Next Article in Journal
Approaching Composition as Showing–Telling through Translanguaging: Weaving Multilingualism, Multimodality, and Multiliteracies in a Digital Collage Proyecto Final
Next Article in Special Issue
Less Direct, More Analytical: Eye-Movement Measures of L2 Idiom Reading
Previous Article in Journal
Acquiring French Intonation against the Backdrop of Heritage Bilingualism: The Case of German–Turkish Learners
Previous Article in Special Issue
Can Heritage Speakers Predict Lexical and Morphosyntactic Information in Reading?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Many Palabras? Codeswitching and Lexical Diversity in Spanish-English Picture Books

by
Viridiana L. Benitez
1,*,
Marissa Castellana
1 and
Christine E. Potter
2
1
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ 85287, USA
2
Department of Psychology, University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX 79968, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Languages 2022, 7(1), 69; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7010069
Submission received: 20 November 2021 / Revised: 8 February 2022 / Accepted: 4 March 2022 / Published: 16 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Cognitive Nature of Bilingual Reading)

Abstract

:
Bilingual picture books have been growing in popularity, with caregivers, teachers, and researchers increasingly interested in understanding how picture books might be able to support the learning of words in two languages. In this study, we present the first evaluation of the quantity and quality of text contained within bilingual picture books in English and Spanish targeted to children ages 0–9 and available to parents in the United States. We focus specifically on a sample of codeswitching books (N = 45) which present text in one language embedded in another language. All books were transcribed and evaluated for (1) the number of words and utterances presented in each language; (2) the quality and complexity of text presented in each language; and (3) how switching occurred between the two languages. Results showed that although picture books in our sample presented predominantly English text and more complex English sentences, relatively more unique words were presented in Spanish. Furthermore, picture books in our sample presented frequent switching between languages, particularly within utterances. We suggest that bilingual picture books provide children with potentially enriching yet asymmetrical opportunities for learning in each language.

1. Introduction

Early reading experiences provide an important foundation for young children’s language development. Children’s vocabulary knowledge is positively predicted by having more books in their home, reading with their caregivers at earlier ages, and spending more time reading with their caregivers (Farrant and Zubrick 2012; Sénéchal and Lefevre 2002; Duursma et al. 2007; Fitton et al. 2018). One explanation for why early reading is critical is that picture books contain rich linguistic input, often including words and concepts that are not typically found in spoken language (Montag et al. 2015). Given that children’s language development is linked to their rich and varied experiences with language in their environments (e.g., Hurtado et al. 2008; Ramírez-Esparza et al. 2017; Sperry and Sperry 1996; Weisleder and Fernald 2013), it is important to evaluate the text that children’s picture books contain as a key source of enriching input contributing to young children’s language development (Montag et al. 2015; Logan et al. 2019).
For bilingual children, who need to learn roughly twice as many words as their monolingual peers, language skills depend not just on their overall input, but also on the input that they receive in each of their languages (Marchman et al. 2017; Place and Hoff 2011; Pearson et al. 1997). Moreover, bilinguals have to contend with challenges stemming from unequal exposure to each of their languages and the possibility that the two languages will be mixed together (Fennell and Byers-Heinlein 2014; Oller et al. 2007). Recognizing these challenges inherent in bilingual learning, caregivers, teachers, and researchers are becoming increasingly interested in understanding how picture books support the learning of words in two languages (Brouillard et al. 2020; Méndez et al. 2015; Read et al. 2021b; Restrepo et al. 2013). However, no empirical research has systematically evaluated the quality of the text in bilingual picture books, which present text in two languages. In the current study, we examined the text of Spanish-English bilingual picture books in order to understand how these books may contribute to the input that children receive within and across languages.
Bilingual children’s books have been growing in popularity, both as a source of support for bilingual language development and as a means to expose monolingual children to words in a second language. In our own search, more than half of the books marketed to families as bilingual were published in the last 10 years (see Appendix A for full list of books). Bilingual families report owning bilingual books in addition to monolingual books in each of their languages (Gonzalez-Barrero et al. 2021). Beyond providing exposure to multiple languages, bilingual books have the potential to support dual-language literacy and academic skills for bilingual children at home, in the classroom, and in libraries (Agosto 1997; Dávila et al. 2017; Castro et al. 2011; Ernst-Slavit and Mulhern 2003; Glazer et al. 2017; Gómez et al. 2021; Hadaway and Young 2013; Rodríguez-Valls 2011; Semingson et al. 2015; Thibeault and Matheson 2020; Walker et al. 1996).
A growing body of research has begun documenting the benefits of bilingual books. For instance, reading bilingual books increases bilingual children’s engagement and confidence during literacy activities (Hu et al. 2012; Zaidi 2020). Furthermore, compared to monolingual books, bilingual books have been found to be just as supportive, and sometimes more supportive, of young children’s vocabulary learning and language skills in two languages (Brouillard et al. 2020; Naqvi et al. 2012; Read et al. 2021b; Tsybina and Eriks-Brophy 2010). Given the potential importance of bilingual picture books for dual language development, recent studies have emphasized the need to design and select bilingual children’s books that promote engagement and learning, focusing on issues related to the appropriateness of the narrative content, translation quality, and design features of the text and illustrations (Chen 2019; Gallagher and Bataineh 2020; Glazer et al. 2017; Hojeij et al. 2019; Walker et al. 1996). Critically, in order to understand the potential role of bilingual books for supporting learning in two languages, examinations of the quantity and quality of text in each language contained within these books are needed.
Previous research exploring bilingual language environments has shown that the language input that bilingual children experience not just holistically, but in each language individually, contributes to dual language development. Bilingual children’s knowledge of their two languages develops separately, and language skills in each of their languages are related to the specific experience they have had with that language (Marchman et al. 2010; Pearson et al. 1997). When children hear more words and a higher diversity of words in a particular language, their vocabulary knowledge in that language grows (David and Wei 2008; Marchman et al. 2017; Hoff et al. 2012; Pearson et al. 1997). Therefore, to understand how the text in bilingual picture books may promote learning, it is important to determine how much input is provided in each language and whether the input is equally rich across the two languages.
It is also important to recognize that the two languages can be used together, not just presented individually (e.g., Bullock and Toribio 2009; Vogel and García 2017). According to both observational studies and parental reports, codeswitching, or the use of two languages together, is a common feature of bilingual children’s auditory input (Bail et al. 2015; Byers-Heinlein 2013; Goodz 1989; Kremin et al. 2021). Children regularly hear switches in language both within a single utterance (e.g., ¡Mira el firetruck!) or between utterances (e.g., ¡Mira el camión de bomberos! Its sirens are so loud!). Interestingly, bilingual children do not show disruptions in their processing when hearing switches that span utterance boundaries but are often less efficient to comprehend sentences that contain a within-utterance switch (Byers-Heinlein et al. 2017; Morini and Newman 2019; Potter et al. 2019). Moreover, codeswitching can interfere with children’s learning of new words (Byers-Heinlein 2013; Byers-Heinlein et al. 2020). Nonetheless, codeswitching is relatively common in bilingual children’s everyday experience. However, we do not yet know whether bilingual picture books offer input that matches the mixing found in spoken language or present the two languages in different ways.
Only a limited set of studies have examined early reading and literacy activities within bilingual environments, using different approaches. One approach involves assessing readers’ talk surrounding shared reading activities. When asked about reading practices at home, bilingual caregivers report using both languages when reading to their child, even if reading a book in a single language (Muysken et al. 1996; Read et al. 2021a). For example, caregivers may translate words or switch to their dominant language while reading a book in their less-preferred language (Gonzalez-Barrero et al. 2021), although the language of preference during reading may depend on the home language environment and the reading preference of the caregiver (Read et al. 2021a). Direct assessments of talk during shared reading to bilingual children, most of which have been conducted via case studies, demonstrate that teachers and caregivers incorporate both languages in their extra-textual talk (Bauer 2000; García and Kleifgen 2020; Gonzalez-Barrero et al. 2021; Kabuto 2010; Li and Fleer 2015; Moody et al. 2021; Pontier and Gort 2016; Song 2016). In addition, children’s learning from reading interactions is shaped by how the two languages are used together (Brouillard et al. 2020; Méndez et al. 2015; Read et al. 2021b; Restrepo et al. 2013). When Spanish-speaking preschoolers heard both English and Spanish during instruction involving shared reading, they learned more words than when they only heard English (Méndez et al. 2015). Together, these studies provide evidence that bilingual children regularly encounter both languages during reading and that differences in how the languages are used can affect the words that they learn.
While parents undoubtedly present important input via their talk surrounding books, the text of books themselves provide a potentially valuable source of input, and we know little about the quality of the text in bilingual children’s books. Given that parents are likely to read picture books to young children from an early age (Deckner et al. 2006; Raikes et al. 2006; Taaffe Young et al. 1998), scrutinizing the text in children’s picture books can provide a useful measure of the kinds of words that children have the opportunity to encounter. This approach has been used recently to assess the quantity and quality of words found in English children’s picture books (Dawson et al. 2021; Logan et al. 2019; Montag et al. 2015). However, no study has examined the quantity and quality of text contained within bilingual children’s picture books. In the present study, we measured the quantity and quality of text in Spanish-English bilingual codeswitching picture books currently available to parents in the U.S. to understand their potential contribution to the language input of young bilingual learners.
Although bilingual books are now available in a variety of language combinations, we focused specifically on books containing English and Spanish given that Spanish is the second most spoken language in the U.S. (U.S. Census Bureau 2019). A limited set of studies has examined the text of Spanish-English bilingual picture books written for children; however, these studies have largely focused on the narrative content of books (Alamillo 2017; Barrera et al. 2003; Botelho and Marion 2020; Chappell and Faltis 2007; Chaudhri and Torres 2021; Clark et al. 2015; Domke 2018; Gomm et al. 2017; Kelly 2020; Naidoo 2011; Naidoo and López-Robertson 2007). Only two studies have provided analyses of the quantity of each language presented in bilingual children’s books, showing that Spanish-English bilingual books contain predominantly English text (Domke 2018; Gomm et al. 2017). According to Domke (2018), English text was also more readable than Spanish text. These findings provide preliminary evidence that Spanish-English bilingual children’s books do not seem to provide equal input in the two languages and are likely skewed toward English. In addition to examining the amount of text in each language, our study further probed the richness of the input that children may encounter in Spanish vs. English, as well as providing the first analysis of how the languages are used together in a large sample of picture books.
Our sample of books was generated using online search engines that parents in the U.S. are likely to use when choosing bilingual picture books in English and Spanish for their children. Previous research has indicated that there are many different types of bilingual books (Domke 2018; Jeffers 2009; Semingson et al. 2015). Our search results included translation books, which present the entire text in both languages, as well as codeswitching books, which involve the embedding of words and/or sentences in one language within another language. The current study targets codeswitching books specifically, as this book format presents different information in each language and also presents multiple ways in which the two languages can be used together, reflecting the potential variability of dual-language experience. Moreover, while a translation book may be essentially two copies of a monolingual book, books containing codeswitching represent a type of input that is unique to the bilingual experience.
Each book was transcribed in its entirety, and we calculated multiple measures of quantitative and qualitative properties of the text in each picture book, taking inspiration from recent studies evaluating the quantity and quality of text in popular children’s books in English (Logan et al. 2019; Montag et al. 2015), and from research describing child-directed speech (Anderson et al. 2021; Hurtado et al. 2008; Rowe 2012). For each picture book in our sample, we computed total word counts, assessed sentence complexity, and measured lexical diversity in each language. We additionally examined switching between the two languages. Our goal was to address three primary research questions.
First, we asked whether the picture books provide different amounts of exposure to Spanish vs. English. Our measures of quantity (calculated separately for each language) were the total number of words (word tokens), the number of unique words excluding repetitions (word types), and the number of utterances fully in that language (i.e., number of English-only utterances and the number of Spanish-only utterances). We predicted that picture books, on average, would present text predominantly in English, consistent with prior studies (Domke 2018; Gomm et al. 2017).
Our second question concerned whether there were differences in the quality of text presented in each language. To assess quality, for each picture book, we considered both the lexical diversity and the sentence complexity found in each language. We operationalized lexical diversity as type/token ratio, which corresponds to the proportion of unique words in a language, accounting for the total number of words appearing in that language. To measure sentence complexity, we calculated the mean length of utterance in each language in each picture book. Our prediction was that picture books, on average, would present greater lexical diversity and sentence complexity in English. We also examined the most frequent words used across our entire corpus of picture books for each language as a preliminary way of testing whether different words and concepts were presented in the two languages.
Our final research question asked how the languages were used together. Because no prior study has explored codeswitching in a large sample of bilingual books, we did not have direct hypotheses about the nature of mixing. Rather, we aimed to describe (1) the frequency of language switching both within and between utterances, (2) the quantity of English and Spanish words presented within utterances that contain both languages, and (3) the complexity of utterances containing both languages.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

Our final sample of codeswitching books (N = 45) was taken from a larger list of bilingual children’s books in Spanish and English compiled from several online sources: Amazon, Google, and two local library system catalogs (Phoenix Public Library and El Paso Public Library). One additional book was transcribed but excluded due to being more than 5 standard deviations from the sample mean for number of word tokens.
Searches were conducted in June and July of 2021 using the search terms “bilingual children’s books” or “Spanish English bilingual children’s books.” The results of each search were filtered to exclude electronic-only materials and to only include books targeted toward children ages 0–9 that contained both Spanish and English. Books were then classified as either codeswitching if the text switched between English and Spanish such that one language was not exclusively used for direct translations, or translational if the full text was presented in both English and Spanish. The search generated a list of 286 titles.
The final list of code-switching books included in the current study can be found in Table A1. Books were published between 1995 and 2020, included 32 authors and 28 publishers, and targeted different age ranges spanning infancy to middle childhood: ages 0–3 (N = 2), ages 2–4 (N = 2), ages 3–7 (N = 9), ages 4–9 (N = 23), and ages 5–9 (N = 9).

2.2. Transcription and Coding

We transcribed all books with the Computerized Language Analysis (CLAN) program using the Computational Human Articulatory Theory (CHAT) transcription format (MacWhinney 2000). Text was transcribed as written in the book by five trained coders. Coders were all fluent in English and at least moderately proficient in Spanish. Although books often contain dialogue by characters (usually in quotation marks), all text was transcribed as coming from a single source. Background text that was part of the illustration on a page was only included if it was part of the main narrative.
Consistent with CHAT conventions, we divided the text into utterances. Coders were instructed to delimit an utterance when a statement ended in a punctuation mark indicating a full stop (period, exclamation, or question mark). In the case of dialogue (placed within quotation marks), coders were instructed to transcribe character statements and the surrounding narrative text, as separate utterances (see the example below).
Text: “Really,” she murmured, “it was not that bad.”
Transcription:
> really.
> she murmured.
> it was not that bad.
Consistent with CHAT conventions, for each transcript, one language was assigned as the default language and the other language was assigned as the secondary language. For all our picture books, English was assigned as the default language and Spanish was assigned as the secondary language. Utterances fully in Spanish were marked as Spanish-only utterances. Spanish words that appeared intermixed with English words within an utterance were marked as Spanish words. Coders were instructed to mark all words that were in Spanish even if these were proper nouns (e.g., Mamá) or have been adopted into the English lexicon (e.g., piñata). Different forms of the same word (e.g., abuela/abuelita, mamá/mami, dog/doggy) were written out as they appeared in the picture book, therefore counting as unique word types. The full codebook used for transcription is available on the Open Science Framework (OSF): https://osf.io/sxm5t.

2.3. Measures

Several measures were derived from the transcriptions of each book, using CLAN, to assess quantitative and qualitative features of each text. All measures were calculated for each picture book individually.
Number of word tokens was defined as the total number of words, number of word types was defined as the number of unique words, and type/token ratio was calculated by taking the number of word types and dividing it by the number of word tokens. We also calculated the total number of utterances separately for English-only, Spanish-only, and mixed-language utterances (which contained both languages). Mean length of utterance (MLU) was determined by averaging the number of word tokens in each utterance.
Amount of exposure in each language (quantity) was assessed by comparing the number of word tokens and word types in English vs. Spanish, as well as the number of English-only utterances vs. the number of Spanish-only utterances. Measures of quality included type/token ratio (lexical diversity) for English vs. Spanish and MLU (sentence complexity) for English-only utterances vs. Spanish-only utterances.
In addition, we examined switching between languages. While there are different approaches to assessing language switching (e.g., Bail et al. 2015; Cantone 2007; David and Wei 2008; Isurin et al. 2009; Kremin et al. 2021; Nicoladis and Genesee 1998), in the current study, we relied on CHAT coding conventions designed to separate a default and secondary language (Muysken 2000; Myers-Scotton 1997). We first identified utterances that contained both English words and words marked in Spanish. These utterances were classified as a mixed-language utterance, and counted as a single within-utterance switch (even if multiple switches occurred within that one utterance, consistent with the conventions used by Bail et al. (2015) to describe switching in spoken language; e.g., “she placed el guisante in the bed for their guest” from La Princesa and the Pea). The number of between-utterance switches was derived by identifying Spanish-only utterances and determining whether a language switch occurred before and/or after that Spanish-only utterance (e.g., “mommy loves kisses sweet as can be”/“besos mami besos” from Besos for Baby). In accordance with CHAT conventions, between-utterance switches were counted when a Spanish-only utterance and another utterance type (English-only or mixed-language utterances) occurred sequentially (i.e., when an English-only utterance was followed by a Spanish-only utterance, when a Spanish-only utterance was followed by an English-only utterance, when a mixed-language utterance was followed by a Spanish-only utterance, and when a Spanish-only utterance was followed by a mixed-language utterance).
We counted the number of within-utterance switches and the number of between-utterance switches in each picture book, and also calculated the percentage of mixed-language utterances in each picture book. Additionally, we calculated the proportion of words in English vs. Spanish in each mixed-language utterance and calculated MLU for mixed-language utterances.

2.4. Reliability

Reliability for a set of six measures was assessed separately for each picture book using Krippendorff’s alpha, which accounts for chance, can accommodate multiple coders, is sensitive to the size of values used in coding data, and takes into consideration the measure of the codes (e.g., ordinal vs. interval; Hayes and Krippendorff 2007; Krippendorff 2011a, 2011b, 2004). Reliability is considered good when values are 0.80 or higher.
An experienced coder (the second author) transcribed two “training” books in our sample. The five coders were then asked to transcribe these two books, and training continued until each coder achieved reliability of 0.80 or higher with the expert’s transcription for each of our main measures. Once reliability for our training books was achieved, each coder was assigned a set of picture books to transcribe. After coders completed their assigned picture books, we conducted an additional test of reliability by having all coders transcribe the same six books. Reliability was then calculated for six measures: (1) overall MLU, (2) overall number of utterances, (3) overall number of word types, (4) overall number of word tokens, (5) number of Spanish word tokens, and (6) number of Spanish-only utterances. These measures were selected for reliability because they provide an assessment of consistency in overall transcription of the picture books as well as consistency in coders’ use of CHAT-specific codes to mark Spanish words and Spanish-only utterances. Reliability was calculated for 17% of the final sample (two training books plus six additional reliability books).
Krippendorff’s alpha was high, averaging 0.94 (0.96 for MLU, 0.98 for number of utterances, 0.99 for word tokens, 0.99 for word types, 0.91 for Spanish word tokens, and 0.82 for Spanish-only utterances), demonstrating good reliability. Reliability for the number of utterances in Spanish was initially low (0.40); coders were reminded of the code for marking utterances fully in Spanish and asked to correct all of their transcripts. After correction, good reliability was achieved.

3. Results

Each of our key measures was derived in CLAN for each picture book separately, and we report descriptives aggregated across picture books, unless otherwise noted. For across-language comparisons, we conducted two-tailed paired sample t-tests, except for our analyses of codeswitching, which were modified in response to the skewness of the data. All analyses and plots were conducted and generated in R (version 3.6.3). All data, CLAN codes used to derive each measure, and R script for the main analyses reported in this manuscript are openly available on OSF: https://osf.io/sxm5t.

3.1. Overall Descriptives

We first examined the total number of word tokens, word types, and utterances in each picture book, collapsing across languages. Picture books in our sample contained on average 581.84 word tokens (SD = 356.49; range: 96–1769). Of those words, the mean number of word types (unique words) was 261.62 (SD = 121.71; range: 50–514), resulting in an average type/token ratio of 0.49 (SD = 0.11; range: 0.16–0.74). Picture books in our sample contained on average 85.98 utterances (SD = 51.3; range: 12–217) with an average MLU of 7.34 (SD = 4.33; range: 3.9–31.25). Means and standard deviations for each of our key measures by language are presented in Table 1.

3.2. Measures of Quantity: Codeswitching Picture Books Present Predominantly English Text

Our first question was whether codeswitching picture books present different quantities of text in Spanish vs. English. To address this question, we compared the average number of word tokens, number of word types, and number of utterances in each language (see Figure 1). Picture books in our sample on average contained significantly more word tokens in English than in Spanish [t(44) = −8.41, p < 0.001] and significantly more word types in English than in Spanish [t(44)= −10.03, p < 0.001]. We also examined the number of utterances in each language (excluding mixed-language utterances). The number of English-only utterances was higher than the number of Spanish-only utterances, a difference that was significant [t(44) = 6.27, p < 0.001].
Examining the percentage of word tokens in Spanish in picture books in our sample, it is clear that English was the dominant language; on average, only 15.42% (SD = 10.6%) of the text in our picture books was composed of Spanish words tokens, with some books having as little as 1% Spanish word tokens and other books having a maximum of 49% Spanish word tokens. Most books (N = 34) contained less than 20% Spanish word tokens, a few books (N = 11) contained between 20% and 50% Spanish word tokens, and no picture book in our sample contained 50% or more Spanish word tokens.

3.3. Measures of Quality: Codeswitching Picture Books Present Shorter Utterances in Spanish but Relatively More Unique Words in Spanish

Our second aim was to compare the quality of text across the two languages. Our first measure of quality was type/token ratio (see Figure 2A). Contrary to our hypothesis, type/token ratio was significantly higher in Spanish than in English [t(44) = 3.62, p < 0.001]. That is, although there may have been fewer words in Spanish presented within our picture books, the lexical diversity of Spanish was higher, demonstrating there were relatively more unique words presented in Spanish.
Our second measure of the quality of the text was mean length of utterance (MLU), which reflects the mean number of word tokens within single-language utterances (excluding mixed-language utterances). The MLU of English-only utterances within picture books in our sample was significantly higher than the MLU of Spanish-only utterances [t(44) = 10.78, p < 0.001]. This suggests that picture books in our sample presented longer and presumably more complex English-only utterances than Spanish-only utterances (see Figure 2B).
Thus, although picture books in our sample contained more total word tokens and word types in English, and more word tokens per utterance in English, relatively more unique words were presented in Spanish than in English.

3.4. Measures of Frequent Words: Across Books, Function Words Appear More Often in English, and Words about Family Are More Common in Spanish

To better understand differences in how the two languages were being used, we explored which words were presented most frequently across our entire sample of picture books. For these analyses, we examined frequencies over the full corpus of text generated by our sample of picture books. Our corpus included a total of 30,381 word tokens, of which 26,805 were in English and 3578 were in Spanish.
We first examined the ten most frequent words for English and Spanish separately (see Table 2). In English, the most frequently used words in our corpus were function words (e.g., articles, conjunctions, prepositions, and pronouns). Although some function words also appear among the most frequent words in Spanish, the most frequently used words in our corpus in Spanish were predominantly content words (nouns, specifically words referring to people).
To rule out the possibility that apparent differences in the information being presented were due to linguistic properties (e.g., because of the use of grammatical gender and plural marking, there are more articles in Spanish, and the common English verb “to be” can be translated as either ser or estar in Spanish), we then examined the translation equivalents for the top five most frequent words in each language (see Table 3). While this analysis revealed that function words did appear relatively often in Spanish, we still found that certain content words were much more frequent in Spanish than in English. For example, the Spanish word abuela appeared 75 times and comprised 2.1% of Spanish words in our corpus, but its English equivalent grandma (or grandmother) occurred only 52 times and comprised just 0.1% of English words in our corpus.
Lastly, to further investigate the possibility that some information (e.g., names for family members) disproportionately appeared in Spanish, we examined the top five most frequently used nouns in English and Spanish in our corpus. Here, we attempted to describe words at the conceptual level, and collapsed across closely related variants of the same word (e.g., abuela, abuelas, and abuelita were grouped together). Table 4 displays the results. We again found differences in the kinds of words being used across the two languages. Specifically, the most common nouns in Spanish made up a much larger proportion of the total Spanish input (9.9%), compared to the most common English nouns, which made up only 0.7% of the total English input. Moreover, in English, the five most frequent nouns appeared to refer to places and routines, while the most frequently used nouns in Spanish were largely names of family members.
Together, our preliminary exploration into the kinds of words being presented frequently across our corpus of codeswitching picture books reveals differences across languages: the most common words in Spanish are mostly referring to family. In contrast, English presents more function words, and individual nouns are far less frequent and less tightly clustered in meaning.

3.5. Measures of Language Mixing: Codeswitching Picture Books Present More within-Utterance Than between-Utterance Language Switches

Our third question concerned how the two languages were presented together. Given high kurtosis (>3) in the distribution of difference scores for the following paired comparisons, we conducted Wilcoxon signed rank tests for these analyses. We first evaluated the amount of switching between languages and then examined the length and composition of mixed-language utterances.
Only two books did not contain within-utterance switches and only nine books did not contain between-utterances switches. Figure 3 presents the distribution of switches as a percentage of overall number of utterances, for within- and between-utterance switches. Picture books in our sample contained on average 27.82 within-utterance switches, or, on average, 38.4% of utterances included switching. Strikingly, there were more mixed-language utterances than Spanish-only utterances [Z = 932.5, p < 0.001]. In addition, books had on average 11.33 between-utterance switches, or, on average, 13.18% of utterances represented a switch from the prior utterance. Thus, there were significantly more within-utterance switches than between-utterances switches [Z = 923, p < 0.001], and overall, switching was common. Figure 3 presents the distribution of switches as a percentage of overall number of utterances, for within- and between-utterance switches.
Further, mixed-language utterances presented more English word tokens (M = 8.01, SD = 5.42) than Spanish word tokens (M = 1.93, SD = 2.16), a difference that was significant [Z = 946, p < 0.001]. Interestingly, the MLU for mixed-language utterances (M = 8.96, SD = 5.42) was higher than the MLU for Spanish-only utterances [Z = 1011, p < 0.001] and higher than the MLU for English-only utterances [Z = 915, p < 0.001], suggesting that mixed-language utterances were typically more complex than single-language utterances.
Together, these results show that codeswitching picture books present high amounts of switching to young children, both within- and between-utterances, with more within-utterance switches than between-utterance switches. Based on MLU, mixed-language utterances were the most complex type of sentence, but consistent with the overall composition of the books, mixed utterances contained more English words than Spanish words. In fact, most mixed-language utterances contained only a word or two in Spanish and conveyed the majority of their content in English.

4. Discussion

The goal of the current study was to examine the quantity and quality of text in Spanish-English codeswitching picture books readily available to parents in the U.S. We considered the amount and complexity of text presented in each language, as well as how the two languages were used together. Overall, our sample of codeswitching picture books contained many more words and utterances in English than in Spanish. Additionally, English utterances had a higher MLU than Spanish utterances, suggesting higher sentence complexity in English. However, the type/token ratio of the Spanish text was higher than the type/token ratio of the English text, demonstrating that relatively more unique words were presented in Spanish than in English. Finally, our sample of picture books presented high amounts of codeswitching, with over a third of utterances including words in both languages, many more than the number of utterances presented solely in Spanish. Together, these findings indicate that codeswitching picture books offer potentially enriching dual-language input, but they appear to provide unequal experience across the two languages.
Consistent with previous research (Domke 2018; Gomm et al. 2017), we found that Spanish-English codeswitching picture books primarily present English text. The majority of our sample (73% of picture books) contained 20% or fewer Spanish word tokens, and every single book in our sample contained more English words than Spanish words. Although this was consistent with our predictions, it is notable how much English is overrepresented within codeswitching picture books, given that these picture books are marketed as “bilingual.” It is an open question whether this type of imbalance is characteristic of codeswitching books in general, or if this is particular to codeswitching books available to families in the U.S, where the perceived status of English and Spanish may differ, particularly in academic or literary contexts (e.g., Cha and Goldenberg 2015; Hoff et al. 2018; Torres 2007). Future work describing codeswitching picture books available in other countries and other language combinations could determine whether codeswitching books tend to present a dominant language or if this pattern is specific to the U.S. context.
In addition to highlighting quantitative differences in English and Spanish text, our results revealed that text written in English and Spanish differed along qualitative dimensions. Consistent with our predictions, the picture books in our sample presented more complex English-only utterances than Spanish-only utterances (as defined by MLU). For example, two consecutive lines from Paletero Man demonstrate this trend: “We called out your name when we saw your coins drop but you must have not heard us because you didn’t stop. Muchas gracias amigos.” Contrary to our predictions, however, picture books in our sample had higher lexical diversity in Spanish than in English, indicating a greater variety of word types in Spanish relative to the total amount of Spanish words. Together, these findings demonstrate that the two languages are being used differently.
Examining the frequency of words in English and Spanish across the entire corpus provided additional evidence that the two languages were used to present different information. Approximately 10% of the total input in Spanish consisted of words referring to just four family members (abuela, mamá, papá, and tía). For example, an excerpt from Between Us and Abuela: A Family Story from the Border uses Spanish to incorporate a family word, ”Abuela drops kisses on our fingers.” In contrast, the most common words in English were function words, and the most frequent nouns in English, which comprised less than 1% of the total English words, were not closely related. For example, all function words (underlined) in this mixed-language utterance from Gazpacho for Nacho appear in English: “Among all the colorful, fresh alimentos, he picked out a few crispy green pimientos.” Thus, these codeswitching picture books introduce different concepts and vocabulary across languages, and they appear to be providing richer grammatical complexity in English.
We selected our picture books to contain codeswitching, and indeed, picture books in our sample contained high amounts of switching between languages. We found that the rate of switching in our books was notably higher than even the highest estimates of the frequency of switching in child-directed speech, which range from 3% to 20% of utterances (Bail et al. 2015; Bentahila and Davies 1995; Goodz 1989; Kremin et al. 2021; Nicoladis and Secco 2000; Pan 1995; Tare and Gelman 2011). We also found that the picture books in our sample presented more within-utterance switches than between-utterance switches. This pattern contrasts recent studies of language mixing in child-directed speech showing that caregivers appear to be more likely to switch languages between utterances than within utterances (Bail et al. 2015; Kremin et al. 2021). Finally, in addition to being highly frequent, mixed-language utterances had a higher MLU than any other utterance type, demonstrating that the complexity of mixed-language utterances is high within the picture books in our sample. For example, the mixed-language utterance “Brave bomberos reach their station, time for rest and relaxation, eat their supper, wash los platos, feed their pets—los perros, gatos” from Fuego Fuego Brave Bomberos demonstrates high MLU with several within-utterance switches. Thus, bilingual picture books present complex input with respect to how the two languages are used together, and this input does not appear to mirror typical patterns of child-directed speech. This difference could be supportive of dual-language learning by adding variability and complexity to the bilingual language input children experience.
Our study contributes to the growing body of evidence demonstrating that young children’s picture books contain rich linguistic input from which young children can learn (Montag et al. 2015; Logan et al. 2019) and that bilingual picture books in particular may be an important format for engaging and teaching bilingual children language and literacy skills (Brouillard et al. 2020; Hu et al. 2012; Naqvi et al. 2012; Read et al. 2021a; Tsybina and Eriks-Brophy 2010; Zaidi 2020). However, several important questions remain. In particular, it is crucial to consider the underlying causes driving the differences in quantitative and qualitative features in the English vs. Spanish text in picture books. There are several non-exclusive possibilities for why these differences are present.
One potential reason for the qualitative differences found between English and Spanish in picture books in our sample could be linguistic differences between Spanish and English. Spanish uses richer inflectional morphology compared to English (e.g., in Spanish, both grammatical gender and plurality are marked for nouns, articles, and adjectives; verbs can be inflected for tense, person, and mood; e.g., Lang 2013; Moreno-Sandoval and Goñi-Menoyo 2002), and it is possible that this leads to an inflated value for our type/token ratio measure. For example, accounting for the Spanish variations of the English word the in our exploration of the types of words presented in English and Spanish across our entire corpus, we found that articles represented a similar proportion of the input in each language.
However, even after we considered different translation equivalents of highly common words, we still found that function words appeared to be more frequent in English, as a relative proportion of the English tokens as well as in the input as a whole. Given that function words are the most frequent words found in natural language (Zipf 1949), the fact that common words such as a, is, and to were more frequent in English could explain why lexical diversity was higher in Spanish while MLU was higher for English-only utterances. It remains an open question whether child-directed texts composed of only Spanish tend to have higher lexical diversity than child-directed text composed of only English, or if this pattern reflects the use of Spanish in combination with English. Future research will explore differences between bilingual and monolingual picture books written in both English and Spanish, but no appropriate corpora for comparison exist at present. Moreover, even if it is the case that Spanish text tends to have higher lexical diversity, we found very different words occurring with high frequency across the two languages, again suggesting that these books present different types of input in English vs. Spanish.
Another factor that may have led to both the quantitative and qualitative differences between languages in our sample of picture books is that English appears to be treated as the primary language, while Spanish is embedded to introduce novel and unique words. This possibility is supported by the characteristics of Spanish-only and mixed-language utterances. On average, English-only utterances were about 3.5 times longer than Spanish-only utterances, which were typically only one to two words in length. Likewise, mixed-language utterances were largely composed of English words, with, on average, only one or two words in Spanish intermixed (e.g., “Yellow are faroles flickering bright.” from Green is a Chile Pepper: A Book of Colors). Thus, Spanish words appear to be sprinkled into English text, with very few longer sequences of Spanish words in succession.
Further supporting the view that Spanish was presented as a supplement to English is the fact that 37 of the 45 books in our sample contained a glossary of translations for the Spanish words, implying that the author or publisher assumed that the reader would have greater familiarity with English. Authors aiming to sell books within the English-dominant U.S. may thus not be targeting bilingual children, but rather attempting to reach English-speaking children for whom Spanish words would represent novel vocabulary in an unfamiliar language (Domke 2018; Torres 2007). If many of these books were intended for the purpose of teaching English-speaking children new Spanish words, that could explain why the lexical diversity was higher in Spanish and why nouns dominated the Spanish input. Future studies could further explore the explicit goals of different types of books, in addition to the text contained within them.
It is also important to consider what messages, implicitly or explicitly, these books may send about the status of the two languages and how the two languages are, and should be, used. Specifically, the predominance of English in the books may reinforce English as a language for reading, learning, and speaking, while signaling that Spanish is less important (Domke 2018; Hammer et al. 2012; Montanari et al. 2020). Beliefs about language status can be instantiated, developed, and challenged across a variety of social contexts, including children’s interactions with family, peers, teachers, and texts (e.g., Jaffe 2003; Kyratzis 2010; Martínez-Roldán and Malavé 2004; Showstack 2017). The privileging of English and minoritization of Spanish within the bilingual picture books in our sample could inform children’s developing beliefs about the relative importance of Spanish vs. English (Fuller and Leeman 2020). Furthermore, the differences in the types of words being used across the two languages may also imply that there are constraints in the use of Spanish, i.e., Spanish is appropriate to use in contexts related to family, but not beyond this context (e.g., Kyratzis 2010). Future research should investigate how these books affect children’s attitudes toward each language, and the consequences of this messaging.
An additional important remaining question concerns how the input provided by codeswitching picture books can actually shape young children’s language learning. Given that picture books in our sample present relatively more unique words in Spanish than in English, this book format may promote learning new Spanish vocabulary. Children readily learn words from shared reading (Flack et al. 2018), especially when they engage in dialogue about those words (Blewitt et al. 2009; Lugo-Neris et al. 2010; Penno et al. 2002). For example, in an intervention study with primarily Spanish-speaking children, Lugo-Neris et al. (2010) found that providing elaboration in Spanish when introducing novel English words during shared reading activities (“bridging”) promoted the acquisition of the novel words. Thus, the inclusion of Spanish words embedded in English text may potentially facilitate bridging, providing opportunities for parents to elaborate on new Spanish vocabulary. Furthermore, the inclusion of glossaries may support word learning for second language learners (Madiba 2010), particularly if they also encourage parents to define and discuss the Spanish words. However, it is also possible that the sporadic inclusion of Spanish words is unhelpful for learning. For instance, Spanish-only utterances in our sample were often single words (e.g., “And just like that, the mouse found her hat. “Adiós!” she said as she skipped away.” from The Fabulous Lost & Found and the Little Mouse who Spoke Spanish), and young bilinguals have been shown to be relatively less efficient to process isolated words (Morini and Newman 2019). Thus, it is an open question whether and how the presentation of Spanish vocabulary embedded within English text provides effective opportunities to learn Spanish words.
Given that picture books in our sample presented mixed-language utterances frequently, this book format may also present challenges beyond that of single-language texts. Research has suggested that mixed-language utterances may be relatively difficult for children to process in realtime, particularly when codeswitched words are presented in the child’s weaker language (Byers-Heinlein et al. 2017, 2020; Morini and Newman 2019; Potter et al. 2019). It could be that by introducing added processing demands, frequent switching in bilingual books could reduce the potential for learning, and that children would benefit more from books that present the languages more separately. Conversely, encountering complex, challenging input in shared reading might be especially supportive of children’s learning of two languages (Brouillard et al. 2020; Read et al. 2021b), just as the inclusion of unique words and complex structures in written text has been argued to support monolingual children’s language development (Fletcher and Reese 2005; Hoff-Ginsberg 1998; Montag et al. 2015). Future studies can explore these competing possibilities.
In order to understand what young learners may gain from the text in codeswitching picture books, it is also important to consider the language skills that they bring to reading interactions. Children’s vocabulary knowledge contributes to how well they acquire information from joint reading activities (Robbins and Ehri 1994; Sénéchal et al. 1995). Bilingual children have unequal experience and proficiency in each of their languages (Marchman et al. 2017; Pearson et al. 1997), and they are more efficient to process words in the language that they hear more often (Marchman et al. 2010; Potter et al. 2019), suggesting that they may not learn words equally easily across their two languages. As we have already suggested, the codeswitching picture books in our sample may be targeting English speakers learning Spanish vocabulary. Would Spanish speakers learning English benefit equally from English-dominant codeswitching books? A recent study found that Spanish speaking children learn English words from Spanish-dominant codeswitching books (Read et al. 2021b). For Spanish-speaking children, English-dominant codeswitching books may also provide opportunities to learn English words and grammatical structure, with the embedded Spanish vocabulary providing a bridge or a scaffold into the English text (Domke 2018; Leacox and Jackson 2014; Lugo-Neris et al. 2010). Given that codeswitching within text can serve several functions (Chaudhri and Torres 2021; Torres 2007), assessing what is being conveyed when a codeswitch occurs could clarify whether one language is being used to scaffold the content presented in the other language.
In addition to recognizing that children may vary in what they learn from the text most readily, we must also recognize how caregivers contribute to reading interactions with their child. A large literature has documented that children’s learning from shared reading can depend on features of the interactions, including how caregivers ask questions, provide comments and feedback, and elaborate on the story beyond what is in the text (Gómez et al. 2021; Jiménez et al. 2006; Whitehurst et al. 1988; Zucker et al. 2013). For children learning a second language in particular, the kinds of talk and behaviors they experience within language interactions is especially important for learning (Hoff and Core 2013; Hurtado et al. 2008) and caregivers vary with respect to the behaviors they incorporate into their interactions (Custode and Tamis-LeMonda 2020; Escobar and Tamis-LeMonda 2017; Luo et al. 2020; Winstone et al. 2021). Currently, it is not clear how codeswitching picture books may support or affect the extratextual talk and behaviors that caregivers provide during reading activities, but future studies could explore whether some books promote or impede high-quality reading experiences.
Moreover, reading interactions may have many potential targets for learning, including language-related goals, factual content, and lessons (Bergman Deitcher et al. 2019; Breitfeld et al. 2021). In our sample, the topic of family appeared to be a significant focus of Spanish content across books, and it could be that parents choose to read bilingual books in order to share cultural values or discuss particular topics. It has been widely suggested that when words and concepts are presented in predictable contexts, this supports their learning (Benitez and Saffran 2018; Benitez and Smith 2012; Chang and Deák 2019; Eiteljoerge et al. 2019; Roy et al. 2015; Schwab et al. 2018; Vlach and Sandhofer 2011; Wojcik and Saffran 2015), and picture books might be able to offer useful predictability. It could be that for Spanish-English bilingual families, some topics are more commonly discussed in one language than the other (e.g., family-related words in Spanish, school-related words in English), and perhaps the use of these concepts in books is reflecting and/or supporting wider usage patterns. Bilingual reading environments are complex, and our study represents an important first step in understanding how picture books might be used to support bilingual language skills. The next steps will include examining how caregivers interact with codeswitching books while reading with their children, how individual differences in children’s and/or caregivers’ language contribute to this interaction, and what language, cultural, and literacy knowledge children ultimately gain from these interactions.

5. Conclusions

Children’s picture books have the potential to provide rich opportunities for learning, and codeswitching picture books in particular provide children with different learning opportunities in English vs. Spanish. Specifically, although codeswitching picture books in English and Spanish contain more text in English and more complex text in English, they present relatively more unique words in Spanish, offering chances to learn new vocabulary in Spanish. Furthermore, the two languages are used in complex ways, and the text included higher amounts of within-utterance language switching than children typically hear in spoken language. Together, these findings indicate that Spanish-English codeswitching picture books may provide a unique source of language input for children learning a second language, but also highlight how much more research is needed to better understand and promote reading materials and practices that support bilingual language development.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.L.B. and C.E.P.; methodology, V.L.B., M.C., C.E.P.; investigation, M.C.; formal analysis, V.L.B. and M.C.; supervision: V.L.B. and C.E.P.; writing—review and editing, V.L.B., M.C. and C.E.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

All data, CLAN codes used to derive each measure, and R script for the main analyses reported in this manuscript are openly available on OSF: https://osf.io/sxm5t.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank undergraduate researchers Allya Hull and Skylar Keefer for assisting in conducting the search of bilingual picture books; Matt Guerra and Jessica Valencia for assisting in transcription and coding; and Karina Chimal, Noemi Rodriguez, and Maria Rodriguez, for assisting in transcription, coding, and running CLAN analyses.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. List of codeswitching picture books in our sample.
Table A1. List of codeswitching picture books in our sample.
BookAuthorPublisherPublication Year
A Box Full of KittensSonia ManzanoAtheneum2007
A New Barker in the HouseTomie dePaolaPutnam Juvenile2002
A Paintbrush for PacoTracey C. KyleLittle Bee Books2018
A Piñata in a Pine Tree: A Latino Twelve Days of ChristmasPat MoraClairon Books2009
AbuelaArthur DorrosPuffin Books1997
At Home with Betty & Cat en CasaHennie JacobsHennie Jacobs2015
Bebé Goes ShoppingSusan Middleton ElyaHarcourt2006
Besos for Baby: A Little Book of KissesJen ArenaLB Kids2014
Between Us and Abuela: A Family Story from the BorderMitali PerkinsMacmillan2019
Calavera Abecedario: A Day of the Dead Alphabet BookJeanette WinterHarcourt2006
Chato’s KitchenGary SotoPutnam Publishing Group1995
Dear Primo: A Letter to My CousinDuncan TonatiuhAbrams Books for Young Readers2010
El BarrioDeborah M. Newton ChocolateHenry Holt & Co1995
Fire! Fuego! Brave BomberosSusan Middleton ElyaBloomsbury2012
Gazpacho for NachoTracey C. KyleTwo Lions2014
Green Is a Chile Pepper: A Book of ColorsRoseanne ThongChronicle Books2014
I Love Saturdays y DomingosAlma Flor AdaAntheneum2004
Kitchen DanceMaurie ManningClairon Books2008
La Princesa and the PeaSusan Middleton ElyaG.P. Putnam’s Sons Books for Young Readers2017
Los Gatos Black on HalloweenMarisa MontesSquare Fish2006
Mañana, IguanaAnn PaulHoliday House2005
Mango, Abuela, and MeMeg MedinaCandlewick Press2015
Marta! Big & SmallJen ArenaRoaring Brook Press2008
My AbuelitaTony JohnstonHarcourt Children’s Books2009
My Papi Has a MotorcycleIsabel QuinteroKokila2019
N is for NavidadSusan Middleton Elya and Merry BanksChronicle Books2007
Niño Wrestles the WorldYuyi MoralesSquare Fish2013
Our Celebración!Susan Middleton ElyaLee & Low2018
Paletero ManLucky DiazHarperCollins2021
Papá and MeArthur DorrosHarperCollins2014
Planting Stories: The Life of Librarian and Storyteller Pura BelpréAnika DeniseHarperCollins2019
Poco LocoJ.R. Krause, Maria ChuaAmazon Children’s Publishing2013
Round Is a Tortilla: A Book of ShapesRoseanne ThongChronicle Books2015
Rubia and the Three OsosSusan Middleton ElyaScholastic2010
Say Hola to SpanishSusan Middleton ElyaLee & Low2013
Say Hola to Spanish at the CircusSusan Middleton ElyaLee & Low Books2013
Say Hola to Spanish Otra Vez (Again!)Susan Middleton ElyaLee & Low1999
Sonrisas for Baby: A Little Book of SmilesJen ArenaLB Kids2020
The Fabulous Lost & Found and the Little Mouse Who Spoke SpanishMark Pallis Neu Westen Press2020
The Fiesta Dress: A Quinceañera TaleCaren McNelly McCormackTwo Lions2012
The Piñata That the Farm Maiden HungSamantha R. VamosCharlesbridge2019
Tía Isa Wants a CarMeg MedinaCandlewick2011
Twas NochebuenaRoseanne Greenfield ThongViking Books for Young Readers2014
¡Vamos! Let’s Go Eat (World of ¡Vamos!)Raúl the ThirdVersify2020
¡Vamos! Let’s Go to the Market (World of ¡Vamos!) Raúl the ThirdVersify2019

References

  1. Agosto, Denise. 1997. Bilingual picture books: Libros para todos. School Library Journal 43: 38–39. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alamillo, Laura. 2017. Translanguaging con mi abuela: Chican@ children’s literature as a means to elevate language practices in our homes. In Voices of Resistance: Interdisciplinary Approaches to Chican@ Children’s Literature. Edited by Laura Alamillo, Larissa M. Mercado-López and Cristina Herrera. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson, Nina J., Susan A. Graham, Heather Prime, Jennifer M. Jenkins, and Sheri Madigan. 2021. Linking Quality and Quantity of Parental Linguistic Input to Child Language Skills: A Meta-Analysis. Child Development 92: 484–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bail, Amelie, Giovanna Morini, and Rochelle S. Newman. 2015. Look at the gato! Code-switching in speech to toddlers. Journal of Child Language 42: 1073–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Barrera, Rosalinda B., Ruth E. Quiroa, and Rebeca Valdivia. 2003. Spanish in Latino picture storybooks in English: Its use and textural effects. In Multicultural Issues in Literacy Research and Practice. Edited by Arlette Ingram Willis, Georgia Earnest García, Rosalinda B. Barrera and Violet J. Harris. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bauer, Eurydice Bouchereau. 2000. Code-switching during shared and independent reading: Lessons learned from a preschooler. Research in the Teaching of English 35: 101–30. [Google Scholar]
  7. Benitez, Viridiana L., and Jenny R. Saffran. 2018. Predictable events enhance word learning in toddlers. Current Biology 28: 2787–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Benitez, Viridiana L., and Linda B. Smith. 2012. Predictable locations aid early object name learning. Cognition 125: 339–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Bentahila, Abdelâli, and Eirlys E. Davies. 1995. Patterns of code-switching and patterns of language contact. Lingua 96: 75–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bergman Deitcher, Deborah, Dorit Aram, and Gali Adar. 2019. Book selection for shared reading: Parents’ considerations and researchers’ views. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 19: 291–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Blewitt, Pamela, Keiran M. Rump, Stephanie E. Shealy, and Samantha A. Cook. 2009. Shared book reading: When and how questions affect young children’s word learning. Journal of Educational Psychology 101: 294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Botelho, Maria José, and Robin Marion. 2020. Representing Spanishes: Language Diversity in Children’s Literature. Journal of Latinos and Education. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Breitfeld, Elise, Christine E. Potter, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2021. Children Simultaneously Learn Multiple Dimensions of Information during Shared Book Reading. Journal of Cognition and Development 22: 744–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Brouillard, Melanie, Daphnée Dubé, and Krista Byers-Heinlein. 2020. Reading to Bilingual Preschoolers: An Experimental Study of Two Book Formats. PsyArXiv, March 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Bullock, Barbara E., and Almeida Jacqueline Toribio. 2009. Themes in the Study of Code-Switching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Byers-Heinlein, Krista. 2013. Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children’s vocabulary size. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 32–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Byers-Heinlein, Krista, Amel Jardak, Eva Fourakis, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2020. Effects of Language Mixing on Bilingual Children’s Word Learning. PsyArXiv, in press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Byers-Heinlein, Krista, Elizabeth Morin-Lessard, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2017. Bilingual infants control their languages as they listen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114: 9032–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Cantone, Katja F. 2007. Code-Switching in Bilingual Children. Dordrecht: Springer, vol. 296. [Google Scholar]
  20. Castro, Dina C., Mariela M. Páez, David K. Dickinson, and Ellen Frede. 2011. Promoting language and literacy in young dual language learners: Research, practice, and policy. Child Development Perspectives 5: 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Cha, Kijoo, and Claude Goldenberg. 2015. The complex relationship between bilingual home language input and kindergarten children’s Spanish and English oral proficiencies. Journal of Educational Psychology 107: 935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Chang, Lucas M., and Gedeon O. Deák. 2019. Maternal discourse continuity and infants’ actions organize 12-month-olds’ language exposure during object play. Developmental Science 22: e12770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Chappell, Sharon, and Christian Faltis. 2007. Spanglish, bilingualism, culture and identity in Latino children’s literature. Children’s Literature in Education 38: 253–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Chaudhri, Amina, and Lourdes Torres. 2021. “Jugos There?” Codeswitching Strategies in Bilingual Picturebooks. Children’s Literature in Education. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chen, Xiaoning. 2019. Selecting and using dual language books in classrooms and beyond. NABE Journal of Research and Practice 9: 191–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Clark, Ellen Riojas, Belinda Bustos Flores, Howard L. Smith, and Daniel Alejandro Gonzalez. 2015. Multicultural Literature for Latino Bilingual Children: Their Words, Their Worlds. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield. [Google Scholar]
  27. Custode, Stephanie A., and Catherine Tamis-LeMonda. 2020. Cracking the code: Social and contextual cues to language input in the home environment. Infancy 25: 809–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. David, Annabelle, and Li Wei. 2008. Individual differences in the lexical development of French–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 11: 598–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Dávila, Denise, Silvia Noguerón, and Max Vasquez-Dominguez. 2017. The Latinx family: Learning y La Literatura at the library. Bilingual Review/Revista Bilingüe 33: 33–49. [Google Scholar]
  30. Dawson, Nicola, Yaling Hsiao, Alvin Wei Min Tan, Nilanjana Banerji, and Kate Nation. 2021. Features of lexical richness in children’s books: Comparisons with child-directed speech. Language Development Research 1: 1–45. [Google Scholar]
  31. Deckner, Deborah F., Lauren B. Adamson, and Roger Bakeman. 2006. Child and maternal contributions to shared reading: Effects on language and literacy development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 27: 31–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Domke, Lisa M. 2018. Probing the Promise of Dual-Language Books. Reading Horizons: A Journal of Literacy and Language Arts 57: 3. [Google Scholar]
  33. Duursma, Elisabeth, Silvia Romero-Contreras, Anna Szuber, Patrick Proctor, Catherine Snow, Diane August, and Margarita Calderón. 2007. The role of home literacy and language environment on bilinguals’ English and Spanish vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 171–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Eiteljoerge, Sarah F. V., Maurits Adam, Birgit Elsner, and Nivedita Mani. 2019. Word-object and action-object association learning across early development. PLoS ONE 14: e0220317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Ernst-Slavit, Gisela, and Margaret Mulhern. 2003. Bilingual books: Promoting literacy and biliteracy in the second-language and mainstream classroom. Reading Online 7: 1096–232. [Google Scholar]
  36. Escobar, Kelly, and Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda. 2017. Conceptualizing variability in US Latino children’s dual-language development. In Handbook on Positive Development of Minority Children and Youth. Cham: Springer, pp. 89–106. [Google Scholar]
  37. Farrant, Brad M., and Stephen R. Zubrick. 2012. Early vocabulary development: The importance of joint attention and parent-child book reading. First Language 32: 343–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Fennell, Christopher, and Krista Byers-Heinlein. 2014. You sound like Mommy: Bilingual and monolingual infants learn words best from speakers typical of their language environments. International Journal of Behavioral Development 38: 309–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Fitton, Lisa, Autumn L. McIlraith, and Carla L. Wood. 2018. Shared book reading interventions with English learners: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research 88: 712–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Flack, Zoe M., Andy P. Field, and Jessica S. Horst. 2018. The effects of shared storybook reading on word learning: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology 54: 1334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Fletcher, Kathryn L., and Elaine Reese. 2005. Picture book reading with young children: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review 25: 64–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Fuller, Janet M., and Jennifer Leeman. 2020. Language Ideologies. In Speaking Spanish in the US. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 63–90. [Google Scholar]
  43. Gallagher, Kay, and Afaf Bataineh. 2020. An investigation into the linguistic landscape of translingual storybooks for Arabic-English bilingual children. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 41: 348–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. García, Ofelia, and Jo Anne Kleifgen. 2020. Translanguaging and literacies. Reading Research Quarterly 55: 553–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Glazer, Morgan, Kara Harris, Maria Anne Ceprano, Dottie Ost, and Mariah Gower. 2017. Creating bilingual books to facilitate second language learning. Journal of Inquiry and Action in Education 8: 5. [Google Scholar]
  46. Gómez, Ligia E., M. Adelaida Restrepo, Arthur M. Glenberg, and Erin Walker. 2021. Enhancing question asking during shared reading in immigrant Latino families. Journal of Latinos and Education. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Gomm, Jeff, Melissa Allen Heath, and Pat Mora. 2017. Analysis of Latino award winning children’s literature. School Psychology International 38: 507–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gonzalez-Barrero, Ana Maria, Nicholas Salama-Siroishka, Daphnée Dubé, Melanie Brouillard, and Krista Byers-Heinlein. 2021. Effects of language dominance on home reading practices of bilingual families. International Journal of Bilingualism 25: 77–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Goodz, Naomi Singerman. 1989. Parental language mixing in bilingual families. Infant Mental Health Journal 10: 25–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hadaway, Nancy L., and Terrell A. Young. 2013. Celebrating and revitalizing language: Indigenous bilingual children’s books. Bookbird: A Journal of International Children’s Literature 51: 58–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hammer, Carol Scheffner, Eugene Komaroff, Barbara L. Rodriguez, Lisa M. Lopez, Shelley E. Scarpino, and Brian Goldstein. 2012. Predicting Spanish–English bilingual children’s language abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 55: 1251–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hayes, Andrew F., and Klaus Krippendorff. 2007. Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures 1: 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hoff, Erika, and Cynthia Core. 2013. Input and language development in bilingually developing children. Seminars in Speech and Language 34: 215–26. [Google Scholar]
  54. Hoff, Erika, Cynthia Core, Silvia Place, Rosario Rumiche, Melissa Señor, and Marisol Parra. 2012. Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language 39: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Hoff, Erika, Jamie M. Quinn, and David Giguere. 2018. What explains the correlation between growth in vocabulary and grammar? New evidence from latent change score analyses of simultaneous bilingual development. Developmental Science 21: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika. 1998. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics 19: 603–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Hojeij, Zeina, Anna M. Dillon, Alecia Perkins, and Ian Grey. 2019. Selecting high quality dual language texts for young children in multicultural contexts: A UAE case. Issues in Educational Research 29: 1201–23. [Google Scholar]
  58. Hu, Ran, Xiaoning Chen, and Xiuping Li. 2012. Exploring bilingual books with five Chinese first graders: Children’s responses and biliteracy development. Reading Horizons: Journal of Literacy and Language Arts 52: 57–87. [Google Scholar]
  59. Hurtado, Nereyda, Virginia A. Marchman, and Anne Fernald. 2008. Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science 11: 31–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Isurin, Ludmila, Donald Winford, and Kees De Bot. 2009. Multidisciplinary Approaches to Code Switching. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  61. Jaffe, Alexandra. 2003. Talk around text: Literacy practices, cultural identity and authority in a Corsican bilingual classroom. In Multilingual Classroom Ecologies. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 42–60. [Google Scholar]
  62. Jeffers, Dawn. 2009. Bilingual books for ESL students and beyond. In Children and Libraries. Edited by Sharon Verbeten. Chicago: Association for Library Service to Children, vol. 7, pp. 38–39. [Google Scholar]
  63. Jiménez, Terese C., Alexis L. Filippini, and Michael M. Gerber. 2006. Shared reading within Latino families: An analysis of reading interactions and language use. Bilingual Research Journal 30: 431–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Kabuto, Bobbie. 2010. Code-switching during parent—Child reading interactions: Taking multiple theoretical perspectives. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 10: 131–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kelly, Laura B. 2020. An analysis of Spanish use in award-winning children’s picturebooks. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 42: 663–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Kremin, Lena V., Julia Alves, Adriel John Orena, Linda Polka, and Byers-Heinlein Krista. 2021. Code-switching in parents’ everyday speech to bilingual infants. Journal of Child Language. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Krippendorff, Klaus. 2004. Content analysis. In An Introduction to Its Methodology, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications Inc., pp. 1–422. [Google Scholar]
  68. Krippendorff, Klaus. 2011a. Agreement and information in the reliability of coding. Communication Methods and Measures 5: 93–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Krippendorff, Klaus. 2011b. Computing Krippendorff’s alpha-reliability. Scholarly Commons 43: 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  70. Kyratzis, Amy. 2010. Latina girls’ peer play interactions in a bilingual Spanish-English US preschool: Heteroglossia, frame-shifting, and language ideology. Pragmatics 20: 557–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Lang, Mervyn F. 2013. Spanish word formation. In Theoretical Linguistics, 1st ed. London: Routledge, vol. 1, pp. 1–284. [Google Scholar]
  72. Leacox, Lindsey, and Carla W. Jackson. 2014. Spanish vocabulary-bridging technology-enhanced instruction for young English language learners’ word learning. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 14: 174–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Li, Liang, and Marilyn Fleer. 2015. Family pedagogy: Parent–Child interaction in shared book reading. Early Child Development and Care 185: 1944–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Logan, Jessica A., Laura M. Justice, Melike Yumus, and Leydi J. Chaparro-Moreno. 2019. When children are not read to at home: The million word gap. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics 40: 386. [Google Scholar]
  75. Lugo-Neris, Mirza J., Carla Wood-Jackson, and Howard Goldstein. 2010. Facilitating vocabulary acquisition of young English language learners. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 41: 314–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Luo, Rufan, Kelly Escobar, and Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda. 2020. Heterogeneity in the trajectories of the US Latine mothers’ dual-language input from infancy to preschool. First Language 40: 275–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. MacWhinney, Brian. 2000. The Childes Project. In Tools for Analyzing Talk, 3rd ed. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, vol. 2, pp. 1–119. [Google Scholar]
  78. Madiba, Mbulungeni. 2010. Fast-tracking concept learning to English as an additional language (EAL) students through corpus-based multilingual glossaries. Alternation 17: 225–48. [Google Scholar]
  79. Marchman, Virginia A., Fernald Anne, and Hurtado Nereyda. 2010. How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 37: 817–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Marchman, Virginia A., Lucía Z. Martínez, Nereyda Hurtado, Theres Grüter, and Anne Fernald. 2017. Caregiver talk to young Spanish-English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science 20: 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Martínez-Roldán, Carmen M., and Guillermo Malavé. 2004. Language ideologies mediating literacy and identity in bilingual contexts. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 4: 155–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Méndez, Lucía I., Elizabeth R. Crais, Dina C. Castro, and Kristen Kainz. 2015. A culturally and linguistically responsive vocabulary approach for young Latino dual language learners. Journal of Speech 1: 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Montag, Jessica L., Michael N. Jones, and Linda B. Smith. 2015. The words children hear: Picture books and the statistics for language learning. Psychological Science 26: 1489–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Montanari, Simona, Robert Mayr, and Kaveri Subrahmanyam. 2020. Speech and language outcomes in low-SES Spanish-English bilingual preschoolers: The role of maternal education. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Moody, Stephanie M., Sharon D. Matthews, and Zohreh R. Eslami. 2021. Translanguaging during shared read alouds: A case study. Literacy Research and Instruction 2: 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Moreno-Sandoval, Antonio, and José Miguel Goñi-Menoyo. 2002. Spanish inflectional morphology in DATR. Journal of Logic 11: 79–105. [Google Scholar]
  87. Morini, Giovanna, and Rochelle S. Newman. 2019. Dónde está la ball? Examining the effect of code switching on bilingual children’s word recognition. Journal of Child Language 46: 1238–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Muysken, Pieter. 2000. Bilingual Speech: A Typology of Codemixing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  89. Muysken, Pieter, Kook Hetty, and Paul Vedder. 1996. Papiamento/Dutch code-switching in bilingual parent–Child reading. Applied Psycholinguistics 17: 485–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Myers-Scotton, Carol. 1997. Duelling Languages: Grammatical Structure in Codeswitching. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
  91. Naidoo, Jamie Campbell. 2011. Celebrating cuentos. In Promoting Latino Children’s Literature and Literacy in Classrooms and Libraries. Santa Barbara: Libraries Unlimited Press. [Google Scholar]
  92. Naidoo, Jamie C., and Julia López-Robertson. 2007. Descubriendo el sabor: Spanish bilingual book publishing and cultural authenticity. Multicultural Review 4: 24–37. [Google Scholar]
  93. Naqvi, Rahat, Keoma J. Thorne, Christina M. Pfitscher, David W. Nordstokke, and Anne McKeough. 2012. Reading dual language books: Improving early literacy skills in linguistically diverse classrooms. Journal of Early Childhood Research 11: 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Nicoladis, Elena, and Fred Genesee. 1998. Parental discourse and codemixing in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism 2: 85–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Nicoladis, Elena, and Giovanni Secco. 2000. The role of a child’s productive vocabulary in the language choice of a bilingual family. First Language 1: 3–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Oller, Kimbrough, Barbara Pearson, and Alan Cobo-Lewis. 2007. Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 191–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Pan, Barbara Alexander. 1995. Code negotiation in bilingual families: My body starts speaking English. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 4: 315–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Pearson, Barbara, Sylvia Fernandez, Vanessa Lewedeg, and Kimbrough Oller. 1997. The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 18: 41–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Penno, Julie F., Ian A. G. Wilkinson, and Dennis W. Moore. 2002. Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educational Psychology 94: 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Place, Silvia, and Erica Hoff. 2011. Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development 82: 1834–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Pontier, Ryan, and Mileidis Gort. 2016. Coordinated translanguaging pedagogy as distributed cognition: A case study of two dual language bilingual education preschool co teachers’ languaging practices during shared book readings. International Multilingual Research Journal 10: 89–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Potter, Christine E., Eva Fourakis, Elizabeth Morin-Lessard, Krista Byers-Heinlein, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2019. Bilingual toddlers’ comprehension of mixed sentences is asymmetrical across their two languages. Developmental Science 22: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  103. Raikes, Helen, Barbara Alexander Pan, Gayle Luze, Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Jill Constantine, Louisa Banks Tarullo, H. Abigail Raikes, and Eileen T. Rodriguez. 2006. Mother-child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development 77: 924–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Ramírez-Esparza, Nairán, Adrián García-Sierra, and Patricia K. Kuhl. 2017. The Impact of Early Social Interactions on Later Language Development in Spanish–English Bilingual Infants. Child Development 88: 1216–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Read, Kirsten, Paloma D. Contreras, and Hector Martinez. 2021a. Tres formas: Shared reading practices with three types of Spanish and English dual-language learning preschoolers. Bilingual Research Journal 44: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Read, Kirsten, Paloma D. Contreras, Bianca Rodriguez, and Jessica Jara. 2021b. ¿Read conmigo?: The effect of code-switching storybooks on dual-language learners’ retention of new vocabulary. Early Education and Development 32: 516–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Restrepo, Morgan A., Gareth P. Morgan, and Marilyn S. Thompson. 2013. The efficacy of a vocabulary intervention for dual-language learners with language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 56: 748–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Robbins, Claudia, and Linnea C. Ehri. 1994. Reading storybooks to kindergartners helps them learn new vocabulary words. Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 54–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Rodríguez-Valls, Fernando. 2011. Coexisting languages: Reading bilingual books with biliterate eyes. Bilingual Research Journal 34: 19–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Rowe, Meredith L. 2012. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development 83: 1762–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Roy, Brandon C., Michael C. Frank, Philip DeCamp, Matthew Miller, and Deb Roy. 2015. Predicting the birth of a spoken word. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112: 12663–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  112. Schwab, Jessica F., Meredith L. Rowe, Natasha Cabrera, and Casey Lew-Williams. 2018. Fathers’ repetition of words is coupled with children’s vocabularies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 166: 437–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Semingson, Peggy, Kathryn Pole, and Jodi Tommerdahl. 2015. Using bilingual books to enhance literacy around the world. European Scientific Journal 11: 132–39. [Google Scholar]
  114. Sénéchal, Monique, and Jo-Ann Lefevre. 2002. Continuity and change in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Development 85: 1552–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  115. Sénéchal, Monique, Eleanor Thomas, and Jo-Ann Monker. 1995. Individual differences in 4-year-old children’s acquisition of vocabulary during storybook reading. Journal of Educational Psychology 87: 218–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Showstack, Rachel E. 2017. Stancetaking and language ideologies in heritage language learner classroom discourse. Journal of Language, Identity & Education 16: 271–84. [Google Scholar]
  117. Song, Kwangok. 2016. “Okay, I will say in Korean and then in American”: Translanguaging practices in bilingual homes. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 16: 84–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Sperry, Linda L., and Douglas E. Sperry. 1996. Early development of narrative skills. Cognitive Development 11: 443–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Taaffe Young, Kathryn, Karen Davis, Cathy Schoen, and Steven Parker. 1998. Listening to parents. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 152: 255–62. [Google Scholar]
  120. Tare, Medha, and Susan A. Gelman. 2011. Bilingual parents’ modeling of pragmatic language use in multiparty interactions. Applied Psycholinguistics 32: 761–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  121. Thibeault, Joël, and Ian A. Matheson. 2020. The cross-linguistic reading strategies used by elementary students in French immersion as they engage with dual-language children’s books. Canadian Modern Language Review 76: 375–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Torres, Lourdes. 2007. In the contact zone: Code-switching strategies by Latino/a writers. Oxford University Press on Behalf of Society for the Study of the Multi-Ethnic Literature of the United States 32: 77–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Tsybina, Irina, and Alice Eriks-Brophy. 2010. Bilingual dialogic book-reading intervention for preschoolers with slow expressive vocabulary development. Journal of Communication Disorders 43: 538–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. U.S. Census Bureau. 2019. Language Spoken at Home. TableID: S1601. Available online: https://data.census.gov/cedsci/table?q=languages%20spoken%20at%20home&tid=ACSST1Y2019.S1601 (accessed on 19 November 2021).
  125. Vlach, Haley A., and Catherine M. Sandhofer. 2011. Developmental differences in children’s context-dependent word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108: 394–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Vogel, Sara, and Ofelia García. 2017. Translanguaging. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Walker, Sue, Viv Edwards, and Ruth Blacksell. 1996. Designing bilingual books for children. Visible Language 30: 269–83. [Google Scholar]
  128. Weisleder, Adriana, and Anne Fernald. 2013. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science 24: 2143–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Whitehurst, Grover J., Francine L. Falco, Christopher J. Lonigan, Janet E. Fischel, Barbara D. DeBaryshe, Marta C. Valdez-Menchaca, and Marie Caulfield. 1988. Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Developmental Psychology 24: 552–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Winstone, Laura K., Viridiana L. Benitez, and Lauren Van Huisstede. 2021. Patterns of maternal interactive behaviors and dual vocabulary development in Mexican-American children. Developmental Psychology 57: 1866–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Wojcik, Erica H., and Jenny R. Saffran. 2015. Toddlers encode similarities among novel words from meaningful sentences. Cognition 138: 10–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  132. Zaidi, Rahat. 2020. Dual-language books: Enhancing engagement and language awareness. Journal of Literacy Research 52: 269–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Zipf, George Kingsley. 1949. Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort: An Introduction to Human Ecology. Cambridge: Addison Wesley. [Google Scholar]
  134. Zucker, Tricia A., Sonia Q. Cabell, Laura M. Justice, Jill M. Pentimonti, and Joan N. Kaderavek. 2013. Role of frequent, interactive prekindergarten shared reading in the longitudinal development of language and literacy skills. Developmental Psychology 49: 1425–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Mean (and standard error of the mean) for (A) Word tokens, (B) Word types, and (C) Number of utterances for English and Spanish within picture books in our sample. Dots represent individual picture books.
Figure 1. Mean (and standard error of the mean) for (A) Word tokens, (B) Word types, and (C) Number of utterances for English and Spanish within picture books in our sample. Dots represent individual picture books.
Languages 07 00069 g001
Figure 2. Mean (and standard error of the mean) for (A) Type/Token ratio and (B) MLU for English and Spanish within picture books in our sample. Dots represent individual picture books.
Figure 2. Mean (and standard error of the mean) for (A) Type/Token ratio and (B) MLU for English and Spanish within picture books in our sample. Dots represent individual picture books.
Languages 07 00069 g002
Figure 3. Histogram of (A) percent of utterances that contain both languages (within-utterance switches), and (B) percent of between-utterance switches. Dashed lines represent the sample mean.
Figure 3. Histogram of (A) percent of utterances that contain both languages (within-utterance switches), and (B) percent of between-utterance switches. Dashed lines represent the sample mean.
Languages 07 00069 g003
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for quantitative and qualitative features of the text.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for quantitative and qualitative features of the text.
MeanSD
Word tokens
   English503.71336.35
   Spanish78.1389.29
Word types
   English218.2789.29
   Spanish43.3643.32
Number of utterances
   English50.4244.64
   Spanish9.5613.73
Type/token ratio
   English0.480.12
   Spanish0.600.22
Mean length of utterance
   English6.252.68
   Spanish1.881.62
Table 2. Raw counts of 10 most frequent words per language.
Table 2. Raw counts of 10 most frequent words per language.
EnglishSpanish
Frequency aPercentage b Frequency aPercentage b
The13385.0%Abuela752.1%
And9233.4%El712.0%
A7422.8%Mamá601.7%
Is6612.5%La541.5%
To6522.4%Mami541.5%
I6042.3%Abuelita491.4%
In3571.3%Papá471.3%
Of3531.3%Un441.3%
My3131.2%Y441.3%
It2931.1%Tía391.1%
a Total number of tokens appearing in the full corpus. b Percentage of total word tokens in that language.
Table 3. Top 5 most frequent words per language and translation equivalents.
Table 3. Top 5 most frequent words per language and translation equivalents.
Top English Words
Frequency aPercentage bTranslation Equivalent(s) cFrequency aPercentage d
The13385.0%El (La, Los, Las)71 (179)5.0%
And9233.4%Y442.0%
A7422.8%Un (Una)44 (62)1.7%
Is6612.5%Es (Está)29 (33)0.9%
To6522.4%A220.6%
Top Spanish Words
Frequency aPercentage bTranslation Equivalent(s) cFrequency aPercentage d
Abuela752.1%Grandma
(Grandmother)
21 (31)0.1%
El712.0%The13385.0%
Mamá601.7%Mom17<0.1%
La541.5%The13385.0%
Mami541.5%Mommy10.0%
a Total number of tokens appearing in the full corpus. Numbers in parentheses include all close variants (italicized), not just the primary token. b Percentage of total word tokens in that language. c Synonyms and other close variants (in parentheses) in the other language. d Percentage of total word tokens in the language of the translation equivalent(s), calculated including all close variants.
Table 4. 5 most frequent nouns per language, including close variants and plurals.
Table 4. 5 most frequent nouns per language, including close variants and plurals.
EnglishSpanish
Frequency aPercentage b Frequency aPercentage b
Day (Days)69 (87)0.3%Abuela (Abuelas, Abuelita, Abuelitas)75 (125)3.5%
School340.1%Mamá (Mamás, Mami, Mamis, Madre, Madres)60 (116)3.2.%
Way340.1%Papá (Papás, Papi, Papis, Padre, Padres)47 (77)2.2%
Home (Homes)28 (30)0.1%Tía (Tías)39 (42)1.2%
People (Person)26 (30)0.1%Piñata (Piñatas)35 (36)0.1%
a Total number of tokens appearing in the full corpus. Numbers in parentheses include all close variants (italicized), not just the primary token. b Percentage of total word tokens in that language, calculated including all close variants.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Benitez, V.L.; Castellana, M.; Potter, C.E. How Many Palabras? Codeswitching and Lexical Diversity in Spanish-English Picture Books. Languages 2022, 7, 69. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7010069

AMA Style

Benitez VL, Castellana M, Potter CE. How Many Palabras? Codeswitching and Lexical Diversity in Spanish-English Picture Books. Languages. 2022; 7(1):69. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7010069

Chicago/Turabian Style

Benitez, Viridiana L., Marissa Castellana, and Christine E. Potter. 2022. "How Many Palabras? Codeswitching and Lexical Diversity in Spanish-English Picture Books" Languages 7, no. 1: 69. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7010069

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop