Next Article in Journal
Diagnosing a 12-Item Dataset of Raven Matrices: With Dexter
Next Article in Special Issue
Turtles All the Way Down: From g to Mitochondrial Functioning
Previous Article in Journal
Mapping Mind-Brain Development: Towards a Comprehensive Theory
Previous Article in Special Issue
Mitochondrial Functioning and Its Relation to Higher-Order Cognitive Processes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Commentary

Mitochondrial Functioning ≠ General Intelligence

by
Alexander O. Savi
1,*,
Han L. J. van der Maas
2,
Gunter K. J. Maris
3 and
Maarten Marsman
2
1
Department of Educational and Family Studies, VU University Amsterdam, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands
2
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, 1018 WS Amsterdam, The Netherlands
3
ACTNext, by ACT, Iowa City, IA 52245, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 25 March 2020 / Revised: 20 April 2020 / Accepted: 24 April 2020 / Published: 3 May 2020
Geary puts forward an appealing argument for the consideration of mitochondrial functioning as a candidate for a formative g Geary (2019); it is also an ambitious argument. After Spearman’s discovery of a psychometric g in 1904 Spearman (1904) and his speculations about a psychological g in 1927 Spearman (1927), researchers have been attracted by the simplicity of a single unifying factor. To date, the identification of this hypothesized factor is still awaited. In fact, various contemporary theories suggest that local complex dynamic mechanisms drive the emergence of the global phenomenon g Dickens and Flynn (2001); van der Maas et al. (2006); Kovacs and Conway (2016); Savi et al. (2019). In the following, we comment on Geary’s attempt at debunking g and suggest a different—but nonetheless compatible—perspective. We also—not entirely accidentally—suggest the network perspective van der Maas et al. (2019) as a more principled method that enables the study of the influence of potentially powerful factors on intelligence in their biological, psychological, and societal context.

1. Hierarchies ≠ Unidirectional

One may indeed be tempted to think that a nested structure suggests that “deficits or inefficiencies at lower levels will ripple through all higher levels but deficits at higher levels …need not have broad influences at lower levels” Geary (2019) (p. 2). Evidently, reality is more complex. As Sternberg pointed out in this very issue Sternberg (2020), in the field of (behavioral) epigenetics many top-down mechanisms have been identified. More specifically, various mechanisms—including DNA methylation Day and Sweatt (2010)—have been found to be involved with memory formation and storage, which is evidently linked to intelligence. We believe that as long as such mechanisms are still poorly understood, one must either be wary of very strict assumptions or establish evidence that mitochondrial functioning is truly independent of higher-level systems.

2. Biological Mechanisms ≠ Formative Factors

In addition, by suggesting that “mitochondrial functioning is the most fundamental biological mechanism”, Geary (2019) (p. 1) seems to ignore influences on mitochondrial functioning by not just higher-level, but also same and lower-level systems; that is, the complex biological and psychological reality in which our mitochondria function. Such influences, whether unidirectional or cyclic, impede the determination of a formative factor or the “most fundamental mechanism”. Cyclic mechanisms can preeminently create chicken-or-the-egg dilemmas. In fact, cyclic mechanisms are abundant in biological systems such as in humans. We highly recommend Bechtel and Abrahamsen’s elaborate account of complex biological mechanisms Bechtel and Abrahamsen (2011). In conclusion, we believe that one cannot simply assume that other (cyclic) mechanisms are either absent or negligible, and that mitochondrial functioning is indeed an independent causal mechanism.

3. Ceilings ≠ Ceiling Effects

Then, Geary suggests that “the capacity for mitochondria to produce energy places a ceiling on the performance of higher-level systems” Geary (2019) (p. 3). This, again, is a strict assumption. A ceiling may never be reached, and if reached, lesser amounts of energy may also be sufficient for superb cognitive ability, with additional energy production providing no further contribution. Put differently, a ceiling does not guarantee a ceiling effect. Is there evidence that variation in the capacity for mitochondria to produce energy causes variation in intelligence? That is, does this ceiling ever reach a level that limits cognitive development? Additionally, are there no other factors that constrain or alter this capacity and that may be deemed equally important?

4. Verbal Theory ≠ Theory

Sternberg rightly remarks that correlation is not causation and that one should therefore be wary of correlation-backed causal claims Sternberg (2020). In addition, we argue that verbal theory is not theory, but speculation. We believe that speculation propels scientific progress; however, only when approached with appropriate care and followed up with extensive verification. Ultimately, the field of human intelligence benefits most from formal theory. Formal theories explicate the exact hypothesized mechanisms from which the observed phenomena emerge and allow for precise predictions of still unobserved phenomena. Evidently, formal theories are by no means the end of scientific progress, if only because substantially different theories can turn out to be mathematically equivalent Kruis and Maris (2016). Indeed, model predictions must be verified, experiments can be used to validate causal claims, and time-series data may provide further proof.

5. General Intelligence ≈ Network

Geary’s “nested structure of brain systems” illustrates, on a high level, some of the factors that may be involved in the dynamic human systems from which the g phenomenon ultimately emerges. However, a common representation that does more justice to all possible complexities involved is a network van der Maas et al. (2017,2019). In a network, nodes are connected by edges to represent the active elements and their relations. Interestingly, the network perspective provides a principled method for exploring the problems discussed above.
In 2006, van der Maas and colleagues introduced the very first network approach to general intelligence van der Maas et al. (2006). Fundamentally, their model proved that psychometric g can simply emerge from cyclic psychological processes. In 2019, we showed how psychometric g emerges from the wiring of facts and procedures during cognitive development Savi et al. (2019). Crucially, our model respects individual differences in the diverse cognitive abilities that shape our intellect, and although rudimentary at present, the model serves as a basis for including biological, psychological, and even societal influences. Importantly, both models were developed within a thorough formal framework and obviate the need to find a single unifying factor that explains everything.
In conclusion, networks defy a race to the reductionistic bottom Borsboom et al. (2018), as they can help acknowledge the inevitable influence of functions at different levels of the human system. Indeed, multilevel networks allow for connecting theories at these different levels. To illustrate this, the theoretical foundation for the community structure in our—mostly psychological—model (Savi et al. 2019, pp. 12–13) is nicely laid out in Barbey’s network neuroscience theory of human intelligence Barbey (2018). Certainly, we also welcome Geary’s plea to consider mitochondrial functioning as a mechanism related to general intelligence. However, we invite researchers in the field of human intelligence to respect the system’s complex and dynamic nature, to approach it as an open rather than closed system, and to explore the proposed mechanisms in their rich biological, psychological, and societal context.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.O.S.; writing–original draft preparation, A.O.S.; writing–review and editing, A.O.S., H.L.J.v.d.M., G.K.J.M., and M.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

M.M. was supported by NWO Veni Grant 451-17-017.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Geary, David C. 2019. Mitochondria as the Linchpin of General Intelligence and the Link between g, Health, and Aging. Journal of Intelligence 7: 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Spearman, Charles. 1904. “General Intelligence,” Objectively Determined and Measured. The American Journal of Psychology 15: 201–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Spearman, Charles. 1927. The Abilities of Man: Their Nature and Assessment. Available online: https://archive.org/details/b29816920 (accessed on 24 April 2020).
  4. Dickens, William T., and James R. Flynn. 2001. Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review 108: 346–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. van der Maas, Han L.J., Conor V. Dolan, Raoul P. P. P. Grasman, Jelte M. Wicherts, Hilde M. Huizenga, and Maartje E. J. Raijmakers. 2006. A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review 113: 842–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2016. Process Overlap Theory: A Unified Account of the General Factor of Intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 27: 151–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Savi, Alexander O., Maarten Marsman, Han L. J. van der Maas, and Gunter K. J. Maris. 2019. The Wiring of Intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological Science 14: 1034–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. van der Maas, Han L.J., Alexander O. Savi, Abe D. Hofman, Kees-Jan Kan, and Maarten Marsman. 2019. The Network Approach to General Intelligence. In General and Specific Mental Abilities. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, pp. 108–31, chapter 5. [Google Scholar]
  9. Sternberg, Robert J. 2020. How Mighty Are the Mitochondria in Causing Individual Differences in Intelligence?—Some Questions for David Geary. Journal of Intelligence 8: 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Day, Jeremy J., and J. David Sweatt. 2010. DNA methylation and memory formation. Nature Neuroscience 13: 1319–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Bechtel, William, and Adele Abrahamsen. 2011. Complex Biological Mechanisms. In Philosophy of Complex Systems. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 257–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kruis, Joost, and Gunter Maris. 2016. Three representations of the Ising model. Scientific Reports 6: 34175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. van der Maas, Han L.J., Kees-Jan Kan, Maarten Marsman, and Claire E. Stevenson. 2017. Network Models for Cognitive Development and Intelligence. Journal of Intelligence 5: 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Borsboom, Denny, Angélique OJ Cramer, and Annemarie Kalis. 2018. Brain disorders? Not really: Why network structures block reductionism in psychopathology research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Barbey, Aron K. 2018. Network Neuroscience Theory of Human Intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22: 8–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Savi, A.O.; van der Maas, H.L.J.; Maris, G.K.J.; Marsman, M. Mitochondrial Functioning ≠ General Intelligence. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020020

AMA Style

Savi AO, van der Maas HLJ, Maris GKJ, Marsman M. Mitochondrial Functioning ≠ General Intelligence. Journal of Intelligence. 2020; 8(2):20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020020

Chicago/Turabian Style

Savi, Alexander O., Han L. J. van der Maas, Gunter K. J. Maris, and Maarten Marsman. 2020. "Mitochondrial Functioning ≠ General Intelligence" Journal of Intelligence 8, no. 2: 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020020

APA Style

Savi, A. O., van der Maas, H. L. J., Maris, G. K. J., & Marsman, M. (2020). Mitochondrial Functioning ≠ General Intelligence. Journal of Intelligence, 8(2), 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020020

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop