Emergence of a Norm from Resistance: Using Simulation to Explore the Macro Implications of Social Identity Theory
Abstract
:1. Introduction: How Committed Social Action Can Drive Large-Scale Changes in Social Norms
2. Materials and Method: Extracting System Structures from Social Identity Theory
2.1. Social Identity Theory Forms the Basis for a Simulation Experiment
2.2. Forty-Two Propositions That Bridge Social Identity Theory and the System Simulation
2.3. Telling the Story of the Emergence of Norm Contestation
2.4. Simulation Model
Using the Literature to Operationalize the Dissimilarity and Internalization Rates in the Simulation Model
3. Exercising the Simulation Model
Base Run Behavior for a Democratic Culture
4. Discussion
4.1. Case Examplar #1: The LGBTQ Rights Movement and Same Sex Marriage
4.2. Case Examplar #2: The Women’s Suffrage Movement
4.3. Effect of Anger on the Emergence of a New Norm
Government punishment might increase anger among those who are oppressed or depressed by the old norm, which, in turn, increases risk taking, rather than decreasing risk taking as intended. This encourages more people to violate the norm. As a consequence, it increases both the number of people whose behaviors are based on the new norm, and those who perceive themselves to be similar to violators.
5. Summary of Dynamics of Norm Change within a Democratic Culture
5.1. Future Work Can Use the Simulator to Explore “What If” Scenarios
- The effects of democratic versus non-democratic cultures. The feedback effects in the basic model of democratic norm change can be parametrically altered to represent alternative non-democratic cultures, setting up a comparative discussion of similarities in outcomes between these two very different cultures.
- The effects of various levels and types of antipreneurial behaviors. While entrepreneurs work to create forces that promote changing norms, antipreneurs are those actors who work to preserve old norms through their systematic activities. Our theory can be used to explore the relative effectiveness of various antipreneurial strategies.
- The effects of various levels of faux activists on New Norm Emergence. While activists work to attract public opinion in favor of the ongoing collective action, faux activists are those who work for governments or other rival groups to turn public opinion against an ongoing collective action by showing extreme violent action. Our theory can be used to explore the relative effectiveness of various faux activists’ strategies.
- The effects of punishment. While punishment is the main mechanism used to enforce a norm, there are times when governments reduce pressure and levels of punishment. Our theory can be used to explore the relative effectiveness of various punishment strategies.
5.2. Limitations of This Study
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Model Variables | Claim/ Assumption | Number of Claims/ Assumptions | Description | Reference |
---|---|---|---|---|
Personal Values | Claim | 1 | Individuals’ values have their roots in the societies’ culture, religion, social media, rules, and normative structure | [74,75,76,77] |
Personal Value | Assumption | 2 | Contesting a norm affects personal values | Logical extension of claim #1 |
Personal Value | Assumption | 3 | The dominant norm affects personal values | Logical extension of claim #1 |
Personal Norm | Claim | 4 | Personal values shape personal norms; a norm is a tool to achieve goals | [78,79] |
Pro-Group Emotion | Claim | 5 | Personal norms shape personal emotions | [80] |
Pro-Group Emotion | Claim | 6 | Violation of a norm causes pro-category emotions among those who have a shared feeling of grievance and, indeed, among those who see violators as brave and risk takers and admire them | [40,41,42] |
Pro-Group Emotion | Claim | 7 | Group violations cause pro-emotion among others | [49] |
Personal Identity | Claim | 8 | Personal norms shape personal identity | [81] |
Violators’ Group Formation | Claim | 9 | Perception of similarity based on shared interest shapes a new group | [35] |
Risk Taking | Claim | 10 | Anger increases risk taking | [47,48] |
Risk Taking | Claim | 11 | Pro-group emotion increases risk-taking | [47,82] |
Risk Taking | Claim | 12 | Fear decreases risk taking | [83] |
Risk Taking | Assumption | 13 | Peer pressure decreases risk taking | Logical extension of claim #12 |
Likelihood of Feeling Angry | Assumption | 14 | Punishment might cause anger among people who perceive the same grievances | Based on historical evidence |
Feeling of Fear | Claim | 15 | Punishment causes fear as it challenges individuals’ interests | [4,35] |
Peer pressure / feelings of disgust | Claim | 16 | Individuals feel in-group peer pressure to behave based on the group norm, otherwise they will be perceived as disgusting and abandoned by their peers | [39,84] |
Behavior | Claim | 17 | Normative context affects personal behavior | [85] |
Peer pressure | Assumption | 18 | The dominant norm affects the perception of peer pressure | Logical extension of claim #17 |
Peer Pressure | Assumption | 19 | The new norm affects perceptions of peer pressure | Logical extension of claim #17 |
Perceiving Similarity Based on Shared Interests | Claim | 20 | Personal identity shapes personal interests and the perception of similarity | [41,42] |
Perceiving Similarity Based on Shared Interests | Claim | 21 | The pro-group emotion makes others perceive their similarity with violators based on their shared interests | [41,42] |
Likelihood of Defining Incompatible Goals | Claim | 22 | There is always a possibility that individuals find their initial goals are not incompatible with a group norm | [86] |
Undergoing Dissimilarity-Focused Compression | Claim | 23 | Distrust awakens the dissimilarity comparison | [86] |
Emergence of Distrust Toward Category Norm | Claim | 24 | Incompatible goals cause distrust | [86] |
Learning and Assigning the Norms of the Group | Claim | 25 | Each group has its own norm and, by joining the distinct category, group members will learn about the norm and start assigning the group norm | [30,33] |
Depersonalization | Claim | 26 | The more members assign the group norm, the more they depersonalize and self-stereotype | [30] |
Internalization of the Norm | Claim | 27 | The more individuals self-stereotype, the more they internalize the norm | [30] |
Group Behavior | Claim | 28 | Group members behave because of norm internalization | [30] |
Emergence of a New Norm | Claim | 29 | Group behavior will become normative after a while | [30] |
Group Behavior | Claim | 30 | Punishment decreases the group violating behavior | [4,5] |
Group Behavior | Claim | 31 | Risk taking increases group members’ riskier behavior or more violating behavior | [40] |
Emergence of a New Norm | Claim | 32 | Legal norms’ strength weakens the contesting norm | [13] |
Dominant Norm/Legal Norm | Assumption | 33 | Contesting a norm weakens the legal norm (the population size of either the contesting or legal norm balance each other) | Logical extension of claim #32 |
Pro-category Emotion | Assumption | 34 | The violation of a dominant norm triggers pro-category emotion | Based on historical evidence like Inqilab Girls |
Extreme Behavior | Claim | 35 | It is always possible that members lose their awareness and show extreme behavior | [40] |
Punishment | Claim | 36 | Extreme behavior increases government punishment | [87] |
Likelihood to Trigger Anti-category Norm Emotion | Claim | 37 | Extreme behavior causes negative emotions among members | [40] |
Group Violating Behavior | Claim | 38 | Anti-category emotion reduces group violating behaviors | [40] |
Pro-category Emotion | Assumption | 39 | The violation of a dominant norm triggers pro-category emotion | Based on historical evidence, such as Inqilab Girls |
Extreme Behavior | Claim | 39 | It is always possible that members lose their awareness and show extreme behavior | [40] |
Punishment | Claim | 40 | Extreme behavior increases government punishment | [87] |
Likelihood to Trigger Anti-category Norm Emotion | Claim | 41 | Extreme behavior causes negative emotions among members | [40] |
Group Violating Behavior | Claim | 42 | Anti-category emotion reduces group violating behavior | [40] |
Appendix B
Variables | Description and Formulation | Type | Unit |
---|---|---|---|
Dominant/Old Norm Population | INTEG (“Stage 3. Dissimilarity Rate”-Transfer Rate, Total Population × P1) People who believe in the old norm and are the population with the potential to learn a new norm | Stock | Person |
Transfer Rate | Transfer Rate: Effect of Perception of Similarity on Transfer Rate × Dominant Norm Population/AD Time to Transfer Change in the number of populations who perceive similarity with and join the violators group | Rate | Person/Year |
First Violation | First Violation = STEP (0.5, 10) This shows the effect of people or groups of people who initially perceive the old norm to be harmful and violate it before other members of society | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
“Pro-Category Emotion” | “Pro-Category Emotion” = SMOOTH ((Personal Norm × “Effect of Group Violating Behaviors on Pro-Category Emotion”) + First Violation, “AT for Pro-Category Emotion”) It shows when population emotion is in favor of the violating behavior | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
Look Up Similarity | S-shaped or logistic growth Relation between the potential population who self-categorize themselves as violators and perception of similarity | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
Lookup Group Violating Behaviors on Pro Emotion | This graphical function shows how an increase in group members’ violating behaviors leads to an increase of emotion in favor of the group | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
Personal Value | Personal Value: SMOOTH (New to Old Population Ratio, AT for Value) This variable shows the change in values among the population | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
Personal Norm | Personal Norm: SMOOTH (Personal Values, AT for Norm) Affected by value, this variable shows the change in norms among the population. | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
Personal Identity | Personal Identity: SMOOTH (Personal Norm, AT for Identity) Affected by norm, this variable shows the change in identity among population | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
Perception of Similarity | This shows the change in perceived similarity based on the shared interest among the population that joins the violators Perception of Similarity: SMOOTH (MAX (“Pro-Category Emotion” + Personal Identity, 0), AT Similarity) | Auxiliary | Dmnl |
Variables | Description | Unit |
---|---|---|
“Depersonalization and Self-Stereotyping” | This stock shows the population who are depersonalized. (“Stage 2. Increase in Collective Belief”, 0) | Person |
Learning and Assigning Norms | This stock shows the population who are learning and assigning a new norm. (“Stage 1. Adapting Rate”, 0) | Person |
Perceived Incompatible Goal | The population who realizes that they have different goals and interests to those of the group norm (“Stage 1. Increase Rate”, 0) | Person |
Emergence of Distrust Toward Category Norms | Having different goal results in the emergence of distrust among group members (“Stage 2. Distrust Increase Rate”, 0) | Person |
Violators Group Population | The population who disobeys a dominant norm (Transfer Rate-“Stage 3. Internalization Rate”-“Stage 3. Dissimilarity Rate”, Total Population × P2) | Person |
Internalized the Contesting Norm Population | The population who internalizes the new norm (“Stage 3. Internalization Rate”-Emergence of Contesting Norm Rate, Total Population × P3) | Person |
“Stage 1. Adapting Rate” | “Stage 1. Adapting Rate” = (Potential Population who learning-Learning and Assigning Norm)/(Adjustment Time to Internalization/3) Change in the number of populations who learn and assign the group norm | Person/Year |
“Stage 1. Increase Rate” | “Stage 1. Increase Rate” = (Potential Population who perceived incompatible goal-Perceived Incompatible Goal)/(Adjustment Time to Dissimilarity/3) Change in the number of populations who perceive their interests to be dissimilar to those of other members | Person/Year |
“Stage 2. Increase in Collective Belief” | “Stage 2. Increase in Collective Belief” = (Learning and Assigning Norm-“Depersonalization and Self-Stereotyping”)/(Adjustment Time to Internalization/3) Change in the number of populations who have a shared believe in the group norm | Person/Year |
“Stage 2. Distrust Increase Rate” | “Stage 2. Distrust Increase Rate” = (Perceived Incompatible Goal-Emergence of Distrust Toward Category Norms)/(Adjustment Time to Dissimilarity/3) Change in the number of populations who lose their trust in the group norm | Person/Year |
“Stage 3. Internalization Rate” | “Stage 3. Internalization Rate” = (“Depersonalization and Self-Stereotyping”-Internalized the Contesting Norm Population)/(Adjustment Time to Internalization/3) Change in the number of populations who internalize the norm through time | Person/Year |
“Stage 3. Dissimilarity Rate” | “Stage 3. Dissimilarity Rate” = Emergence of Distrust Toward Category Norms/(Adjustment Time to Dissimilarity/3) Change in number of populations who perceive dissimilarity | Person/Year |
Potential Population Learning the Norm | Those parts of the violator population who learn the norm Learning Coefficient × Violators Group Population | Person |
Potential Population Who Perceived Incompatible Goal | Potential Population who Perceived Incompatible Goal = Potential Population who learning × Likelihood of defining incompatible goal Those parts of the violator population who realize their goals and interests are different than the group’s | Person |
Percentage of Dissimilarity | Percentage of violators who, after learning a group norm, perceive dissimilarity between their interests and the group’s interests | Person |
Learning Coefficient | Potential Population who Perceived Incompatible Goal = Potential Population who learning × Likelihood of defining incompatible goal An exogenous variable which shows the percentage of violators who will be educated in favor of a new norm | Person |
Appendix B.1. Sub-Model: “Emergence of New Norm Sub-Model”
× Effect of Punishments on Group Behavior)) × Behavior Coefficient
Appendix B.2. Sub Model: “Exit from the New Norm”
Variables | Description | Unit |
---|---|---|
New Norm Population | Population who accepts and behaves based on the new norm New Norm Population = INTEG (Emergence of Contesting Normative Context Rate-Exit Rate, Total Population × P4) | Person |
Emergence of Contesting Normative Context Rate | Change in the number of populations who behave based on the new norm Emergence of Contesting Normative Context Rate = MIN (Internalized the Contesting Norm Population, Internalized the Contesting Norm Population × Effect of Group Violating Behaviors on Emergence of Contesting Normative Context Rate)/AD Time to Emergence of New Norm | Person/Year |
Group Violating Behaviors | This auxiliary shows the increase in group violating behaviors Group Violating Behaviors = (Risk Taking × Effect of Peer Punishment on Group Violating Behaviors)/(Extreme Behavior Punishment × “Anti- category Emotion” × Effect of Punishments on Group Behavior) | Dmnl |
Punishment of Extreme Behavior | Punishment which is executed by governments to suppress extreme behavior during collective action Extreme Behavior Punishment = SMOOTH (Likelihood of Extreme Behaviors × 1.2, AT for Punishment) | Dmnl |
Likelihood of Extreme Behaviors | This is the likelihood of unacceptable behavior, such as breaking public goods | Dmnl |
Risk Taking | It shows a population’s risk taking based on their cost and benefit calculations Risk Taking = MIN (Benefit/Cost, 1) | Dmnl |
Cost | Variable shows the loss and expense of violating behavior. Negative feelings include feelings of disgust and/or shame from peers, and feelings of fear of the government’s punishment, which could be the fear of being arrested, losing one’s job, or economic loss. These are the two main costs associated with reduced risk taking Cost = Negative Feelings | Dmnl |
Benefit | Variable showing the gains and advantages of violating behaviors Benefit = Positive Feeling | Dmnl |
Positive Feeling | Feeling which increase the benefit of risk taking SMOOTH3I(“Pro-Category Emotion” × Feeling of Anger, AT for Feeling, 0.1) | Dmnl |
Negative Feeling | Feeling which increase the cost of risk taking Negative Feelings = SMOOTH (Effect of Fear on Negative Feeling + Effect of Disgust on Negative Feelings, AT for Feeling) | Dmnl |
“Anti-category Emotion” | The emotion which is triggered by extreme behavior and decreases the group’s behavior “Anti- category Emotion” = SMOOTH (Likelihood of Extreme Behaviors due to Deindividuation, AT for Anti Emotion) | Dmnl |
Look Up Punishment | This shows the punishment which is executed by governments to suppress norm violators, which is the ratio of the new-to-old norm populations. We define it this way, and assume that governments gain their power from their supporters’ populations; as the number of their supporter/followers’ decreases compared to the contesters’ population, they have less power to punish norm violators. [(0,0)–(20,1)], (0,0.1), (0.1,0.15), (0.25,0.3), (0.5,0.5), (0.75,0.8), (1,1), (1.3,0.8), (2,0.5), (4,0.3), (10,0.15), (20,0.1) | Dmnl |
Feeling of Anger | The feeling that violators perceive due to governments’ severe and/or unjustified punishments SMOOTH(Effect of punishment on Anger(Contesters to Old Ratio) × Max Effect, AT for Anger) | Dmnl |
Feeling of Fear | The feelings which violators experience due to government punishments, or fears of losing their jobs or being arrested SMOOTH(Effect of Punishment on Fear(Contesters to Old Ratio) × Maxim Eff, AT for Fear) | Dmnl |
Lookup Group Violating Behaviors | This shows the relationship between group violating behaviors on transferring from internalizing to new norm | Dmnl |
Feeling of Disgust | The feeling which violators perceive due to peer pressure SMOOTH (Peer Punishment (New to Old Population Ratio) × Maximum Effect, AT for Disgust) | Dmnl |
Exit Rate | Change in the new norm population due to no longer behaving based on that norm Exit Rate = (New Norm Population × Percentage)/Time Delay | Person/Year |
Appendix C
Parameter | Initial Value |
---|---|
Percentage of Dissimilarity | 5% |
Learning Coefficient | 70% |
Likelihood of Extreme Behaviors | 0.1 |
Effect of Anger on Positive Feeling | 0.7 |
Effect of First Violation on Pro-Category Emotion | 0.5 |
Parameter | Initial Value | High Anger | Low Anger | No Anger |
---|---|---|---|---|
Percentage of Dissimilarity | 5% | 5% | 5% | 5% |
Learning Coefficient | 70% | 70% | 70% | 70% |
Likelihood of Extreme Behaviors | 0.1 | 0.1 | 0.1 | 0.1 |
Effect of Anger on Positive Feeling towards Violators | 0.7 | 1 | 0.4 | 0 |
Effect of First Violation on Pro-Category Emotion | 0.5 | 0.5 | 0.5 | 0.5 |
References
- Finnemore, M. National Interests in International Society; Cornell University Press: Ithaca, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Klotz, A. Norms reconstituting interests: Global racial equality and US sanctions against South Africa. Int. Organ. 1995, 49, 451–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finnemore, M.; Sikkink, K. International norm dynamics and political change. Int. Organ. 1998, 52, 887–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Posner, R.A.; Rasmusen, E.B. Creating and enforcing norms, with special reference to sanctions. Int. Rev. Law Econ. 1999, 19, 369–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Villatoro, D.; Sen, S.; Sabater-Mir, J. Of social norms and sanctioning: A game theoretical overview. Int. J. Agent Technol. Syst. (IJATS) 2010, 2, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barnes, J. The ‘war on terror’ and the battle for the definition of torture. Int. Relat. 2016, 30, 102–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sikkink, K. The United States and torture: Does the spiral model work? Camb. Stud. Int. Relat. 2013, 126, 145–163. [Google Scholar]
- Sandholtz, W. Prohibiting Plunder: How Norms Change; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Heller, R.; Kahl, M.; Pisoiu, D. The ‘dark’side of normative argumentation—The case of counterterrorism policy. Glob. Const. 2012, 1, 278–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Acharya, A. Norm subsidiarity and regional orders: Sovereignty, regionalism, and rule-making in the third world. Int. Stud. Q. 2011, 55, 95–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slaughter, A.-M. A New World Order; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Wunderlich, C. Rogue States as Norm Entrepreneurs; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Deitelhoff, N.; Zimmermann, L. Norms under challenge: Unpacking the dynamics of norm robustness. J. Glob. Secur. Stud. 2019, 4, 2–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forrester, J.W. Industrial dynamics. J. Oper. Res. Soc. 1997, 48, 1037–1041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forrester, J.W. Urban dynamics. IMR Ind. Manag. Rev. (Pre-1986) 1970, 11, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Richardson, G.P.; Pugh, A.L., III. Introduction to system dynamics modeling with DYNAMO. J. Oper. Res. Soc. 1997, 48, 1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brewer, J.; Hunter, A. Multimethod Research: A Synthesis of Styles; Sage Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Babbie, E. The Practice of Social Research, 6th ed.; Woolworth Inc.: Belmont, CA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Rowe, G.; Wright, G. The Delphi technique as a forecasting tool: Issues and analysis. Int. J. Forecast. 1999, 15, 353–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morgan, D.L. Qualitative Research Methods: Focus Groups as Qualitative Research; SAGE Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1997; Volume 10, p. 9781412984287. [Google Scholar]
- McCracken, G. The Long Interview-Qualitative Research Methods; Series 13; Sage Publications, Inc.: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Richardson, G.P.; Andersen, D.F. Teamwork in group model building. Syst. Dyn. Rev. 1995, 11, 113–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strauss, A.; Corbin, J. Basics of Qualitative Research; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Bernard, H.R.; Bernard, H.R. Social Research Methods: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Andersen, D.L.; Luna-Reyes, L.F.; Diker, V.G.; Black, L.; Rich, E.; Andersen, D.F. The disconfirmatory interview as a strategy for the assessment of system dynamics models. Syst. Dyn. Rev. 2012, 28, 255–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nescolarde-Selva, J.A.; Usó-Doménech, J.-L.; Gash, H. What Are Ideological Systems? Systems 2017, 5, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldman, D.C. The development and enforcement of group norms. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1984, 9, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tajfel, H. Human Groups and Social Categories: Studies in Social Psychology; Cup Archive; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Wei, W.; Gu, C.; Yang, C. Examining the Influence of Moral Norms on Dockless Shared Bicycle Users’ Parking Behavior—An Exploratory Study Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior. Systems 2022, 10, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, J.C.; Hogg, M.A.; Oakes, P.J.; Reicher, S.D.; Wetherell, M.S. Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory; Basil Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Dirks, K.T.; Ferrin, D.L. The role of trust in organizational settings. Organ. Sci. 2001, 12, 450–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mussweiler, T. Comparison processes in social judgment: Mechanisms and consequences. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, J.C. Social identification and psychological group formation. In The Social Dimension: European Developments in Social Psychology; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1984; Volume 2, pp. 518–538. [Google Scholar]
- Turner, J.C.; Reynolds, K.J. Self-categorization theory. In Handbook of Theories in Social Psychology; Sage Publications Ltd.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2011; Volume 2, pp. 399–417. [Google Scholar]
- Granovetter, M. Threshold models of collective behavior. Am. J. Sociol. 1987, 83, 1420–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Neumann, J.; Morgenstern, O. Theory of Games and Economic Behavior; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1944. [Google Scholar]
- Clore, G.L.; Gasper, K.; Garvin, E. Affect as information. In Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2001; pp. 121–144. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz, N.; Clore, G.L. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 45, 513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, J.C. Social Influence; Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.: Pacific Grove, CA, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Forsyth, D.R. Group Dynamics; Cole Publishing Company: Brooks, CA, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Snow, D.A.; Benford, R.D. Master frames and cycles of protest. Front. Soc. Mov. Theory 1992, 133, 155. [Google Scholar]
- Snow, D.A.; Rochford, E.B., Jr.; Worden, S.K.; Benford, R.D. Frame alignment processes, micromobilization, and movement participation. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1986, 51, 464–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Axelrod, R. An evolutionary approach to norms. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 1986, 80, 1095–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keltner, D.; Haidt, J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cogn. Emot. 2003, 17, 297–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López, F.L.y.; Luck, M.; d’Inverno, M. Constraining autonomy through norms. In Proceedings of the 1st International Joint Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, Part 2, Auckland, NZ, USA, 9–13 May 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Salimi, K. Norm Contestation and Its Effects on Emergence of a New Norm. Ph.D. Thesis, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Campos-Vazquez, R.M.; Cuilty, E. The role of emotions on risk aversion: A prospect theory experiment. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2014, 50, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertain. 1992, 5, 297–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, G. Achieving emotions in collective action: Emotional processes and movement mobilization in the 1989 Chinese student movement. Sociol. Q. 2000, 41, 593–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sjöstedt, R. The discursive origins of a doctrine: Norms, identity, and securitization under Harry S. Truman and George W. Bush. Foreign Policy Anal. 2007, 3, 233–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joseph, K.; Morgan, G.P.; Martin, M.K.; Carley, K.M. On the coevolution of stereotype, culture, and social relationships: An agent-based model. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 2014, 32, 295–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barlas, Y. Formal aspects of model validity and validation in system dynamics. Syst. Dyn. Rev. J. Syst. Dyn. Soc. 1996, 12, 183–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barlas, Y. Credibility, validity and testing of Dynamic Simulation Models. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Simulation and Modeling Methodologies, Technologies and Applications, Lisbon, Portugal, 29–31 July 2016; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Buliali, J.L. A model of reliability, average reliability, availability, maintainability and supportability for services with system dynamics approach. In Proceedings of the 2018 International Seminar on Intelligent Technology and Its Applications (ISITIA), Bali, Indonesia, 30–31 August 2018; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Mudjahidin; Hendrawan, R.A.; Aristio, A.P.; Buliali, J.L.; Yuniarto, M.N. Testing methods on system dynamics: A model of reliability, average reliability, and demand of service. Procedia Comput. Sci. 2019, 161, 968–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, H.; Andersen, D.F. Building confidence in causal maps generated from purposive text data: Mapping transcripts of the Federal Reserve. Syst. Dyn. Rev. 2012, 28, 311–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qudrat-Ullah, H.; Seong, B.S. How to do structural validity of a system dynamics type simulation model: The case of an energy policy model. Energy Policy 2010, 38, 2216–2224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwaninger, M.; Grösser, S. System dynamics modeling: Validation for quality assurance. In System Dynamics: Theory and Applications; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2020; pp. 119–138. [Google Scholar]
- Senge, P.M.; Forrester, J.W. Tests for building confidence in system dynamics models. Syst. Dyn. TIMS Stud. Manag. Sci. 1980, 14, 209–228. [Google Scholar]
- Sterman, J. Business Dynamics; McGraw-Hill, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Tomoaia-Cotisel, A.; Allen, S.D.; Kim, H.; Andersen, D.; Chalabi, Z. Rigorously interpreted quotation analysis for evaluating causal loop diagrams in late-stage conceptualization. Syst. Dyn. Rev. 2022, 38, 41–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mahoney, J. Path dependence in historical sociology. Theory Soc. 2000, 29, 507–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pierson, P. Increasing returns, path dependence, and the study of politics. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 2000, 94, 251–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aminzade, R. Historical sociology and time. Sociol. Methods Res. 1992, 20, 456–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zuckerman, A.S. Reformulating explanatory standards and advancing theory in comparative politics. In Comparative Politics: Rationality, Culture, and Structure; Lichbach, M., Zuckerman, A.S., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997; pp. 277–310. [Google Scholar]
- Gilardi, F. Who learns from what in policy diffusion processes? Am. J. Polit. Sci. 2010, 54, 650–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carter, D. Stonewall: The Riots that Sparked the Gay Revolution; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Hirshman, L.R. Victory: The Triumphant Gay Revolution; Harper Perennial: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Lott, J.; John, R.; Kenny, L.W. Did women’s suffrage change the size and scope of government? J. Political Econ. 1999, 107, 1163–1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mullinix, K.J.; Bolsen, T.; Norris, R.J. The feedback effects of controversial police use of force. Polit. Behav. 2021, 43, 881–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, B. Gay Power!: The Stonewall Riots and the Gay Rights Movement, 1969; Twenty-First Century Books: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Richardson, G.P. Can systems thinking be an antidote to extensive evil? Syst. Res. Behav. Sci. 2021, 38, 401–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloomfield, A. Norm antipreneurs and theorising resistance to normative change. Rev. Int. Stud. 2016, 42, 310–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaidise, E. The role of civil society in developing and consolidating democracy: Evidence from Israel. In Proceedings of the European Consortium for Political Research Workshop, Uppsala, Sweden, 13–18 April 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Parsons, T.; Shils, E.A. Towards a General Theory of Social Action; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1951. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz, S.H. Basic human values: Theory, measurement, and applications. Rev. Française Sociol. 1992, 47, 929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bicchieri, C. The Grammar of Society: The Nature and Dynamics of Social Norms; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Dechesne, F.; Dignum, V.; Tan, Y.-H. Understanding compliance differences between legal and social norms: The case of smoking ban. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, Taipei, Taiwan, 2–6 May 2011; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Vickers, G. Values, Norms, and Policies. Policy Sci. 1973, 4, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mercer, J. Feeling like a state: Social emotion and identity. Int. Theory 2014, 6, 515–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winston, C. Norm structure, diffusion, and evolution: A conceptual approach. Eur. J. Int. Relat. 2018, 24, 638–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nguyen, Y.; Noussair, C.N. Risk aversion and emotions. Pac. Econ. Rev. 2014, 19, 296–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wake, S.; Wormwood, J.; Satpute, A.B. The influence of fear on risk taking: A meta-analysis. Cogn. Emot. 2020, 34, 1143–1159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salimi, K.; Frydenlund, E.; Padilla, J.J.; Haaland, H.; Wallevik, H. How does humanitarianism spread? Modeling the origins of citizen initiatives through norm diffusion. In Proceedings of the 2018 Winter Simulation Conference (WSC), Gothenburg, Sweden, 9–12 December 2018; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Mead, E.L.; Rimal, R.N.; Ferrence, R.; Cohen, J.E. Understanding the sources of normative influence on behavior: The example of tobacco. Soc. Sci. Med. 2014, 115, 139–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Posten, A.C.; Mussweiler, T. When distrust frees your mind: The stereotype-reducing effects of distrust. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Marx, G.T. Thoughts on a neglected category of social movement participant: The agent provocateur and the informant. Am. J. Sociol. 1974, 80, 402–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Model Variables | Claim/ Assumption | Number of Claims and/or Assumptions | Description | Reference |
---|---|---|---|---|
Formation of Violator Group | Claim | 9 | Perception of similarity based on shared interest shapes a new group | [34,35] |
Likelihood of Feeling Angry | Assumption | 14 | Punishment might cause anger among people who perceive the same grievances | Based on historical evidence |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Salimi, K.; Richman, J.T.; Karp, R.; Richardson, G.P.; Andersen, D. Emergence of a Norm from Resistance: Using Simulation to Explore the Macro Implications of Social Identity Theory. Systems 2022, 10, 143. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10050143
Salimi K, Richman JT, Karp R, Richardson GP, Andersen D. Emergence of a Norm from Resistance: Using Simulation to Explore the Macro Implications of Social Identity Theory. Systems. 2022; 10(5):143. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10050143
Chicago/Turabian StyleSalimi, Khadijeh, Jesse T. Richman, Regina Karp, George P. Richardson, and David Andersen. 2022. "Emergence of a Norm from Resistance: Using Simulation to Explore the Macro Implications of Social Identity Theory" Systems 10, no. 5: 143. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10050143
APA StyleSalimi, K., Richman, J. T., Karp, R., Richardson, G. P., & Andersen, D. (2022). Emergence of a Norm from Resistance: Using Simulation to Explore the Macro Implications of Social Identity Theory. Systems, 10(5), 143. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10050143