The Writing Process and the Written Product in Bimodal Bilingual Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Bimodal Bilingualism
3. Bimodal Bilingual Studies of the DHH and CODA Group
4. Written Product
5. Writing Process
6. Method
6.1. Participants
6.2. Language Backgrounds
6.3. Writing Task and Procedure
6.4. Keylogging Tool
7. Analysis
7.1. Measures of the Written Product
7.2. Measures of the Writing Process
7.3. Statistical Analysis
8. Results
8.1. Group Level Statistical Analysis between DHH and CODA Children
8.2. Correlation Analysis
9. Discussion
10. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Ambrose, Sophie E., Marc E. Fey, and Laurie S. Eisenberg. 2012. Phonological Awareness and Print Knowledge of Preschool Children with Cochlear Implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 55: 811–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Amraei, Kourosh, Susan Amirsalari, and Mohammed Ajalloueyan. 2017. Comparison of Intelligence Quotients of First- and Second-Generation Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 92: 167–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arfé, Barbara, Sara Ghiselli, and Silvia Montino. 2016. The Written Language of Children with Cochlear Implant. Hearing, Balance and Communication 14: 103–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Asker-Árnason, Lena, Tina Ibertsson, Malin Wass, Åsa Wengelin, and Birgitta Sahlén. 2010. Picture-Elicited Written Narratives, Process and Product, in 18 Children with Cochlear Implants. Communication Disorders Quarterly 31: 195–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Asker-Árnason, Lena, Viktoria Åkerlund, Cecilia Skoglund, Ingela Ek-Lagergren, Åsa Wengelin, and Birgitta Sahlén. 2012. Spoken and written narratives in Swedish children and adolescents with hearing impairment. Communication Disorders Quarterly 33: 131–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2009. Bilingualism: The Good, the Bad, and the Indifferent. Bilingualism 12: 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus Craik, and Gigi Luk. 2008. Cognitive Control and Lexical Access in Younger and Older Bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 34: 859–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brackenbury, Tim, Tiffany Ryan, and Trinka Messenheimer. 2006. Incidental word learning in a hearing child of deaf adults. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11: 76–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chabal, Sarah, and Viorica Marian. 2015. Speakers of Different Languages Process the Visual World Differently. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 144: 539–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chen-Pichler, Deborah, Diane Lillo-Martin, and Jeffrey Levi Palmer. 2018. A Short Introduction to Heritage Signers. Sign Language Studies 18: 309–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cormier, Kearsy, Eleni Orfanidou, and Adam Schembri. 2012. First Language Acquisition Differs from Second Language Acquisition in Prelingually Deaf Signers: Evidence from Sensitivity to Grammaticality Judgement in British Sign Language. Cognition 124: 50–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Davidson, Kathryn, Diane Lillo-Martin, and Deborah Chen Pichler. 2014. Spoken English Language Development among Native Signing Children with Cochlear Implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 19: 239–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duñabeitia, Jon Andoni, Adelina Estévez, Pedro Macizo, Juan Andrés Hernández, Manuel Carreiras, Eneko Antón, and Luis J. Fuentes. 2013. The Inhibitory Advantage in Bilingual Children Revisited. Experimental Psychology 61: 234–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Emmorey, Karen, Gigi Luk, Jennie E. Pyers, and Ellen Bialystok. 2008a. The Source of Enhanced Cognitive Control in Bilinguals. Psychological Science 19: 1201–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Emmorey, Karen, Helsa B. Borinstein, Robin Thompson, and Tamar H. Gollan. 2008b. Bimodal Bilingualism. Bilingualism 11: 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flower, Linda, and John R. Hayes. 1981. A Cognitive Process Theory of Writing. College Composition and Communication 32: 365–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gärdenfors, Moa. n.d. Development of Writing in Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students. Submitted.
- Gärdenfors, Moa, Victoria Johansson, and Krister Schönström. 2019. Spelling in Deaf, Hard of Hearing and Hearing Children with Sign Language Knowledge. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giezen, Marcel R., Henrike K. Blumenfeld, Anthony Shook, Viorica Marian, and Karen Emmorey. 2015. Parallel Language Activation and Inhibitory Control in Bimodal Bilinguals. Cognition 141: 9–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Giezen, Marcel R., and Karen Emmorey. 2017. Evidence for a Bimodal Bilingual Disadvantage in Letter Fluency. Bilingualism 20: 42–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Goodwin, Corina, Kathryn Davidson, and Diane Lillo-Martin. 2017. English Article Use in Bimodal Bilingual Children with Cochlear Implants: Effects of Language Transfer and Early Language Exposure. In Proceedings of the 41st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Edited by Maria LaMendola and Jennifer Scott. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 283–95. [Google Scholar]
- Hall, Matthew L., and Sheila Dills. 2020. The Limits of ‘Communication Mode’ as a Construct. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 25: 383–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halliday, Michael. 1985. Spoken and Written Language. Victoria: Deakin University Press, pp. 61–75. [Google Scholar]
- Hassanzadeh, S. 2012. Outcomes of Cochlear Implantation in Deaf Children of Deaf Parents: Comparative Study. Journal of Laryngology and Otology 126: 989–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, John R., and Linda S. Flower. 1980. Identifying the Organization of Writing Processes. In Cognitive Processes in Writing. Edited by Lee W. Gregg and Erwin R. Steinberg. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 5–30. [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann, Kristin, and Solveig Chilla. 2015. Bimodal bilingual language development of hearing children of deaf parents. European Journal of Special Needs Education 30: 30–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holmström, Ingela. 2018. Undervisning i Svenskt Teckenspråk Som Andraspråk. En Rapport from UTL2-Projektet; Stockholm. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/327176864_Undervisning_i_svenskt_teckensprak_som_andrasprak_En_rapport_fran_UTL2-projektet_Forskning_om_teckensprak_XXVI_FOT-rapport (accessed on 22 December 2020).
- Humphries, Tom, Poorna Kushalnagar, Gaurav Mathur, Donna J. Napoli, Carol Padden, Christian Rathmann, and Scott R. Smith. 2012. Language Acquisition for Deaf Children: Reducing the Harms of Zero Tolerance to the Use of Alternative Approaches. Harm Reduction Journal 9: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Humphries, Tom, Raja Kushalnagar, Gaurav Mathur, Donna Jo Napoli, Carol Padden, Christian Rathmann, and Scott Smith. 2013. The Right to Language. Journal of Law, Medicine and Ethics 41: 872–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Johansson, Victoria. 2009. Developmental Aspects of Text Production in Writing and Speech. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Lund, Lund, Sweden. [Google Scholar]
- Kellogg, R. 2008. Training Writing Skills: A Cognitive Developmental Perspective. Journal of Writing Research 1: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Lario, Julio Roca, Rosa M. Manchón, and Liz Murphy. 2006. Generating Text in Native and Foreign Language Writing: A Temporal Analysis of Problem-Solving Formulation Processes. Modern Language Journal 90: 100–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leijten, Mariëlle, and Luuk Van Waes. 2013. Keystroke Logging in Writing Research: Using Inputlog to Analyze and Visualize Writing Processes. Written Communication 30: 358–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lindgren, Eva, Asbjørg Westum, Hanna Outakoski, and Kirk P. H. Sullivan. 2019. Revising at the Leading Edge: Shaping Ideas or Clearing up Noise. In Observing Writing: Insights from Keystroke Logging and Handwriting. Edited by Eva Lindgren and Kirk P. H. Sullivan. Boston: Brill Academic Publishers, pp. 346–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindgren, Eva, Kristyan Spelman Miller, and Kirk P. H. Sullivan. 2008. Development of Fluency and Revision in L1 and L2 Writing in Swedish High School Years Eight and Nine. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 156: 33–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacArthur, Charles A. 2016. Instruction in Evaluation and Revision. In Handbook of Writing Research, 2nd ed. Edited by J. MacArthur, Charles A. Graham and Steve Fitzgerald. New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- MacWhinney, Brian. 2000. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, 3rd Edition. Computational Linguistics 26: 657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malvern, D. David, Brian J. Richards, Ngoni Chipere, and Pilar Durán. 2004. Lexical Diversity and Language Development. London: Palgrave Macmillan. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mauldin, Laura. 2012. Parents of Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants: A Study of Technology and Community. Sociology of Health and Illness 34: 529–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayberry, Rachel I. 2007. When Timing Is Everything: Age of First-Language Acquisition Effects on Second-Language Learning. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 537–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCutchen, Deborah. 1996. A Capacity Theory of Writing: Working Memory in Composition. Educational Psychology Review 8: 299–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCutchen, Deborah. 2000. Knowledge, Processing, and Working Memory: Implications for a Theory of Writing. Educational Psychologist 35: 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitchell, Ross E. 2006. How Many Deaf People Are There in the United States? Estimates from the Survey of Income and Program Participation. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 11: 112–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Morford, Jill P., Erin Wilkinson, Agnes Villwock, Pilar Piñar, and Judith F. Kroll. 2011. When Deaf Signers Read English: Do Written Words Activate Their Sign Translations? Cognition 118: 286–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oliveira, Kryssia Layane Santos de, Antonio Lucas Ferreira Feitosa, Gabriel Trevizani Depolli, and Cristiane Monteiro Pedruzzi. 2020. Reading and Writing Performance in Cochlear Implant Users: Integrative Review. Audiology -Communication Research 25: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palviainen, Åsa, Paula Kalaja, and Katja Mäntylä. 2012. Development of L2 Writing: Fluency and Proficiency. AFinLA-e Soveltavan Kielitieteen Tutkimuksia 2012: 47–59. [Google Scholar]
- Polinsky, Maria. 2018. Sign Languages in the Context of Heritage Language: A new Direction in Language Research. Sign Language Studies 18: 412–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2017. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. In R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Core Team, Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 February 2021).
- Schönström, Krister, and Ingela Holmström. 2017. Elicited Imitation Tasks (EITs) as a Tool for Measuring Sign Language Proficiency in L1 and L2 Signers. In Proceedings of the 6th Conference on Learning and Assessment: Making the Connections. Edited by Esther Gutiérrez Eugenio. Bologna: Alte, pp. 6–7. [Google Scholar]
- Schoonen, Rob, Patrick Snellings, Marie Stevenson, and Amos Van Gelderen. 2009. Towards a Blueprint of the Foreign Language Writer: The Linguistic and Cognitive Demands of Foreign Language Writing. Writing in Foreign Language Contexts: Learning, Teaching, and Research, 77–101. [Google Scholar]
- Singleton, Jenny L., and Matthew D. Tittle. 2000. Deaf Parents and Their Hearing Children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5: 221–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Singleton, Jenny L. 2004. Vocabulary Use by Low, Moderate, and High ASL-Proficient Writers Compared to Hearing ESL and Monolingual Speakers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 9: 86–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sutcliffe, Andrew, Ann Dowker, and Ruth Campbell. 1999. Deaf Children’s Spelling: Does It Show Sensitivity to Phonology? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 4: 111–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- SOU. 2016. Utredningen om kvalitet i utbildningen för elever med vissa funktionsnedsättningar. Samordning, ansvar och kommunikation–vägen till ökad kvalitet i utbildningen för elever med vissa funktionsnedsättningar. Available online: https://data.riksdagen.se/fil/F69F0228-52D3-488F-ADCF-72C427E62D10 (accessed on 10 April 2021).
- Strong, Michael, and Philip M Prinz. 1997. A Study of the Relationship Between American Sign Language and English Literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2: 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Svartholm, Kristina. 2006. Svenska Som Andraspråk För Döva–En Ämnesöversikt. In Teckenspråk: Sociala och historiska perspektiv. Edited by Karin Hoyer, Monica Londen and Jan-Ola Östman. Helsinki: Helsinki University, Institutionen för Nordiska Språk och Nordisk Litteratur, pp. 23–52. [Google Scholar]
- von Koss, Torkildsen, Janne Frøydis Morken, Wenche A. Helland, and Turid Helland. 2016. The dynamics of narrative writing in primary grade children: Writing process factors predict story quality. Reading and Writing 29: 529–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wengelin, Åsa. 2002. Examining Pauses in Writing: Theory, Methods and Empirical Data. Computer Key-Stroke Logging and Writing: Methods and Applications. Ph.D. dissertation, Gothenburg University, Gothenburg, Sweden. [Google Scholar]
- Wengelin, Åsa. 2006. Examining Pauses in Writing: Theory, Methods and Empirical Data. In Computer Key-Stroke Logging and Writing: Methods and Applications. Studies in Writing. Edited by Kirk P. H. Sullivan and Eva Lindgren. Sweden: Elsevier, pp. 107–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Williams, Cheri, and Elizabeth Lowrance-Faulhaber. 2018. Writing in Young Bilingual Children: Review of Research. Journal of Second Language Writing 42: 58–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, Kimberly A., Lisa M. Bowers, Hannah M. Dostal, and Shannon C. Graham. 2014a. Deaf writers’ application of American Sign Language knowledge to English. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 17: 410–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, Kimberly A., Shannon C. Graham, Hannah M. Dostal, and Lisa M. Bowers. 2014b. A description of ASL features in writing. Ampersand 1: 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Gender | Age | Sign- RepL2 | DHH Parent(s)? | Type of School | Hearing Technology | Implant Age | Hearing in dB with CI/HA | Hearing in dB without CI/HA | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boy | 10.7 | 3.88 | Yes | HoH-class | HA | 40–54 | 55–69 | ||
DHH | Girl | 11.0 | 3.54 | No | Hearing class | CI | 1.2 years | 25–39 | >90 |
Girl | 11.1 | 3.86 | Yes | Hearing class | CI | 1.6 years | 25–39 | >90 | |
Girl | 11.3 | 3.78 | No | HoH-class | CI | 0.9 years | 25–39 | >90 | |
Girl | 11.4 | 3.80 | Yes | Hearing class | CI | 2.2 years | 25–39 | >90 | |
Boy | 11.6 | 3.98 | Yes | HoH-class | HA | n/a | 55–69 | ||
Girl | 12.0 | 3.84 | Yes | HoH-class | HA | n/a | 55–69 | ||
Boy | 12.7 | 3.68 | No | HoH-class | HA | 40–54 | 55–69 | ||
Girl | 12.8 | 3.98 | Yes | HoH-class | HA | 25–39 | 40–54 | ||
Girl | 12.8 | 3.92 | Yes | Hearing class | HA | 55–69 | 70–89 | ||
Girl | 10.9 | 3.92 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
CODA | Boy | 11.0 | 3.78 | Yes | Hearing class | ||||
Boy | 11.0 | 3.70 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
Boy | 11.0 | 3.92 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
Girl | 11.2 | 3.84 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
Girl | 11.3 | 3.62 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
Girl | 11.4 | 3.44 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
Girl | 11.6 | 3.76 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
Girl | 11.7 | 3.46 | Yes | Hearing class | |||||
Girl | 12.5 | 3.52 | Yes | Hearing class |
Measures | Definitions and Ways of Approach |
---|---|
Number of Words | The number of words based on the final products with help of Microsoft Word’s word count. Words with incorrect spaces, for example ytter dörren instead of the correct ytterdörren, ‘front door’, were counted as one word. Names with spaces such as Rosa pantern, ‘Pink Panther’, were also counted as one word, so that they could be equated with personal names consisting of one word, such as Rufus or Panter. |
Spelling Errors | Each misspelled word was counted as an error. In the next step the number of spelling errors was divided by the total of number of words. The measure is thus a proportion of spelling errors for all words. |
Lexical Diversity | Lexical diversity reveals how varied a text is and works as an indicator of a writer’s vocabulary: the more unique words a text has, the higher the lexical diversity. To count lexical diversity, the analysis program CLAN was used (MacWhinney 2000). Through this program, information about the lexical diversity through the ratio of unique words to the total number of words counted by means of VocD was generated (Malvern et al. 2004). |
Lexical Density | Lexical density reveals how dense a text, if the text contains many lexical or grammatical words. The more lexical words, the higher the density, resulting in a more information-packed text (Halliday 1985; Johansson 2009). The content and function words were manually identified, and then extracted by means of CLAN’s word count. The proportion of content words to the total number of words were then calculated in Excel (MacWhinney 2000). |
Measures | Definitions and Ways of Approach |
---|---|
Writing Time | Information about the writing time between the pressing of the first and the last key. |
Characters in Total | The total number of characters (including space, punctuation, comma, etc. that have been written during the whole writing session, including those removed). |
Deleted Characters | A kind of revision. The number of deleted characters was divided by the number of the total number of characters in the text. |
Offline Text Flow | Characters in final product divided by writing time in seconds. |
Online Text Flow | Characters in total divided by writing time in seconds. |
Transition Time | The average pause time between letters within a word—the time it takes for a writer to find a key. |
Pauses before Words | This measure generates information about the time it on average takes to pause before writing a new word. The criterion was set to 2 s. |
Pauses within Words | This measure generates information about the time it on average takes to pause within a word. The criterion was set to 2 s. |
Number of Pauses within Words | This measure frames the number of pauses placed within a word that were longer than 2 s, without taking their length into consideration. |
Pause Percentage | The amount of the writing session that consisted of pauses by dividing the total pause time with the total writing time. |
Writing Process | Written Product | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group | Characters in Total | Deleted Characters | Writing Time | Percentage Pausing | Transition Time | Offline Text Flow | Online Text Flow | Pauses before Words | Pauses within Words | Number of Pauses within Words | Number of Words | Spelling Errors | Lexical Diversity | Lexical Density |
DHH | 2211 | 14.6% | 20.90 | 23.6% | 0.26 | 1.5 | 1.8 | 3.7 | 3.2 | 4 | 353 | 2.5% | 38.9 | 0.37 |
DHH | 2500 | 28.1% | 33.01 | 41.3% | 0.25 | 0.9 | 1.3 | 2.9 | 2.3 | 5 | 318 | 4.4% | 54.1 | 0.43 |
DHH | 2203 | 9.0% | 58.25 | 51.5% | 0.54 | 0.6 | 0.6 | 3.5 | 2.9 | 36 | 337 | 3.6% | 75.1 | 0.55 |
DHH | 1413 | 14.3% | 31.70 | 48.2% | 0.58 | 0.6 | 0.7 | 4.1 | 2.4 | 19 | 243 | 2.5% | 39.2 | 0.46 |
DHH | 1625 | 6.9% | 20.63 | 62.4% | 0.31 | 1.2 | 1.3 | 4.4 | 2.1 | 6 | 269 | 0.0% | 45.0 | 0.41 |
DHH | 1803 | 14.0% | 31.23 | 43.9% | 0.35 | 0.8 | 1.0 | 3.4 | 3.6 | 4 | 297 | 3.0% | 59.3 | 0.44 |
DHH | 2370 | 17.5% | 29.18 | 34.7% | 0.27 | 1.1 | 1.4 | 3.6 | 2.2 | 7 | 360 | 0.3% | 58.1 | 0.40 |
DHH | 2197 | 23.7% | 39.14 | 46.3% | 0.36 | 0.7 | 0.9 | 3.9 | 2.9 | 15 | 322 | 0.9% | 52.5 | 0.42 |
DHH | 1858 | 17.1% | 20.49 | 37.6% | 0.26 | 1.3 | 1.5 | 3.4 | 3.1 | 3 | 300 | 0.3% | 56.0 | 0.47 |
DHH | 3718 | 41.1% | 49.66 | 48.1% | 0.17 | 0.7 | 1.2 | 3.1 | 4.5 | 8 | 394 | 1.0% | 71.0 | 0.48 |
Average | 2189.8 | 18.6% | 33.42 | 43.7% | 0.33 | 0.9 | 1.2 | 3.6 | 2.9 | 10.7 | 319.3 | 1.9% | 54.9 | 0.44 |
CODA | 3067 | 9.0% | 44.13 | 54.7% | 0.25 | 1.1 | 1.2 | 5.1 | 2.4 | 10 | 522 | 0.6% | 56.5 | 0.37 |
CODA | 1271 | 8.3% | 22.08 | 46.9% | 0.46 | 0.9 | 1.0 | 4.0 | 3.1 | 14 | 227 | 11.5% | 37.1 | 0.41 |
CODA | 1638 | 11.2% | 21.87 | 50.0% | 0.27 | 1.1 | 1.2 | 3.8 | 4.6 | 3 | 290 | 4.1% | 38.3 | 0.39 |
CODA | 2517 | 11.9% | 37.63 | 48.5% | 0.31 | 1.0 | 1.1 | 3.9 | 2.8 | 13 | 432 | 5.1% | 48.2 | 0.38 |
CODA | 2178 | 17.1% | 25.40 | 41.6% | 0.24 | 1.2 | 1.4 | 3.6 | 2.2 | 3 | 345 | 0.9% | 54.3 | 0.41 |
CODA | 2718 | 17.0% | 34.19 | 45.9% | 0.25 | 1.1 | 1.3 | 3.1 | 2.8 | 6 | 402 | 4.7% | 67.5 | 0.45 |
CODA | 1284 | 6.8% | 16.92 | 39.5% | 0.31 | 1.2 | 1.3 | 3.1 | 2.7 | 10 | 225 | 1.8% | 32.0 | 0.37 |
CODA | 2261 | 10.7% | 41.47 | 51.9% | 0.38 | 0.8 | 0.9 | 3.7 | 3.1 | 16 | 408 | 7.8% | 47.5 | 0.38 |
CODA | 2083 | 9.5% | 19.64 | 45.2% | 0.19 | 1.6 | 1.8 | 4.1 | 2.6 | 4 | 335 | 0.3% | 51.0 | 0.43 |
CODA | 6746 | 11.7% | 57.59 | 33.4% | 0.19 | 1.7 | 2.0 | 3.4 | 3.6 | 12 | 1155 | 3.5% | 74.0 | 0.34 |
Average | 2576.3 | 11.3% | 32.09 | 45.7% | 0.28 | 1.2 | 1.3 | 3.8 | 3.0 | 9.1 | 434.1 | 4.0% | 50.6 | 0.39 |
Significance | 0.05 * | 0.09 | 0.02 * |
Signing Knowledge | Hearing | Characters in Total | Deleted Characters | Writing Time | Pause Percentage | Transition Time | Offline Text Flow | Online Text Flow | Pauses before Words | Number of Pauses within Words | Number of Words | Lexical Diversity | Lexical Density | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lexical Density | −0.51 * | −0.49 * | ||||||||||||
Lexical Diversity | 0.67 ** | 0.76 *** | 0.54 * | |||||||||||
Number of Words | 0.96 *** | 0.62 ** | 0.46 * | 0.49 * | ||||||||||
Number of Pauses within Words | 0.61 ** | 0.75 *** | −0.54 * | −0.64 ** | ||||||||||
Pauses before Words | −0.48 * | −0.48 * | 0.47 * | |||||||||||
Online Text Flow | 0.48 * | −0.60 ** | −0.80 *** | 0.94 *** | ||||||||||
Offline Text Flow | −0.55 * | −0.49 * | −0.66 ** | |||||||||||
Transition Time | −0.48 * | |||||||||||||
Pause Percentage | ||||||||||||||
Writing Time | 0.69 *** | |||||||||||||
Deleted Characters | 0.49 * | −0.46 * | ||||||||||||
Characters in Total | ||||||||||||||
Hearing | ||||||||||||||
Signing Knowledge |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Gärdenfors, M. The Writing Process and the Written Product in Bimodal Bilingual Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children. Languages 2021, 6, 85. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6020085
Gärdenfors M. The Writing Process and the Written Product in Bimodal Bilingual Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children. Languages. 2021; 6(2):85. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6020085
Chicago/Turabian StyleGärdenfors, Moa. 2021. "The Writing Process and the Written Product in Bimodal Bilingual Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children" Languages 6, no. 2: 85. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6020085
APA StyleGärdenfors, M. (2021). The Writing Process and the Written Product in Bimodal Bilingual Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children. Languages, 6(2), 85. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6020085