Structural Priming, Levels of Awareness, and Agency in Contact-Induced Language Change
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Linguistic Models on Levels of Awareness
2.1. Humboldt’s Inner Form and Outer Form
2.2. Silverstein’s Limits of Awareness
- Unavoidable referentiality (1981: 5): the fact that some linguistic elements necessarily refer to something. This is a very broad criterion, which holds for many grammatical elements. Silverstein illustrates it with pronouns such as the tu/vous opposition in French. Both tu and vous clearly refer to the hearer.
- Continuous segmentability (1981: 6): the fact that many linguistic expressions like ‘the woman’, ‘walks’ form a single segmentable unit, while others such as ‘has … -ed’ do not. Silverstein assumes that speakers are potentially much more aware of single units in language than of discontinuous expressions. Silverstein does not present any psycholinguistic evidence for this, but this evidence may be found in studies on ‘unit-like’ processing in human memory. The notion of ‘locality’ in the generative tradition (e.g., Sportiche et al. 2014) may also be linked to this.
- Relative presupposition (1981: 7): the fact that the use of a linguistic expression may require some specific set of pragmatic conditions. When we use ‘this’, we are either referring to something visually present in the domain of discourse, or to something mentioned before.
- Decontextualized deducibility (1981: 10): the fact that the use of a particular form entails a particular presupposition, regardless of context. Thus, the use of ‘my brother’ entails that I have a brother.
- Metapragmatic transparency (1981: 14): the fact that for many utterances the form of the utterance directly reveals the intention of the speaker. Thus ‘can you open the window?’ is more transparent in this sense than ‘it is hot in here’, even though both utterances may have been uttered with the same intention, namely conveying the need to open a window to the hearer. Speakers may be much more aware of dedicated grammatical patterns than of broad implicatures.
2.3. Van Coetsem: Agentivity and the Stability Gradient
- Continuous segmentability–Fewer constituents
- Unavoidable referentiality–More structured
- Relative presupposition–Closed list
2.4. Sociolinguistic Work
3. Cross-Linguistic Priming and Language Change
3.1. Structural Priming: Some Background
3.2. Priming and Language Change
4. Priming in Relation to Awareness and Agency
4.1. Priming as an Automatic Process
4.2. Priming and Linguistic Levels of Awareness
4.3. Priming and Socio-Pragmatic Dimensions of Awareness and Agency
4.4. Some Suggestions for Future Research on Priming and Awareness in Relation to Language Change
- A grammatical analysis leading to more specific hypotheses about possible awareness of linguistic items and structures, building on the work of Van Humboldt and then Silverstein and Van Coetsem, but more systematically exploring different types of items one by one. As already alluded to in Section 4.2, such a linguistic analysis should not only incorporate discrete aspects of language, but also distributional aspects based on frequency of occurrence, such as surprisal.
- Experimental work testing the awareness of speakers of these items and structures and linking it to psycholinguistic theories on the cognitive mechanisms of language production and comprehension. In the current paper, we explored the potential links between research on structural priming, language change, and awareness of linguistic structures. Naturally, the next step is the further test these potential links, using fine-grained measurements that allow for a sound assessment of conscious awareness in linguistic processing, and how this relates to priming. Of course, this experimental work can also inform the linguistic analyses we suggest in point 1 above. In fact, Branigan and Pickering (2017) have recently proposed that priming research could indeed serve as a key experimental approach to inform theories of linguistic structure.
- Parallel to this experimental work, a detailed meta-analysis of the priming literature about the linguistic items and patterns involved and how it relates to strategic and automatic aspects of priming. Such a meta-analysis could not only inform the experimental work suggested in point 2 above, but also the linguistic analysis suggested in point 1.
- Further development and testing of how cross-linguistic priming and awareness of linguistic structures are related to contact-induced language change. After all, the empirical basis of the link between structural priming and contact-induced change is still rather limited, especially in relation to awareness of linguistic structures—the only evidence pointing in this direction are surprisal effects found by Kootstra and Şahin (2018). Therefore, more research on cross-linguistic priming and contact-induced change should be done, exploring multiple language pairs and multiple linguistic items and structures, preferably varying in terms of their predicted level of conscious processing (based on research suggested in points 1 and 2 above).
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Arai, Manabu, Roger P. G. van Gompel, and Christoph Scheepers. 2007. Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension. Cognitive Psychology 54: 218–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Babel, Anna M., ed. 2016a. Awareness and Control in Sociolinguistic Research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Babel, Anna M. 2016b. Affective motivations for borrowing: Performing local identity through loan phonology. Language and Communication 49: 70–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckner, Clay, Richard Blythe, Joan Bybee, Morten H. Christiansen, William Croft, Nick C. Ellis, John Holland, Jinyun Ke, Diane Larsen-Freeman, and Tom Schoenemann. 2009. Language is a complex adaptive system: Position paper. Language Learning 59: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernolet, Sarah, and Robert J. Hartsuiker. 2010. Does verb bias modulate syntactic priming? Cognition 114: 455–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bernolet, Sarah, Robert J. Hartsuiker, and Martin Pickering. 2007. Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33: 931–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bernolet, Sarah, Robert J. Hartsuiker, and Martin J. Pickering. 2012. Effects of phonological feedback on the selection of syntax: Evidence from between-language syntactic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15: 503–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Birdsong, David. 1989. Metalinguistic Performance and Interlinguistic Competence. Berlin and New York: Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bock, J. Kathryn. 1986. Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology 18: 355–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bock, J. Kathryn. 1989. Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cognition 31: 163–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bock, J. Kathryn, and Zenzi M. Griffin. 2000. The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129: 177–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bock, J. Kathryn, and Helga Loebell. 1990. Framing sentences. Cognition 35: 1–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Branigan, Holly P., and Martin J. Pickering. 2017. An experimental approach to linguistic representation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40: e282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering, and Alexandra A. Cleland. 2000. Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition 75: B13–B25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering, Jamie Pearson, and Janet F. McLean. 2010. Linguistic alignment between people and computers. Journal of Pragmatics 42: 2355–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brennan, Susan E., and Herbert H. Clark. 1996. Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22: 1482–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brennan, Susan E., and Joy E. Hanna. 2009. Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science 1: 274–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bresnan, Joan, Anna Cueni, Tatiana Nikitina, and R. Harald Baayen. 2007. Predicting the dative alternation. In Cognitive Foundations of Interpretation. Amsterdam: KNAW, pp. 69–94. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, Esther L. 2015. The role of discourse context frequency in phonological variation: A usage-based approach to bilingual speech production. International Journal of Bilingualism 19: 387–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruyn, Adrienne, Pieter Muysken, and Maaike Verrips. 1999. Double object constructions in Creole languages. In Language Creation and Language Change. Creolization, Diachrony, and Development. Edited by Michel DeGraff. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 329–74. [Google Scholar]
- Brysbaert, Marc, Goedele Van Dyck, and Marijke Van de Poel. 1999. Visual word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence from masked phonological priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25: 137–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bybee, Joan. 2010. Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cai, Zhenguang G., Martin J. Pickering, Hao Yan, and Holly P. Branigan. 2011. Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language 65: 431–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. 2010. New Directions in Sociolinguistic Cognition. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 15: 30–39. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. 2016. Toward a cognitively realistic model of meaningful sociolinguistic variation. In Awareness and Control in Sociolinguistic Research. Edited by Anna M. Babel. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 123–51. [Google Scholar]
- Chang, Franklin, Gary S. Dell, and J. Kathryn Bock. 2006. Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review 113: 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, Eve Vivienne. 1978. Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do. In The Child’s Conception of Language. Edited by A. Sinclair, R. Jarvella and W. Levelt. New York: Springer Verlag, pp. 17–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cleland, Alexandra A., and Martin J. Pickering. 2003. The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language 49: 214–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corley, Martin, and Christoph Scheepers. 2002. Syntactic priming in English sentence production: Categorical and latency evidence from an Internet-based study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9: 126–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, Jacqueline M., and Michael P. Kaschak. 2008. Patterns of experience with verbs affect long-term cumulative structural priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 155: 967–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Croft, William. 2000. Explaining Language Change: An Evolutionary Approach. London: Pearson Education. [Google Scholar]
- de Bot, Kees, Wander Lowie, and Marjolijn Verspoor. 2007. A Dynamic Systems Theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10: 7–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dell, Gary S., and Franklin Chang. 2014. The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369: 20120394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dell, Gary S., and Victor S. Ferreira. 2016. Thirty years of structural priming: An introduction to the special issue. Journal of Memory and Language 91: 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Desmet, Timothy, and Mieke Declercq. 2006. Cross-linguistic priming of syntactic hierarchical configuration information. Journal of Memory and Language 54: 610–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dijksterhuis, Ap, and John A. Bargh. 2001. The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Edited by M. P. Zanna. New York: Academic Press, vol. 33, pp. 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doğruöz, A. Seza, and Ad Backus. 2009. Innovative constructions in Dutch Turkish: An assessment of ongoing contact-induced change. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12: 41–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Düzel, Emrah, Andrew P. Yonelinas, George R. Mangun, Hans-Jochen Heinze, and Endel Tulving. 1997. Event-related brain potential correlates of two states of conscious awareness in memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94: 5973–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eckert, Penelope. 2012. Three waves of variation study: The emergence of meaning in the study of sociolinguistic variation. Annual Review of Anthropology 41: 87–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, N. C. 2015. Implicit AND Explicit Language Learning: Their dynamic interface and complexity. In Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages. Edited by Patrick Rebuschat. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 3–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferguson, Charles A. 1975. Toward a characterization of English foreigner talk. Anthropological Linguistics 17: 1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Fernández, Eva M., Ricardo Augusto De Souza, and Agustina Carando. 2017. Bilingual innovations: Experimental evidence offers clues regarding the psycholinguistics of language change. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 251–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, Victor S. 1996. Is it better to give than to donate? Syntactic flexibility in language production. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 724–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, Victor S., and J. Kathryn Bock. 2006. The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes 21: 1011–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, Victor S., J. Kathryn Bock, Michael P. Wilson, and Neal J. Cohen. 2008. Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science 19: 940–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forster, Kenneth I. 1998. The pros and cons of masked priming. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27: 203–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, Elaine. 1996. Cross-language priming from ignored words: Evidence for a common representational system in bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 353–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fricke, Melinda, and Gerrit Jan Kootstra. 2016. Primed codeswitching in spontaneous bilingual dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language 91: 181–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frost, Ram, Avital Deutsch, Orna Gilboa, Michal Tannenbaum, and William Marslen-Wilson. 2000. Morphological priming: Dissociation of phonological, semantic, and morphological factors. Memory & Cognition 28: 1277–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garrod, Simon, and Anthony Anderson. 1987. Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition 27: 181–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Garrod, Simon, and Martin J. Pickering. 2004. Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 8–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garrod, Simon, and Martin J. Pickering. 2007. Alignment in dialogue. In The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Edited by Gareth Gaskell. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 443–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garrod, Simon, and Martin J. Pickering. 2013. Dialogue: Interactive alignment and its implications for language learning and language change. In The Language Phenomenon. Edited by P. -M. Binder and K. Smith. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 47–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giles, Howard, Justine Coupland, and Nicolas Coupland. 1991. Accommodation Theory: Communication, Context, and Consequence. In Contexts of Accommodation. Edited by Howard Giles, Justine Coupland and Nicolas Coupland. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grice, Paul. 1975. Logic and conversation. In Syntax and Semantics. 3: Speech Acts. Edited by Peter Cole and Jerry Morgan. New York: Academic Press, pp. 41–58. [Google Scholar]
- Gries, Stefan Th., and Gerrit Jan Kootstra. 2017. Structural priming within and across languages: A corpus-based perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 235–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gries, Stefan Th., and Anatol Stefanowitsch. 2004. Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on ‘‘alternations”. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 9: 97–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Sarah Bernolet. 2017. The development of shared syntax in second language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 219–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Martin J. Pickering. 2008. Language integration in bilingual sentence production. Acta Psychologica 128: 479–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hartsuiker, Robert J., Martin J. Pickering, and Eline Veltkamp. 2004. Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish/English bilinguals. Psychological Science 15: 409–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hartsuiker, Robert J., Sarah Bernolet, Sofie Schoonbaert, Sarah Speybroeck, and Dieter Vanderelst. 2008. Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language 58: 214–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartsuiker, Robert J., Saskia Beerts, Maaike Loncke, Timothy Desmet, and Sarah Bernolet. 2016. Cross-linguistic structural priming in multilinguals: Further evidence for shared syntax. Journal of Memory and Language 90: 14–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Indefrey, Peter, Hülya Şahin, and Marianne Gullberg. 2017. The expression of spatial relationships in Turkish–Dutch bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 473–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacob, Gunnar. 2018. Morphological priming in bilingualism research. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21: 443–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacob, Gunnar, Kalliopi Katsika, Neiloufar Family, and Shanley E. M. Allen. 2017. The role of constituent order and level of embedding in cross-linguistic structural priming. Bilingualism 20: 269–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, Larry L. 1991. A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language 305: 513–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaeger, T. Florian, and Neal Snider. 2007. Implicit learning and syntactic persistence: Surprisal and cumulativity. University of Rochester Working Papers in the Language Sciences 3: 26–44. [Google Scholar]
- Jaeger, T. Florian, and Neal Snider. 2013. Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition 127: 57–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jaeger, T. Florian, and Kodi Weatherholtz. 2016. What the heck is salience? How predictive language processing contributes to sociolinguistic perception. Frontiers in Psychology 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jäger, Gerhard, and Anette Rosenbach. 2008. Priming and unidirectional language change. Theoretical linguistics 34: 85–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, Olivia, and Thalia Wheatley. 2015. Pupil dilation patterns reflect the contents of consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 35: 128–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kantola, Leila, and Roger P. G. van Gompel. 2011. Between- and within-language priming is the same: Evidence for shared bilingual syntactic representations. Memory & Cognition 39: 276–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaschak, Michael P. 2007. Long-term structural priming affects subsequent patterns of language production. Memory & Cognition 35: 925–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaschak, Michael P., Timothy J. Kutta, and Christopher Schatschneider. 2011. Long-term cumulative structural priming persists for (at least) one week. Memory & Cognition 39: 381–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaschak, Michael P., Timothy J. Kutta, and Jacqueline M. Coyle. 2012. Long and short term cumulative structural priming effects. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29: 728–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kidd, Evan, Emilie Tennant, and Sanjo Nitschke. 2015. Shared abstract representation of linguistic structure in bilingual sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22: 1062–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kootstra, Gerrit Jan, and Willemijn J. Doedens. 2016. How multiple sources of experience influence bilingual syntactic choice: Immediate and cumulative cross-language effects of structural priming, verb bias, and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19: 710–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kootstra, Gerrit Jan, and Pieter C. Muysken. 2017. Cross-linguistic priming in bilinguals: Multidisciplinary perspectives on language processing, acquisition, and change. (Introduction to special issue). Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 215–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kootstra, Gerrit Jan, and Hülya Şahin. 2018. Cross-linguistic structural priming as a mechanism of contact-induced language change: Evidence from Papiamento-Dutch bilinguals in Aruba and the Netherlands. Language 94: 902–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroll, Judith F., Susan C. Bobb, and Zofia Wodniecka. 2006. Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9: 119–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
- Labov, William. 2007. Transmission and Diffusion. Language 83: 344–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levelt, Willem J. M. 1989. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Loebell, Helga, and J. Kathryn Bock. 2003. Structural priming across languages. Linguistics 41: 791–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lucas, Margery. 2000. Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 7: 618–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luk, J. 2013. Bilingual language play and local creativity in Hong Kong. International Journal of Multilingualism 10: 236–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacDonald, Maryellen C. 2013. How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- MacLeod, Bethany. 2015. A critical evaluation of two approaches to defining perceptual salience. Ampersand 2: 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mahowald, Kyle, Ariel James, Richard Futrell, and Edward Gibson. 2016. A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language 91: 5–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Melloni, Lucia, Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Notger Muller, Eugenio Rodriguez, and Wolf Singer. 2011. Expectations Change the Signatures and Timing of Electrophysiological Correlates of Perceptual Awareness. Journal of Neuroscience 31: 1386–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mendoza-Denton, Norma. 2002. Language and Identity. In The Handbook of Language Variation and Change. Edited by J. K. Chambers, Peter Trudgill and Natalie Schilling-Estes. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 475–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moro, Francesca, and Marian Klamer. 2015. Give-constructions in heritage Ambon Malay in the Netherlands. Journal of Language Contact 8: 263–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Otheguy, Ricardo, Ana Celia Zentella, and David Livert. 2007. Language and dialect contact in Spanish in New York: Toward the formation of a speech community. Language 83: 770–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pardo, Jennifer S. 2006. On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119: 2382–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, Martin J., and Holly P. Branigan. 1999. Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 136–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, Martin J., and Victor S. Ferreira. 2008. Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin 134: 427–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, Martin J., and Simon Garrod. 2004. Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 169–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rivlina, Alexandra. 2015. Bilingual creativity in Russia: English-Russian language play. World Englishes 34: 436–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salamoura, Angeliki, and John N. Williams. 2006. Lexical activation of cross-language syntactic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9: 299–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scheepers, Christoph, Claudine N. Raffray, and Andriy Myachykov. 2017. The lexical boost effect is not diagnostic of lexically-specific syntactic representations. Journal of Memory and Language 95: 102–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scherre, Maria M. P., and Anthony J. Naro. 1991. Marking in discourse: “Birds of a feather”. Language Variation and Change 3: 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, Richard W. 1990. The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics 11: 129–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schoonbaert, Sofie, Robert J. Hartsuiker, and Martin J. Pickering. 2007. The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language 56: 153–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schütz-Bosbach, Simone, and Wolfgang Prinz. 2007. Perceptual resonance: Action-induced modulation of perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 349–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Searle, John R. 1969. Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, vol. 626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shin, Jeong-Ah, and Kiel Christianson. 2009. Syntactic processing in Korean-English bilingual production: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Cognition 112: 175–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shin, Jeong-Ah, and Kiel Christianson. 2012. Structural Priming and Second Language Learning. Language Learning 62: 931–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silverstein, Michael. 1981. The limits of awareness. In Working Papers in Sociolinguistics. Austin: Southwest Educational Development Laboratory. [Google Scholar]
- Sodacı, Hande, Ad Backus, and Gerrit Jan Kootstra. 2019. Role of structural priming in contact-induced change: Subject pronoun expression in L1 Turkish by Turkish-Dutch bilinguals. PsyArchiv. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sportiche, Dominique, Hilda Koopman, and Edward Stabler. 2014. An Introduction to Syntactic Analysis. West Sussex: Wiley Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Tarone, Elaine. 1980. Communication strategies, foreigner talk, and repair in interlanguage. Language Learning 30: 417–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomason, Sarah Grey. 2006. Language Change, intentional. In Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd ed. Edited by Keith Brown. Abingdon-on-Thames: Routledge, vol. 6, pp. 346–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomason, Sarah, and Terrence Kaufman. 1988. Language Contact, Creolization, and Genetic Linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Approach to Child Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tooley, Kristen M., Matthew Traxler, and Tamara Y. Swaab. 2009. Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence of syntactic priming in sentence comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35: 19–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Torres Cacoullos, Rena, and Catherine E. Travis. 2010. Testing convergence via code-switching: Priming and the structure of variable subject expression. International Journal of Bilingualism 15: 241–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Travis, Catherine E., Rena Torres Cacoullos, and Evan Kidd. 2017. Cross-language priming: A view from bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 283–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trofimovich, Pavel, and Sara Kennedy. 2014. Interactive alignment between bilingual interlocutors: Evidence from two information-exchange tasks. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17: 822–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trudgill, Peter. 1981. Linguistic accommodation: Sociolinguistic observations on a sociopsychological theory. In Papers from the Parasession on Language and Behavior. Edited by Carrie S. Masek, Roberta A. Hendrick and Mary Frances Miller. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 218–37. [Google Scholar]
- Trudgill, Peter. 1986. Dialects in Contact. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Tsiplakou, Stavroula. 2009. Doing (bi)lingualism: Language alternation as performative construction of online identities. Pragmatics 19: 361–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Baaren, Rick B., R. W. Holland, B. Steenaert, and A. van Knippenberg. 2003. Mimicry for money: Behavioral consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39: 393–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Coetsem, Frans. 1988. Loan Phonology and the Two Transfer Types in Language Contact. Publications in language sciences 27. Dordrecht: Foris Publications. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Coetsem, Frans. 1995. Outlining a model of the transmission phenomenon in language contact. Leuvense Bijdragen 84: 63–85. [Google Scholar]
- van de Meerendonk, Nan, Herman H. J. Kolk, Dorothee J. Chwilla, and Constance Th. W. M. Vissers. 2009. Monitoring in Language Perception. Language and Linguistics Compass 3: 1211–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Gompel, Roger P. G., and Manabu Arai. 2018. Structural priming in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21: 448–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Hell, Janet G., and Ton Dijkstra. 2002. Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9: 780–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Hell, Janet G., and Darren Tanner. 2012. Second language proficiency and cross-language lexical activation. Language Learning 62: 148–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Heuven, Walter J. B., Ton Dijkstra, Jonathan Grainger, and Herbert Schriefers. 2001. Shared neighborhood effects in masked orthographic priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 8: 96–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Verschik, A. 2017. Language Contact, Language Awareness, and Multilingualism. In Language Awareness and Multilingualism, Encyclopedia of Language and Education, 3rd ed. Edited by Jasone Cenoz, Durk Gorter and Stephen May. Cham: Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Villerius, Sofie. 2019. Development of Surinamese Javanese: Language Contact and Change in a Multilingual Context. Doctoral dissertation, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands. [Google Scholar]
- von Humboldt, Wilhelm. 1836. The Heterogeneity of Language and Its Influence on the Intellectual Development of Mankind (orig. Ueber die Verschiedenheit des menschlichen Sprachbaus und ihren Einfluss auf die geistige Entwicklung des Menschengeschlechts), 2nd ed. New edition: On Language. On the Diversity of Human Language Construction and Its Influence on the Mental Development of the Human Species. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Weber, Kirsten, and Peter Indefrey. 2009. Syntactic priming in German-English bilinguals during sentence comprehension. NeuroImage 46: 1164–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weiner, E. Judith, and William Labov. 1983. Constraints on the agentless passive. Journal of Linguistics 19: 29–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weinreich, Uriel. 1953. Languages in Contact: Findings and Problems. New York: Linguistic Circle of New York. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weinreich, Uriel, William Labov, and Marvin I. Herzog. 1968. Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. In Directions for Historical Linguistics. Edited by W. P. Lehmann and Y. Malkiel. Austin: University of Texas Press, pp. 95–195. [Google Scholar]
- Whorf, Benjamin. 1956. Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Edited by John B. Carroll. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
1 | Note that this routinization as a result of priming can just as well be seen as the opposite of change: speakers copy each other, leading to entrenchment of a specific linguistic preference (we thank a reviewer for pointing us to this). But when this routinization within a speech community leads to differences in linguistic preferences compared to another or the ‘standard’ speech community, then it is plausible to maintain that priming of linguistic choices can indeed lead to change. |
2 | From the perspective of a historical linguist, a day after testing may not be seen as ‘long term’. However, what this effect shows is that priming effects do not disappear directly; they linger and can thus influence linguistic choices beyond the next trial, and even beyond testing sessions. Given that priming takes place frequently in real life, the ‘long-term’ potential of priming effects can lead to cumulative priming of linguistic choices. |
3 | This does not mean that lexical priming does not involve automatic processing. Indeed, many studies on lexical priming have found that these forms of priming even occur in the absence of conscious processing of the prime (see e.g., Forster 1998). Importantly, however, in the specific context of structural priming, it is plausible to assume that explicit memory of lexical overlap between prime and target underlies the boosted structural priming effect. |
4 | Note that the notion of salience is, in itself, a complex notion that is defined in multiple ways, based on many different criteria, sometimes with a risk of circularity (Macleod 2015). Future research may lead to more clarity of what salience exactly is, and how it is related to different aspects of linguistic behavior and change. |
5 | This form of accommodation is clearly also related to the notion of foreigner talk (e.g., Ferguson 1975; Tarone 1980). |
6 |
More Stable | Less Stable |
---|---|
Fewer constituents | More constituents |
More structured | Less structured |
Closed list | Open list |
More automatic | Less automatic |
Less conscious | More conscious |
e.g., articulatory habits | e.g., content words |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kootstra, G.J.; Muysken, P. Structural Priming, Levels of Awareness, and Agency in Contact-Induced Language Change. Languages 2019, 4, 65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4030065
Kootstra GJ, Muysken P. Structural Priming, Levels of Awareness, and Agency in Contact-Induced Language Change. Languages. 2019; 4(3):65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4030065
Chicago/Turabian StyleKootstra, Gerrit Jan, and Pieter Muysken. 2019. "Structural Priming, Levels of Awareness, and Agency in Contact-Induced Language Change" Languages 4, no. 3: 65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4030065
APA StyleKootstra, G. J., & Muysken, P. (2019). Structural Priming, Levels of Awareness, and Agency in Contact-Induced Language Change. Languages, 4(3), 65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4030065