlhaas, Peter J., and Wolf Singer. 2006. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: Relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron 52: 155–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Ullén, Fredrik, Lea Forsman, Örjan Blom, Anke Karabanov, and Guy Madison. 2008. Intelligence and variability in a simple timing task share neural substrates in the prefrontal white matter. Journal of Neuroscience 28: 4238–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. van Doorn, Johnny, Don van den Bergh, Udo Böhm, Fabian Dablander, Koen Derks, Tim Draws, Alexander Etz, Nathan J. Evans, Quentin F. Gronau, Julia M. Haaf, and et al. 2021. The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28: 813–26. [Google Scholar]
  102. Von Stumm, Sophie, and Robert Plomin. 2015. Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence. Intelligence 48: 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Walsh, Vincent, Elisabeth Ashbridge, and Alan Cowey. 1998. Cortical plasticity in perceptual learning demonstrated by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia 36: 45–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Wendelken, Carter, Emilio Ferrer, Simona Ghetti, Stephen K. Bailey, Laurie Cutting, and Silvia A. Bunge. 2017. Frontoparietal structural connectivity in childhood predicts development of functional connectivity and reasoning ability: A large-scale longitudinal investigation. Journal of Neuroscience 37: 8549–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  105. White, P. O. 1982. Some major components in general intelligence. In A Model for Intelligence. Edited by Hans J. Eysenck. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 44–90. [Google Scholar]
  106. Yow, W. Quin, and Xiaoqian Li. 2015. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. What is the relationship between fluid intelligence and cognitive abilities? We conceptualise them as emerging properties of dynamic interactions between multiple internal and external constraints. The system (individual) adapts to the external world (represented here by cognitive tasks (the nine boxes)) by actively selecting and implementing goals. This appears to involve the coordination of a core set of structures and functions (common executive function or common EF). However, to succeed on some tasks, the individual must also replace (update) a goal with another and/or rapidly shift between goals. Fluid intelligence is the ability to reason or implement a goal (independent of acquired knowledge) and update the working memory. It therefore overlaps with common EF and updating.
Figure 1. What is the relationship between fluid intelligence and cognitive abilities? We conceptualise them as emerging properties of dynamic interactions between multiple internal and external constraints. The system (individual) adapts to the external world (represented here by cognitive tasks (the nine boxes)) by actively selecting and implementing goals. This appears to involve the coordination of a core set of structures and functions (common executive function or common EF). However, to succeed on some tasks, the individual must also replace (update) a goal with another and/or rapidly shift between goals. Fluid intelligence is the ability to reason or implement a goal (independent of acquired knowledge) and update the working memory. It therefore overlaps with common EF and updating.
Languages 07 00100 g001
Figure 2. Early bilinguals demonstrated greater fluid intelligence than late bilinguals and monolinguals. Error bars represent ±1 standard error of the mean.
Figure 2. Early bilinguals demonstrated greater fluid intelligence than late bilinguals and monolinguals. Error bars represent ±1 standard error of the mean.
Languages 07 00100 g002
Table 1. Participant characteristics, by group, gender, and parental education.
Table 1. Participant characteristics, by group, gender, and parental education.
GroupNAge (SD)% FemalesEducation (SD) 1
Early bilingual4926.71 (4.34)49%3.83 (1.35)
Late bilingual6427.97 (8.76)55%3.27 (1.30)
Monolingual5731.86 (13.90)42%3.25 (1.42)
1 Parents’ education score: 1 = no/incomplete schooling; 2 = high school; 3 = diploma; 4 = bachelor’s degree; 5 = master’s degree; 6 = doctorate.
Table 2. Group characteristics: mean number of languages (SD) and median proficiency (range).
Table 2. Group characteristics: mean number of languages (SD) and median proficiency (range).
All BilingualsEarly BilingualsLate BilingualsSig.
Languages
All languages3.79 (1.33)4.06 (1.21)3.58 (1.39) . 056  
Conversational 13.12 (1.05)3.41 (1.10)2.91 (0.97).011 *
Advanced 22.52 (0.96)2.94 (0.92)2.20 (0.88)<.001 ***
Proficiency 3
L1 proficiency4.00 (1.00)4.00 (0.00)4.00 (1.00).382
L2 proficiency4.00 (2.00)4.00 (2.00)3.00 (2.00)<.001 ***
L2 minus L10.00 (2.00)0.00 (2.00)−1.00 (2.00).002 **
Note: Continuous data (number of languages) are reported using the mean and standard deviation, and analysed using independent t-tests. Ordinal data (language proficiency) are reported using the median and range, and analysed using Mann-Whitney U tests. 1 Number of languages the participant is conversational, advanced, or fluent/native/native-like in. 2 Number of languages the participant is advanced or fluent/native/native-like in. 3 Proficiency: 1 = basic; 2 = conversational; 3 = advanced; 4 = fluent/native/native-like. L1 = first language; L2 = second language; L2 minus L1 = L2 proficiency minus L1 proficiency (0 = equally proficient in both languages). p < .10, * p < .05, ** p < .01 *** p < .001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

D’Souza, D.; Dakhch, Y. Is Early Bilingual Experience Associated with Greater Fluid Intelligence in Adults? Languages 2022, 7, 100. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020100

AMA Style

D’Souza D, Dakhch Y. Is Early Bilingual Experience Associated with Greater Fluid Intelligence in Adults? Languages. 2022; 7(2):100. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020100

Chicago/Turabian Style

D’Souza, Dean, and Yousra Dakhch. 2022. "Is Early Bilingual Experience Associated with Greater Fluid Intelligence in Adults?" Languages 7, no. 2: 100. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020100

APA Style

D’Souza, D., & Dakhch, Y. (2022). Is Early Bilingual Experience Associated with Greater Fluid Intelligence in Adults? Languages, 7(2), 100. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7020100

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop