Next Article in Journal
Comparative Proteomic Analysis of Non-Bleached and Bleached Fragments of the Hydrocoral Millepora complanata Reveals Stress Response Signatures Following the 2015–2016 ENSO Event in the Mexican Caribbean
Previous Article in Journal
Engineered Exosomes Complexed with Botulinum Toxin Type A for Enhanced Anti-Aging Effects on Skin
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Impact of Male Social Status on Vaginal Secretory Responses in Mice

1
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
2
Gill Institute for Neuroscience, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Biology 2025, 14(8), 1041; https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14081041
Submission received: 6 July 2025 / Revised: 11 August 2025 / Accepted: 12 August 2025 / Published: 13 August 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Developmental and Reproductive Biology)

Simple Summary

A recently described murine model of vaginal secretory responses to male pheromones offers a window into how mice determine the likelihood of coitus and the chemical messengers that mediate some of these responses. Here we show that female mice experience a vaginal response to the scent of dominant but not subordinate males. In a second finding, we find that in contrast to farnesenes, a second candidate volatile preputial gland-derived chemical messenger 1-hexadecanol does not elicit a response. These findings narrow both the range of conditions and the chemical messengers that may elicit a coital preparatory response in laboratory mice.

Abstract

We have recently described a murine model of vaginal secretion that allows the measurement of minute changes in vaginal secretion. Using this model, we determined that female mice experience a vaginal secretory response to the scent of males, a response regulated by circadian and estrous factors since females did not respond during their sleep phase, nor when in metestrus. Female mice can distinguish the social status of a male by scent cues and show a preference for the scent of dominant males. We therefore tested whether or not vaginal responses to male scent differ by the social status of that male. Vaginal secretory responses were measured using a recently described method employing a colorimetric thread. In addition, while we have shown that the proposed female attractant α/β farnesenes evoked a strong vaginal response in female mice, a second volatile preputial gland-derived messenger, 1-hexadecanol, has also been proposed to serve as a female attractant. Here we test whether or not 1-hexadecanol similarly stimulates a vaginal secretory response. We now report that the female vaginal secretory response differs according to the social status of the male: the urine-borne scent of dominant males elicited a vaginal response, while samples from non-dominant males did not. In related odor-preference tests we confirmed that female mice spend more time investigating the urine scent of dominant males. We additionally tested whether or not a second putative female attractant 1-hexadecanol would elicit a vaginal secretory response. Like the α/β farnesenes, 1-hexadecanol is volatile, derived from preputial glands, and induces an investigatory response in females. However female mice did not experience a vaginal secretory response to the scent of 1-hexadecanol. We did confirm that females spent more time investigating hexadecanol over vehicle, indicating that there can be a disconnect between behavioral measures of interest and a vaginal preparatory response. In summary, we find that subordinate male mice do not elicit a vaginal secretory response, indicating that male social status impacts the physiological responses of females to the prospect of coitus. We additionally find that in contrast to farnesenes, the putative female attractant 1-hexadecanol does not elicit a vaginal response. These findings underscore the potential value of this murine model and indicate that even in mice, vaginal responses are under complex regulation.

1. Introduction

Mice rely heavily on chemical messengers to communicate key information relating to their identity, social status, emotional state, and health (reviewed in [1,2,3]). Female mice can distinguish the social status of males by scent and spend more time investigating the scent of dominant relative to subordinate males [4]. Urine of male mice carries factors that communicate their social status, particularly α and β farnesenes, volatile terpenic messengers that are produced in the male preputial gland [5]. Brain et al. [6] showed that preputial glands of subordinate males appear to be less well developed. Harvey et al. similarly showed that preputial glands were smaller in subordinate males and additionally showed profound differences in levels of α/β farnesenes, with levels much higher in dominant males relative to subordinate males [7]. If subordinate males produce less of the farnesenes, this may translate to different physiological responses in females. We have recently described a murine model of vaginal secretion that allows the measurement of minute changes in vaginal secretion in laboratory mice [8]. Exposure to the odor of male mice was sufficient to induce a robust vaginal secretory response. Such a response would serve to prepare the reproductive apparatus at the prospect of coitus (e.g., [9]). This model is well-suited to test whether or not reduced chemical messenger production in subordinate males translates into a reduced vaginal secretory response. This has not previously been tested and therefore represents a gap in knowledge. Our hypothesis is that subordinate males will elicit a reduced vaginal response relative to dominant males.
A related question has to do with the chemical messengers themselves. Is there a one-to-one relationship between a given messenger and the message or can different messengers communicate the same message? In the case of attractants, several messengers with different origins and chemical properties have been proposed, based largely on investigation times in the face of scent choice. This has provided important insights into rodent behavior, but there is an open question as to whether or not a compound determined to be an attractant in this way will also elicit a vaginal secretory response. In this model we have shown that the vaginal preparatory response can be induced by the preputial gland-derived volatile chemical messengers α/β farnesenes [8,10]. But other messengers have been identified as female attractants, including 1-hexadecanol [11,12,13] and the non-volatile major urinary protein darcin [14], as well as 3,4-dehydro-exo-brevicomin and 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole (SBT) [15]. Of these, 1-hexadecanol is, like the farnesenes, proposed to be both volatile and derived from the male preputial glands. We have demonstrated the effectiveness of farnesenes in eliciting a vaginal secretory response but 1-hexadecanol, a second volatile preputial gland-derived proposed pheromone, has not been tested, representing a second gap in knowledge. Our hypothesis is that if 1-hexadecanol is an attractant pheromone it will similarly evoke a vaginal response.
In the present study, the central aim is to make use of a previously unavailable assay to test physiological vaginal secretory responses to the prospect of coitus. We tested whether or not the social status of males impacts the vaginal secretory responses of females, and additionally tested for a secretory response to the putative female attractant 1-hexadecanol.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Animals

Mice were group-housed in standard ventilated caging (cage floor dimensions: 33.5 × 18 cm, depth 14 cm), with corn cob-based bedding (Bed-o’combs laboratory animal bedding, The Andersons, Maumee, OH, USA). Mice were group-housed 3–4 mice per cage and were fed Inotiv Teklad 2918 irradiated rodent diet ad libitum. Because mice are nocturnal, we tested their responses during their active phase by maintaining them on a reverse light cycle. If moved from a different light cycle, mice were given 2 weeks to acclimatize. CD1 strain mice were used, partly because we have found that, in contrast to the C57BL/6 strain, these mice appear to have an intact circadian response [8]. The mice were bred in a colony in the same facility and kindly provided by Dr. Ken Mackie (Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA). Only female mice were tested, though urine from males was used as an odorant. Animals were returned to the colony after the experiments. All animal care and experimental procedures used in this study were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Indiana University (protocol# 24-030, approval date 3 September 2024) and conformed to the Guidelines of the National Institutes of Health on the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Experiments complied with ARRIVE guidelines.

2.2. Sex and Number of Mice Used for Each Experiment

Female mice (age 2–6 months) were tested for vaginal responses. This age range was used because females are readily able to mate and bear young during this age range and because the vaginal secretory response is a robust physiological response that is essential for procreation. Males provided urine samples. Dominant and subordinate males were identified based on relative weight within a given cage (reviewed in [16]). In each cage of 3–4 males (age 3–6 months), the heaviest and lightest male was taken as dominant and subordinate, respectively, if there was at least a 10 g difference in their weights.
Total number of mice used for these experiments: 98.
Responses to dominant males: 10 females.
Responses to subordinate males: 9 females (repeated with proestrus/estrus only: 8 females).
Scent investigation dominant vs. subordinate: 21 females.
Responses to 1-hexadecanol: 21 females (repeated with proestrus/estrus only: 13 females).
Responses to 1-hexadecanol (physical contact): 9 females.
Scent investigation time 1-hexadecanol: 7 females.
Sample sizes were determined in our previous study introducing this method of measuring vaginal secretory function [8]. A specific randomization paradigm was not employed to allocate mice for experiments.

2.3. Measurement of Vaginal Moisture

We have recently described the use of colorimetric threads for measurement of vaginal moisture as well as the method of manufacture of these threads [8]. An image of the cannulated thread and a sample stained thread for measurement can be seen in Figure 1 of Andreis et al., 2022 [17]. This method is brief (10 s of measurement), minimally invasive, well-tolerated by the mice, and does not require anesthesia. The 10 s duration results in the measurement of clear differences under various experimental conditions, as outlined in [8]. Briefly, to allow for consistent placement in the vaginal cavity, a phenol red-coated thread is inserted into a glass capillary (A-M Systems, 0.86 mm inner diameter, Cat#:593200, fire-polished to prevent injury), leaving 3 mm of thread outside the opening of the capillary. The capillary is then placed into the vaginal cavity of an unanesthetized mouse for 10 s [8]. The distance that liquid from the vaginal cavity travels along the thread is quantified and taken as a measure of vaginal moisture, as described previously [8].

2.4. Preparation of 1-Hexadecanol as an Odorant

1-Hexadecanol is implicated as a female attractant and was prepared based on Zhang et al. [12]. For these experiments, 1-hexadecanol was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (Burlington, MA, USA). 1-Hexadecanol was prepared as a 1M stock solution in ethanol and diluted to 100 μM in mineral oil. An amount of 1 mL of the mineral oil mixture was placed in a plastic dish between the wire cage top and the filter top. This permitted mice to smell the odorant while preventing physical access. For the investigation time experiment we prepared the 1-hexadecanol as above but diluted the stock solution to 100 μM in ethanol. Vehicle consisted of a 1:10,000 dilution of ethanol in mineral oil.

2.5. Collection and Preparation of Urine

For the urine testing condition, urine was obtained from a male mouse by scruffing the animal and collecting, with a kimwipe, freely secreted urine. The kimwipe from one male was placed on a separator positioned on the wire caging, in a plastic weigh boat, but below the cage filter top, so preventing physical contact with the mice while sealing in the scent. Females were placed into the cage and allowed to explore. After 1 h, a second vaginal moisture measurement was obtained and females were moved back into their home cage. A cage prepared in this manner was used only once (for one cage of females).

2.6. General Method of Exposure to Odorant (1-Hexadecanol)

For odorant exposure experiments, several cages containing a group of CD1 strain female mice (3–4/cage) were brought into an adjoining room separated by a closed door. Mice were tested in late-morning, starting at 10AM to reduce the likelihood of unusual scents. All baseline vaginal moisture readings were obtained for each mouse as described under Measurement of Vaginal Moisture above. This takes less than a minute per mouse so is accomplished in under half an hour. After this, the first cage of female mice were transferred into a scent-containing cage and allowed to explore. Subsequent groups of mice were transferred at ~4–5 min intervals. After an hour of exploration, the first group of mice were tested, with vaginal moisture readings taken for each female a second time to allow comparison to her own pre-exposure baseline. Subsequent cages were tested as they reached 1 h. Experimenters (both the experimenter obtaining a sample and the individual quantifying the distance that the sample traveled along the thread) were aware of the experiment that was being conducted (i.e., they were not blinded).
In a follow-on experiment, mice were allowed to physically interact with 1-hexadecanol. The experiment was conducted as above except that 1 mL of 100 μM 1-hexadecanol was added to a kimwipe that was mixed in with fresh bedding. During the hour of exploration females reliably came into physical contact with the 1-hexadecanol infused kimwipes, allowing for contact-exposure.

2.7. Measuring Investigation Time

Investigation time has a long history of use to assess olfactory discrimination [18]. For studies of investigation time, we strove to mimic the conditions of Zhang et al. since they reported positive findings with [13]. Fresh/clean empty cages were used (dimensions as above). On the distal ends of cages, in the center, circles (20 mm diameter) were drawn on the outside of the cage using faint blue marker to decrease visual salience of the mark. The circle was placed 5 cm from the cage bottom to allow ready accessibility to mice yet requiring some effort. For scent marking, 10 μL of solution was pipetted into the circle area on the inside of the cage and smeared evenly over the area using the pipette tip. Marks were given 5 min time to dry. At this time, the female mouse was placed into the cage, in the middle, and the top of the cage was covered with a clear, clean glass pane to prevent animal escape and other smells from intruding while offering full visibility to scorers. The mouse was allowed 5 min for exploration. Nose to circle area interactions were timed by two scorers with stopwatches, with one scorer for each end of the cage. Scorers alternated sides. As in Zhang et al., animals that did not interact with either of the circles were excluded from the statistical analysis. For the 1-hexadecanol experiment one of eight animals was excluded.

2.8. Method for Determining Estrous Phase

To determine the estrous cycle phase of a given mouse we used the method described by Byers et al. [19]. We obtained a vaginal smear by pipetting 10 μL of sterile saline into the proximal vaginal canal using a 20 μL pipette. The liquid was then collected back into the pipette and placed onto a slide and covered with a cover slip. The slide was then imaged within 3 h using a camera-equipped phase-contrast microscope. The picture was saved for scoring. Evaluation of the images was done via a two-person consensus approach: each person scored the images; in the case of disagreement, each made a case for a given phase and the scorers arrived at a consensus. Occasionally, scorers were uncertain about the phase because it exhibited characteristics of both. This occurred most frequently for estrus vs. metestrus. In that case, the sample was scored accordingly (e.g., estrus/metestrus) and the vaginal response data were excluded from analyses. These ambiguous estrous typings represented ~10% of the total.
Non-receptive estrous phases (i.e., metestrus, diestrus) were used as exclusion criteria for the repeated 1-hexadecanol vaginal response experiment (Section 3.3) to rule out the possibility that the non-response was due to a preponderance of non-receptive females in that experiment.

2.9. Statistics

Analyses were done using Graphpad Prism version 10. Experiments were analyzed using a two-tailed paired t-test comparing an experimental condition to the same-animal baseline. Detailed assessment of vaginal measurement data, including determination of sample size, was previously reported in [8].

3. Results

3.1. Vaginal Secretory Responses to the Scent of Dominant vs. Subordinate Males

In the initial experiments, female CD1 strain mice of indeterminate estrous phase were exposed to the urine of dominant or subordinate males, as described in Section 2. The urine of dominant males induced a vaginal secretory response (Figure 1A, paired t-test vs. baseline for dominant males, p = 0.046, t = 2.31, df = 9, n = 10), while the urine of subordinate males did not (Figure S1; p = 0.51, t = 0.69, df = 8, n = 9). In this initial experiment, the estrous state of the female mice was not monitored because we had previously found that the vaginal secretory response was sufficiently robust to be observed reliably in a general population of adult female mice. However, the negative finding for subordinate males could, in principle, have arisen if a large proportion of females were in their non-receptive state (e.g., [8]). We therefore repeated the experiment with a different cohort of females, now monitoring the estrous phase and including only data from females in their receptive phase (pro-estrus and estrus). We found that here too there was no response to subordinate males (Figure 1B; p = 0.31, t = 1.102, df = 7, n = 8).

3.2. Investigation Time of the Scents of Dominant vs. Subordinate Males

We confirmed previous findings [4] showing that females spend more time investigating the scent of dominant (DOM) vs. subordinate (SUB) males (Figure 2, paired t-test investigation time DOM vs. SUB: p = 0.025, t = 2.38, df = 20, n = 21).

3.3. Vaginal Secretory Responses to the Putative Female Attractant 1-Hexadecanol

As noted, we have previously reported that farnesenes elicit a robust vaginal secretory response in female mice of indeterminate estrus phase. Here we tested the response to a second candidate volatile pheromone 1-hexadecanol [12]. We exposed mice as described by Zhang et al. to closely approximate their method (described in Section 2) but found that hexadecanol did not elicit a vaginal secretory response (Figure S2, paired t-test vs. baseline: p = 0.14, t = 1.59, df = 13, n = 14). Because this initial finding was negative, we repeated the experiment with a different cohort of mice of known phase of estrus. We found that mice in their receptive phase (proestrus and estrus) still did not respond to the hexadecanol (Figure 3, paired t-test vs. baseline: p = 0.87, t = 0.1721, df = 12, n = 13). We additionally tested whether or not physical interaction with the hexadecanol would result in a positive response. Mice that were in their receptive phase did not experience a vaginal secretory response to hexadecanol (Figure S2B, paired t-test vs. baseline: p = 0.063, t = 0.497, df = 8, n = 9).

3.4. Female Mice Do Spend More Time Investigating Hexadecanol

Our negative findings for vaginal secretory responses to hexadecanol could be explained if the mice were unable to smell the hexadecanol. We therefore tested investigation times as reported by Zhang et al., closely mimicking their experimental approach. We confirmed that females spend more time investigating hexadecanol than vehicle (Figure 4; paired t-test for time spent investigating hexadecanol vs. vehicle, p = 0.008, t = 3.90, df = 6, n = 7), indicating that they are able to detect the hexadecanol.

4. Discussion

Mice rely on chemical messengers to communicate a wide variety of information, including social status. We recently determined that female mice respond to male volatile pheromones with a vaginal secretory response, presumably as a preparatory response to the prospect of coitus. This offers a window into the physiological responses to pheromonal signals that we have here employed to examine whether or not this response is impacted by the male’s social status. We find that female laboratory mice experience a vaginal secretory response to dominant, but not to subordinate, males. In addition, while we had previously reported that the scent of chemical messengers α/β farnesenes induced a vaginal secretory response, here report that a second proposed female attractant, hexadecanol, does not.
When housed together, male mice and rats rapidly establish a social dominance hierarchy with profound physiological and behavioral consequences that also impact female responsiveness and, likely, mating success [4]. Chemical messengers may help females ascertain the social status of a given male, with evidence pointing to male preputial glands as a likely source of such chemical messengers. These glands undergo rapid morphological changes in response to male social status [6] and secrete both volatile and non-volatile compounds that communicate social hierarchy to conspecifics [10,20]. Females prefer to linger in spaces with higher preputial gland excretion content [21]. Several volatile and nonvolatile messengers have been actively investigated with several lines of evidence pointing to α/β farnesenes. The levels of farnesenes are higher in the preputial glands of dominant males, with levels shifting in response to changes in social status in as little as a week’s time [7]. α/β farnesenes therefore likely provide key information that females use to determine the social status of males. As noted, it is now possible to discern a physiological response–vaginal secretion–to male pheromones, and we have shown that farnesenes can elicit the vaginal secretory response [8].
But are there other chemical messengers that play the same role? And are there attractants that do not trigger a vaginal response? The study of attractants has been a subject of interest, not least for the pest-control industry, and a variety of additional candidate female attractants have been proposed, including the non-volatile darcin [14] and hexadecanol [11,13]. We chose hexadecanol here because, like the farnesenes, hexadecanol was described as volatile and produced by preputial glands [12,13]. In contrast to farnesenes, members of the terpene family that are known for their volatility, hexadecanol is a long chain fatty alcohol with a comparatively low vapor pressure [22], but our experiments suggest that mice are able to detect its scent. It is possible that only a subset of attractants also stimulate a vaginal response. This raises the question of what constitutes an attractant. The standard method of determining attraction has been to quantify the time spent investigating scents to assay the valence of those scents to rodents (e.g., [11]). In the present context, female mice that spent more time investigating a scent have generally been taken as revealing an attraction. But investigation time is also used to examine novelty, as in the novel object recognition assay (e.g., [23]). Duration of investigation is a behavior that may be subject to many influences. Attraction, novelty, threat, or simply the balance of chemical messengers may each merit investigation. And what is taken as attraction may in fact be interest. In this respect, a stimulated vaginal secretory response offers a more defined window into what a putative attractant is communicating. While 1-hexadecanol does not appear to communicate the prospect of coitus, it may well communicate other information and elicit different responses.
These findings additionally have a bearing on the preparation hypothesis that posits that females will automatically experience a vaginal secretory response when confronted with the likelihood of coitus (e.g., [9]). This is presumably at least partly to protect the reproductive apparatus but may also serve to provide a more welcoming environment (e.g., pH) for spermatozoa. We recently showed that female laboratory mice do not invariably respond to a stimulus that would ordinarily elicit a vaginal secretory response: they do not respond during their rest phase or during metestrus. This implies that a more nuanced preparation hypothesis may apply here, one that may rely in part on how the likelihood of coitus is assessed by the female. In many species there is a courtship process, and there is no reason to suppose that mice differ in this regard. Vocalizations outside the human auditory range likely play a role (reviewed in [24]), as do chemical messengers, as discussed above. It is possible that females do not experience a vaginal secretory response to the scent of a subordinate male because interaction with a subordinate male is unlikely to lead to mating. A study of the impact of dominance on mating success showed that subordinate males are less likely to initiate mating and do not even approach females in the first 15 min [4]. Vaginal preparation for mating in that instance would be a pointless expenditure of physiological resources since coitus is unlikely.

5. Conclusions

The ability to monitor vaginal secretory responses to encounters with males offers a window into how mice determine the likelihood of coitus and the chemical messengers that mediate some of these responses. Here we have shown that female mice experience a vaginal response to the scent of dominant but not subordinate males. In a second finding we find that, in contrast to farnesenes, a second candidate volatile preputial gland-derived chemical messenger, 1-hexadecanol, does not elicit a response. These findings narrow both the range of conditions and the chemical messengers that may elicit a coital preparatory response in laboratory mice.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/biology14081041/s1, Figure S1: Vaginal secretory responses to subordinate males in females of unknown estrous phase. In mice not tested for phase of estrus, vaginal secretory responses were not seen in response to the scent of subordinate males. Measurements were taken before (baseline, BSL) and an hour after exposure to urine from subordinate males (SUB). Bar graph shows mean + SEM. ns, not significant. n = 9. Figure S2: Vaginal secretory responses to the putative sexual attractant hexadecanol. (A) In mice not tested for phase of estrus, exposure to the scent of hexadecanol (“hexadec”) for 1 h did not elicit a vaginal secretory response relative to baseline (BSL). (B) Mice in their receptive phase (proestrus/estrus) allowed physical contact with hexadecanol still did not experience an increase in vaginal moisture. Bar graphs show mean + SEM. Ns, not significant. p = 0.14 (A) and p = 0.63 (B), by paired t-test vs. baseline, n = 14, 9.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, N.M. and A.S.; formal analysis, S.M., A.S. and N.M.; investigation, S.M., A.S. and N.M.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S.; writing—review and editing, N.M. and S.M.; supervision, A.S. and N.M.; project administration, A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of Indiana University (protocol code 24-030, approved 3 September 2024).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data that make up the figures in this manuscript will be included in a separate Supplementary File.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Hurst, J.L.; Beynon, R.J. Scent wars: The chemobiology of competitive signalling in mice. Bioessays 2004, 26, 1288–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Roberts, S.A.; Davidson, A.J.; McLean, L.; Beynon, R.J.; Hurst, J.L. Pheromonal induction of spatial learning in mice. Science 2012, 338, 1462–1465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Martin-Sanchez, A.; McLean, L.; Beynon, R.J.; Hurst, J.L.; Ayala, G.; Lanuza, E.; Martinez-Garcia, F. From sexual attraction to maternal aggression: When pheromones change their behavioural significance. Horm. Behav. 2015, 68, 65–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. D’Amato, F.R. Effects of male social status on reproductive success and on behavior in mice (Mus musculus). J. Comp. Psychol. 1988, 102, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Jemiolo, B.; Xie, T.M.; Novotny, M. Socio-sexual olfactory preference in female mice: Attractiveness of synthetic chemosignals. Physiol. Behav. 1991, 50, 1119–1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Brain, P.F.; Homady, M.H.; Mainard, P. Preputial glands, dominance and aggressiveness in mice. Bull. Zool. 1983, 50, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Harvey, S.; Jemiolo, B.; Novotny, M. Pattern of volatile compounds in dominant and subordinate male mouse urine. J. Chem. Ecol. 1989, 15, 2061–2072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Murataeva, N.; Mattox, S.; Yust, K.; Du, W.; Straiker, A. Murine vaginal secretory responses to a male volatile chemical messenger. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 27707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Dawson, S.J.; Suschinsky, K.D.; Lalumiere, M.L. Habituation of sexual responses in men and women: A test of the preparation hypothesis of women’s genital responses. J. Sex. Med. 2013, 10, 990–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ma, W.; Miao, Z.; Novotny, M.V. Induction of estrus in grouped female mice (Mus domesticus) by synthetic analogues of preputial gland constituents. Chem. Senses 1999, 24, 289–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Liu, Q.; Zhang, Y.; Wang, P.; Guo, X.; Wu, Y.; Zhang, J.X.; Huang, L. Two preputial gland-secreted pheromones evoke sexually dimorphic neural pathways in the mouse vomeronasal system. Front. Cell Neurosci. 2019, 13, 455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Zhang, J.X.; Rao, X.P.; Sun, L.; Zhao, C.H.; Qin, X.W. Putative chemical signals about sex, individuality, and genetic background in the preputial gland and urine of the house mouse (Mus musculus). Chem. Senses 2007, 32, 293–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Zhang, J.X.; Liu, Y.J.; Zhang, J.H.; Sun, L. Dual role of preputial gland secretion and its major components in sex recognition of mice. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 95, 388–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Roberts, S.A.; Simpson, D.M.; Armstrong, S.D.; Davidson, A.J.; Robertson, D.H.; McLean, L.; Beynon, R.J.; Hurst, J.L. Darcin: A male pheromone that stimulates female memory and sexual attraction to an individual male’s odour. BMC Biol. 2010, 8, 75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Novotny, M.; Harvey, S.; Jemiolo, B.; Alberts, J. Synthetic pheromones that promote inter-male aggression in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1985, 82, 2059–2061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Fulenwider, H.D.; Caruso, M.A.; Ryabinin, A.E. Manifestations of domination: Assessments of social dominance in rodents. Genes. Brain Behav. 2022, 21, e12731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Andreis, K.; Billingsley, J.; Naimi Shirazi, K.; Wager-Miller, J.; Johnson, C.; Bradshaw, H.; Straiker, A. Cannabinoid CB1 receptors regulate salivation. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 14182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gregg, B.; Thiessen, D.D. A simple method of olfactory discrimination of urines for the Mongolian gerbil, Meriones unguiculatus. Physiol. Behav. 1981, 26, 1133–1136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Byers, S.L.; Wiles, M.V.; Dunn, S.L.; Taft, R.A. Mouse estrous cycle identification tool and images. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e35538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Gawienowski, A.M.; Orsulak, P.J.; Stacewicz-Sapuntzakis, M.; Joseph, B.M. Presence of sex pheromone in preputial glands of male rats. J. Endocrinol. 1975, 67, 283–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Orsulak, P.J.; Gawienowski, A.M. Olfactory preferences for the rat preputial gland. Biol. Reprod. 1972, 6, 219–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ho, D.; Williamson, S.; Richmond, H. NIST Chemistry Handbook; National Institute of Standards and Technology (NIST): Gaithersburg, MD, USA, 2025. [Google Scholar]
  23. Zhang, R.; Xue, G.; Wang, S.; Zhang, L.; Shi, C.; Xie, X. Novel object recognition as a facile behavior test for evaluating drug effects in AbetaPP/PS1 Alzheimer’s disease mouse model. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2012, 31, 801–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Capas-Peneda, S.; Saavedra Torres, Y.; Prins, J.B.; Olsson, I.A.S. From mating to milk access: A review of reproductive vocal communication in mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 833168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Vaginal secretory responses to dominant or subordinate males. Vaginal moisture was measured before (baseline, BSL) and after female CD1 strain laboratory mice were exposed to male urine. (A) Females responded to urine of dominant (DOM) males. (B) Females were non-responsive to the scent of subordinate (SUB) males even in a receptive phase of estrus. Bar graphs show mean + SEM. ns, not significant, *, p < 0.05 by paired t-test vs. same-animal baseline. n = 10, 8.
Figure 1. Vaginal secretory responses to dominant or subordinate males. Vaginal moisture was measured before (baseline, BSL) and after female CD1 strain laboratory mice were exposed to male urine. (A) Females responded to urine of dominant (DOM) males. (B) Females were non-responsive to the scent of subordinate (SUB) males even in a receptive phase of estrus. Bar graphs show mean + SEM. ns, not significant, *, p < 0.05 by paired t-test vs. same-animal baseline. n = 10, 8.
Biology 14 01041 g001
Figure 2. Investigation time of scents of dominant vs. subordinate males. Duration of investigation time (seconds) shows that female mice spend more time investigating urine of dominant (DOM) over subordinate (SUB) males. Bar graphs show mean + SEM. *, p < 0.05, paired t-test investigation time DOM vs. SUB: p = 0.025, n = 21.
Figure 2. Investigation time of scents of dominant vs. subordinate males. Duration of investigation time (seconds) shows that female mice spend more time investigating urine of dominant (DOM) over subordinate (SUB) males. Bar graphs show mean + SEM. *, p < 0.05, paired t-test investigation time DOM vs. SUB: p = 0.025, n = 21.
Biology 14 01041 g002
Figure 3. Vaginal secretory responses to the putative sexual attractant hexadecanol. In female mice in their receptive estrous phase, exposure to the scent of hexadecanol (HEX) for 1 h did not elicit a vaginal secretory response relative to baseline (BSL). Bar graphs show mean + SEM. ns, not significant. p = 0.87 by paired t-test vs. baseline, n = 13.
Figure 3. Vaginal secretory responses to the putative sexual attractant hexadecanol. In female mice in their receptive estrous phase, exposure to the scent of hexadecanol (HEX) for 1 h did not elicit a vaginal secretory response relative to baseline (BSL). Bar graphs show mean + SEM. ns, not significant. p = 0.87 by paired t-test vs. baseline, n = 13.
Biology 14 01041 g003
Figure 4. Investigation time of hexadecanol. Duration of investigation time (in seconds) is shown for vehicle and hexadecanol (100 μM). Bar graphs show mean + SEM. **, p < 0.01, paired t-test hexadecanol vs. vehicle, n = 7.
Figure 4. Investigation time of hexadecanol. Duration of investigation time (in seconds) is shown for vehicle and hexadecanol (100 μM). Bar graphs show mean + SEM. **, p < 0.01, paired t-test hexadecanol vs. vehicle, n = 7.
Biology 14 01041 g004
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Murataeva, N.; Mattox, S.; Straiker, A. The Impact of Male Social Status on Vaginal Secretory Responses in Mice. Biology 2025, 14, 1041. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14081041

AMA Style

Murataeva N, Mattox S, Straiker A. The Impact of Male Social Status on Vaginal Secretory Responses in Mice. Biology. 2025; 14(8):1041. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14081041

Chicago/Turabian Style

Murataeva, Natalia, Sam Mattox, and Alex Straiker. 2025. "The Impact of Male Social Status on Vaginal Secretory Responses in Mice" Biology 14, no. 8: 1041. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14081041

APA Style

Murataeva, N., Mattox, S., & Straiker, A. (2025). The Impact of Male Social Status on Vaginal Secretory Responses in Mice. Biology, 14(8), 1041. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14081041

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop