Next Article in Journal
Changes in the Retinal Microvasculature Measured Using Optical Coherence Tomography Angiography According to Age
Next Article in Special Issue
Strengthened Default Mode Network Activation During Delay Discounting in Adolescents with Anorexia Nervosa After Partial Weight Restoration: A Longitudinal fMRI Study
Previous Article in Journal
High-Frequency Near-Infrared Diode Laser Irradiation Attenuates IL-1β-Induced Expression of Inflammatory Cytokines and Matrix Metalloproteinases in Human Primary Chondrocytes
Previous Article in Special Issue
Increased Amygdala Activations during the Emotional Experience of Death-Related Pictures in Complicated Grief: An fMRI Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

A Systematic Review of the Effectiveness of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation Techniques to Reduce Violence Proneness by Interfering in Anger and Irritability

by
Ángel Romero-Martínez
*,
Sara Bressanutti
and
Luis Moya-Albiol
Psychobiology Department, University of València, Blasco Ibañez Avenue 21, 46010 Valencia, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9(3), 882; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9030882
Submission received: 18 February 2020 / Revised: 6 March 2020 / Accepted: 19 March 2020 / Published: 24 March 2020

Abstract

:
The field of neurocriminology has proposed several treatments (e.g., pharmacological, brain surgery, androgen-deprivation therapy, neurofeedback) to reduce violence proneness, but unfortunately, their effectiveness has been limited due to their side-effects. Therefore, it is necessary to explore alternative techniques to improve patients’ behavioural regulation with minimal undesirable effects. In this regard, non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, which are based on applying changing magnetic fields or electric currents to interfere with cortical excitability, have revealed their usefulness in alleviating the symptomatology of several mental disorders. However, to our knowledge, there are no reviews that assess whether these techniques are useful for reducing violence proneness. Therefore, we conducted a systematic review following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) criteria using the following databases: PsycINFO, PubMed, Dialnet, Psicodoc, Web of Knowledge, and the Cochrane Library. We initially identified 3746 entries, and eventually included 56 publications. Most of the studies were unanimous in concluding that the application of these techniques over the prefrontal cortex (PFC) was not sufficient to promote anger and irritability reductions in euthymic individuals of both genders. Nevertheless, the application of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, especially transcranial direct current stimulation, over the right PFC seemed to reduce violent reactions in these individuals by interfering with the interpretation of the unfavourable situations (e.g., threating signals) or inner states that evoked anger. In antisocial and pathological populations, the conclusions were provided by a few pilot studies with important methodological weaknesses. The main conclusion of these studies was that bilateral stimulation of the PFC satisfactorily reduced anger and irritability only in inmates, patients with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), people who suffered a closed-head injury, and agitated patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Moreover, combining these techniques with risperidone considerably reduced aggressiveness in these patients. Therefore, it is necessary to be cautious about the benefits of these techniques to control anger, due the methodological weaknesses of these studies. Nonetheless, they offer valuable opportunities to prevent violence by designing new treatments combining brain stimulation with current strategies, such as psychotherapy and psychopharmacology, in order to promote lasting changes.

1. Introduction

In recent decades, criminologists have paid attention to the growing knowledge that the neurosciences offer about human behavioural dysregulations. This situation led to the birth of neurocriminology. Neurocriminology is the scientific study of the biological bases (and their interactions with environmental variables) of violence proneness, as well as the application of this knowledge to prevent and/or reduce it [1,2,3]. A deeper understanding of the aetiology of violence will make it possible to design specific treatments and/or increase the effectiveness of current ones.
Even though several psychotherapeutic treatments have been proposed to reduce violence, their effectiveness has been limited [1,2,3]. In fact, a large number of subjects tend to reoffend after treatment. Moreover, many patients tend to abandon the intervention before it ends [4]. Thus, combining the above-mentioned treatments with pharmacological strategies, such as antidepressants, mood stabilizers, antipsychotics, and beta-blockers has been recommended and tends to have long-lasting effects [5,6]. Unfortunately, in many patients, these drugs tend to present side-effects, such as loss of sexual desire, weight gain, and insomnia, among others, during the initial stages of treatment [6,7,8]. These side-effects increase the risk of discontinuing the treatment before the appearance of therapeutic effects, which in turn increases the risk of violence recidivism and/or the maintenance of previous behavioural dysregulations. Furthermore, it is well-known that some of these violent individuals refuse to take the treatment because they do not recognize that they have mental health problems [9]. Finally, not all patients tend to respond to pharmacological strategies; there are even individuals who are refractory or intolerant to specific drugs. Moreover, it is necessary to maintain their use over long periods of time, which increases the risk of discontinuation. Thus, it is important to explore alternative therapeutic strategies in order to reduce aggressive behaviour.
In recent years, many researchers have employed non-invasive brain stimulation techniques to alleviate the symptomatology of several psychiatric disorders, such as depression, anxiety, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and bipolar disorder [10,11,12,13,14]. In particular, some, but not all, of these techniques are valid therapeutic alternatives for treatment-refractory major depression [15]. Additionally, these techniques also present diagnostic applications in neurology, such as presurgical motor and language mapping, and they are a valid instrument to diagnose several neurological conditions. These tools generally apply changing magnetic fields or electric currents through the surface of the skull to the neurons in the cortex. These changing magnetic pulses and/or current flows interfere with the depolarization of a group of neurons, which in turn affects their synapsis with other neurons transmitting these alterations in neural circuits and/or the brain networks [13,16,17]. Hence, we cannot consider their effect to be only local, because their initial effect tends to transynaptically extend across brain networks. Moreover, it has been suggested that their effects are capable of inducing changes in cortical excitability that can be maintained for days, but it is less clear how long these effects last after this period [17]. Moreover, it should be highlighted that cognitive processes and behaviour are sustained by complex neural networks made up of cortical and subcortical structures [5]. Therefore, the application of changing magnetic pulses and/or electric currents over the brain’s cortex might partly modulate these processes.
The most common and well-known non-invasive brain stimulation techniques are repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS), transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), and cranial electrotherapy stimulation (CES). Although these techniques are based on the effects of applying changing magnetic fields or electric currents to interfere in cortical excitability, they differ in several aspects. Thus, their effects vary, for example, depending on the use of a coil or electrodes, the coil shape, the stimulation site, the pattern of stimulation, polarity, and current intensity, among others [13,14,15,16,17,18,19]. For example, in TMS devices, an electric pulse is sent through the coil, which generates a changing magnetic pulse. When this coil is placed over the head, this magnetic pulse travels across the skull. These magnetic pulses tend to interfere in the depolarization of a group of neurons. It can be applied as single or repetitive pulses (repetitive TMS or rTMS). The Theta-Burst Stimulation (TBS) represents a patterned form of rTMS. In fact, patients receive a series of short magnetic pulses (bursts) at a high frequency, which corresponds to theta brain oscillations. These bursts can be applied as continuous (cTBS) or intermittent protocols (iTBS). Regarding the tSMS, it is based on the principle of applying static magnetic fields over the scalp with a constant intensity and orientation, which tends to reduce cortical excitability. With regard to tDCS, it consists of the application of weak and constant direct current to the brain via electrodes placed on the head. These current flows modulate cortical excitability, which would lead to facilitation of neural excitation (anodal tDCS) or inhibition of neural activity (cathodal tDCS). Lastly, CES devices generate and send low-intensity alternating current electrical stimulation via electrodes that can be placed, for example, on the earlobes [13,14,15,16,17,18,19].
Violence in humans is a complex phenomenon with multiple explanatory causes. Many authors have identified two specific cortical structures, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the temporal lobe, as important for behavioural regulation. In this regard, violence proneness is commonly present after registering injuries (e.g., tumours, traumatic brain injuries, brain haemorrhages) in these cortical structures [3,4,5]. In fact, specific traumas in one or both of these brain structures might facilitate outbursts and violent reactions due to difficulties in inhibiting limbic irritability [3,4,5]. This is a well-known model to explain reactive violence or the violence guided by emotional insights after perceiving a potential threat. Nevertheless, there is another kind of violence, known as proactive, that is characterized by predatory unemotional attacks rather than emotional reactions. Because these two types of reactions are explained by different cognitive processes, it is logical to conclude that their underlying brain structures would differ. Proactive reactions tend to be related to an increased activation of the ventral striatal and the angular gyrus [5]. Nonetheless, it is not well-understood whether non-invasive brain stimulation techniques would be appropriate to reduce specific behavioural dysregulations, such as violence proneness, by interfering in current brain networks by applying magnetic pulses and/or current flows. Thus, it would be interesting to conduct a systematic review assessing whether these techniques, without controlling for coadjutant treatments, are appropriate for decreasing violence proneness.
With all this in mind, the principal aim of this systematic review was to answer the question of whether specific non-invasive stimulation techniques (i.e., rTMS, TBS, tSMS, tDCS, and CES) are valid strategies on their own to reduce anger, hostility, and/or irritability levels (e.g., state or trait), thus decreasing violence proneness. This would occur because violence is partly explained by alterations in anger-states, which are part of the violence facilitation system [20,21]. Therefore, our first aim was to analyse whether studies registered changes in several facets of human violence, such as feelings of anger and anger expression, after the application of these non-invasive brain stimulation techniques in several populations (normative, violent individuals, and patients with mental disorders/pathological conditions). Moreover, several variables would be considered, such as demographic variables (e.g., age, gender), the brain structures stimulated, and variables of the non-invasive brain stimulation techniques (e.g., intensity, allocation, number of sessions), as potential mediators in the effects of these techniques on violence proneness. Finally, considering the existing data so far, we propose a series of recommendations for the correct application of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques to reduce violence proneness. Moreover, the conclusions derived from this manuscript will help the scientific community, clinicians, and patients to learn about the available evidence on treatments for violence control and their advantages and disadvantages in order to make evidence-based choices.

2. Search Strategy

We followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) quality criteria for reviews to conduct this systematic review [5,6,22,23]. A literature search was performed through the following databases: PsycINFO, PubMed, Dialnet, Psicodoc, Web of Knowledge, and the Cochrane Library. Moreover, we also completed the previously mentioned process with hand-searching. All these processes were carried out from October to December of 2019. Regarding manuscript selection, we paid special attention to carefully choosing manuscripts with good methodological quality in order to increase the value of this systematic review. Nevertheless, some manuscripts were selected due to the lack of scientific literature in this specific field (e.g., inmates, pathological conditions).
The search strings considered relevant for this field of research and applied to the databases were: ((Transcranial magnetic stimulation) OR (transcranial direct current stimulation), OR (cranial electrotherapy stimulation) OR (theta burst stimulation), OR (transcranial static magnetic field stimulation)) AND ((rumination) OR (mood) OR (violence) OR (aggressive) OR (anger) OR (hostility)) OR (irritability) OR (inmate)).
All the papers selected for final inclusion met the following criteria: (a) they were empirical studies with humans; (b) they assessed the association of non-invasive brain stimulation technique application with trait or state anger, proneness to violence, and aggressive strategies in laboratory tasks; (c) there was no controlled concomitant psychotropic medication treatment or statistical control of their role during TMS treatment; (d) presence of a control group (e.g., sham-controlled, waiting list) and/or randomization of the sample; (e) they did not collapse non-invasive brain stimulation techniques without differentiating the effects of each; and (f) they were written in English. If we found a manuscript that did not offer much information about inclusion/exclusion criteria or participants’ characteristics, we decided to discard the manuscript. For example, several manuscripts included pathological conditions closely related to irritability (e.g., Alzheimer Disease, epilepsy, autism spectrum disorders), but we only included those that did not mix, or controlled for, psychotropic treatments with non-invasive brain stimulation techniques.
Article selection was carried out by two independent researchers. The level of interrater agreement between the two researchers was 95%. In cases of disagreement, we discussed these manuscripts in order to see whether they adhered to the inclusion criteria of our systematic review.

3. Results

We initially identified 3745 publications in PsycINFO, PubMed, Dialnet, Psicodoc, Web of Knowledge, and the Cochrane Library. Furthermore, we included an additional reference found by hand-searching that was not included in the previously mentioned databases. After assessing the existence of duplicated manuscripts, 1142 were removed, leaving 2604 for the screening of titles and abstracts. After that, the full text of 1090 articles were read, finally including 56 publications (Figure 1). The main characteristics of the participants and studies included in this review are summarized in Table 1 (e.g., participants’ characteristics, brain structures stimulated, main results of anger-state assessment, type of design).
Initially, due to the number of studies included with a normative population, we decided to divide them into two blocks. First, we summarized studies that analysed whether brain stimulation techniques produced anger state (measured by self-reports) improvements and/or changes. Second, manuscripts were presented that assessed how brain stimulation interferes with participants’ performance on laboratory tasks that assess violence. Nonetheless, it should be noted that certain studies were repeated in both blocks because they studied mood changes assessed by self-reports and laboratory task performance. Afterwards, we will present the results based on a study that applied these techniques in violent offenders—specifically, inmates. Moreover, we finish the results by presenting several studies with patients affected by autism spectrum disorder (ASD), unipolar depressed patients of the melancholic subtype, abstinent smokers, people with language disorders, closed-head-injury patients, Alzheimer’s, refractory partial epilepsy and Schizophrenia.

3.1. Normative Individuals

By including studies with normative participants, we increased the external validity of the conclusions derived from our study. In fact, this will reveal whether these techniques would be useful to promote mood changes not only in individuals with pathological conditions, but also in healthy individuals.

3.2. Self-Reports

Surprisingly, only three studies [28,53,78] out of 35 (0.9%) revealed significant reductions in anger state after the application of brain stimulation techniques. It is interesting to highlight that both studies mainly stimulated the prefrontal cortex (PFC), which has usually been associated with behavioural control [79]. Nevertheless, most of the studies that also stimulated the PFC failed to report significant anger-state or irritability improvements after the application of these techniques [24,25,26,27,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,48,49,50,51,52,54,57]. The rest of the studies focused the brain stimulation on the left temporo-parietal junction [36], vermis and cerebellar hemispheres [56], inferior frontal cortex [47], and earlobes [55], but they also failed to reduce anger. These studies seem quite unanimous in concluding that there are no effects on mood in normative populations. In order to understand these results, it is important to pay attention to the methodological aspects of the above-mentioned studies. For example, there were several brain stimulation techniques employed that stimulated different brain regions, with different intensities or frequencies of brain stimulation and patterns of application (e.g., single session, several sessions over several weeks, consecutive or alternating sessions). Furthermore, most of them included sham-controlled brain stimulation as a control condition. It is important to know that all of them were based on healthy young adults around 20 years old, which increases the reliability of the results.
Conversely, the two studies that revealed significant results presented important methodological weaknesses that should be mentioned. In this regard, Hoffman et al. [28] only employed 20 participants with a pre-post design without a placebo group (sham-controlled). Additionally, Choy et al. [53] assessed violence tendencies based on hypothetical scenarios that participants had to rate on a quantitative continuous scale (from 0 to 100). Although the sample size was relatively high in comparison with the previous study, the use of a test without providing internal consistency and reliability reduced our confidence in these results. The third study failed to obtain significant reductions in irritability on one of the self-reports included to assess irritability [29]. Finally, based on these limited sample sizes and the high number of statistical comparisons, it would be necessary to conduct Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons in both studies. However, the authors did not control or report this aspect. Therefore, we assumed that these techniques were not sufficient to promote anger state changes in normative healthy young adults.

3.3. Laboratory Task

With regard to violence proneness measured by laboratory tasks, most of the studies included revealed significant and relatively similar results. Furthermore, it is interesting to note that these studies measured violence proneness with a well-validated computer task, the Taylor Aggression Paradigm (TAP) [41,47,48,54,61,62,63,64]. On this task, participants compete with a virtual opponent, and they must respond to a provocation (fictitious character/opponent). In fact, they can respond and punish the opponent with an electric shock. This test makes it possible to manipulate the degree of provocation and assess the participants’ perception of provocation and their type of aggressive reaction (reactive or proactive). Finally, it is also important to keep in mind that all the studies included a sham-group as controls, and some of them were double-blinded [41,54,60,63].
Together, the studies paid attention to the effects of brain stimulation on the PFC in interfering with participants’ aggressive behaviour measured by the TAP. First, when participants received stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), only men experienced a reduction in proactive violence on the TAP [61]. Conversely, the stimulation of the left DLPFC increased reactive and proactive violence in both genders after provocation [59], but a study with women failed to obtain differences across conditions [65]. Likewise, another study revealed that complete stimulation of the left frontal cortex also increases aggressive reactions to provocation [41]. If we focus on other regions of the PFC, the results are consistent with those mentioned above. In other words, stimulation of the right PFC entails decreases in aggressive behaviour, whereas stimulation of the left PFC increases aggressive behaviour. Thus, after stimulation of the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), participants experienced a decrease in violent reactions to provocation [64], even after being exposed to violent videogames [24,27,37,46,47,48,52,55,56,57,58,61,62,63,64]. Nonetheless, stimulation of the left VLPFC increased aggression after provocation [54]. Furthermore, bilateral stimulation of the ventromedial PFC (VMPFC) produced a decrease in their aggressive reactions after provocation [63]. Moreover, stimulation of the right inferior frontal gyrus was also associated with less explicit violent reactions to provocations but showed a Machiavellian reaction to increase their chances of obtaining profits [60]. Lastly, contrary to these conclusions, only one study that bilaterally stimulated the inferior frontal cortex presented no effect on TAP performance [48].
The studies included in this block presented relatively homogeneous methodological characteristics (sample size, sample demographic characteristics, anger assessment, study design). This increased the reliability of the results but reduced the external validity of the conclusions. Moreover, it should be highlighted that most of the studies reported significant differences, but most of these studies divided the sample into small groups across conditions. Having a reduced sample size that was subsequently divided into additional groups increased the likelihood of a type I error. Thus, most of these significant differences were close to 0.05. Thus, if they did not correct for multiple comparisons, this reduced our confidence in these results. It should also be highlighted that these studies only paid attention to PFC, and neglected other cortical regions closely related to aggression proneness.

3.4. Violent Population

To extend the external validity and clinical applications of these results, it is necessary to include violent individuals. As explained above, we only included manuscripts that excluded participants with concomitant psychotropic medication or that statistically controlled their effects. Based on this criterion, we only included six manuscripts. First, we will describe the results of a manuscript that studied brain stimulation techniques with inmates. After that, we will provide information about five studies with patients with autism spectrum disorders (ASD).
A single study focused on the effects of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques on behavioural dysregulation in inmates [66]. It is important to keep in mind that this study included a sham-controlled group, but with a reduced sample size (less than 10 participants per group). The authors observed that inmates, especially the group of murderers, experienced a decrease in self-reported anger expression (physical and verbal) after bilateral anodal tDCS of the PFC. Despite these encouraging results, the group of murderers was too small (eight participants). Furthermore, it did not control for potential confounding variables. In fact, prison populations are commonly heterogeneous in their demographic characteristics, but the authors did not offer much information about each group in order to understand how these variables affected participants’ sensitivity to brain stimulation effects. Lastly, the authors commented that they included a non-prisoner group as controls, but they did not offer information about this group (sample size, characteristics, sham-controlled or not).

3.5. Pathological Conditions

Regarding studies with patients with ASD, all of them reported improvements in behavioural regulation (e.g., irritability, agitation, repetitive behavioural patterns) after bilateral stimulation of the DLPFC [67,68,69,70,71]. Even though these results are encouraging regarding the effects of rTMS in this population, these studies presented several methodological limitations that should be discussed. For example, the main limitation is the type of research design, without double-blinding and randomization of the sample. Another important limitation is the reduced sample size, with no more than 60 participants in each study. Furthermore, these studies included half of the ASD participants as a waiting-list control group, but this is not adequate as a control. It would be necessary to include a sham-controlled group, or even a third group of unaffected participants. Hence, this diminishes the confidence in these results and reinforces the need to conduct additional studies with robust research designs. There were also two studies that showed significant reductions in anger levels in two different populations, such as patients who had suffered a closed-head injury 6 months earlier [75] and Alzheimer patients [76]. Finally, other studies reported a lack of effects on treatment-resistant depressive patients with no pharmacological treatment [72], on anger-state in current-abstinent smokers [73], on patients with refractory partial epilepsy who received a dosage of antiepileptics [77], and on patients with schizophrenia [78]. Even a study with mentally retarded minors with language disorders reinforced the fact that non-invasive brain stimulation techniques produced an increase in irritability in approximately 40% of these patients [74]. Curiously, these studies presented more methodological strengths than the aforementioned studies with positive changes. Thus, this fact should be discussed properly in order to clarify these unknowns.

4. Discussion

To our knowledge, there is no other literature review that provides a detailed description of the effects of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques on anger management in several populations. We carefully selected studies with good methodological quality (e.g., double-blind, randomized, sham-controlled). Most of the studies were unanimous in concluding that non-invasive brain stimulation techniques on the PFC is not sufficient to promote mood alterations (e.g., anger-state) in euthymic (normative) populations of both genders. Nevertheless, the dominance of the right PFC over the left PFC reduced violent reactions in normative individuals of both genders by interfering in the interpretation of the unfavourable situations or inner states that evoked anger. In antisocial individuals and people with mental disorders, bilateral stimulation of the PFC satisfactorily reduced anger and irritability in inmates, patients with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), patients who had suffered a closed-head injury, and agitated patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Moreover, combining these techniques with risperidone considerably reduced aggressiveness in these patients. Unfortunately, in people with language disorders, people with treatment-resistant depression, smokers, and others with refractory partial epilepsy and schizophrenia, it did not promote anger-state alterations. Therefore, it is necessary to be cautious about the benefits of these techniques for controlling anger and irritability, and it is relevant to consider these strategies to reduce anger-state as coadjutant treatments to psychotherapy and psychopharmacology in order to promote lasting changes in violent populations.
The studies included in this review indicate that there is an asymmetric predominance of PFC functioning in eliciting anger states, lowering the threshold of their appearance when activation of the left PFC predominates over the right, specifically, stimulation of the left DLPFC and VLPFC. By contrast, the activation of the right DLPFC, VLPFC, and inferior frontal gyrus, as well as the bilateral VMPFC, tends to be associated with decreases in anger-oriented states and increased efforts to control anger expression [41,47,48,54,59,60,61,62,63,64]. Obviously, the relationship between the application of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques over the PFC and the reduction in anger or irritability is complex, as expected. Moreover, it is also important to keep in mind that the PFC is not only involved in anger induction [20]. Mood induction is sustained by several brain circuits, including cortical and subcortical structures [5]. Thus, it would be possible to interfere in the threshold of the appearance of violence by interfering in the interpretation of contextual or inner states, instead of modifying mood.
Regarding mood alterations, studies were unanimous in sustaining that, in euthymic (normative) individuals, we cannot conclude that non-invasive stimulation techniques might promote mood improvements, particularly decreases in anger, but it seemed relevant for murderers [60], young individuals with ASD [67,68,69,70,71], patients who had suffered a closed-head injury [75], and agitated patients with Alzheimer’s disease [76]. It is important to note that pathological populations might be sensitive to brain stimulation because their functioning is relatively different from the normative. For example, it has been demonstrated that antisocial individuals show increased left PFC predominance over the right PFC [80]. This hemispheric dominance might explain the frequent appearance of an approach-oriented emotion of anger in response to a minimal sign of threat or the maintenance of these emotional states as a characteristic of an individual (high anger trait). Therefore, it makes sense that the bilateral application of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques over the DLPFC might help to regulate hemispheric asymmetries in violent inmates or ASD patients. Nevertheless, it was not useful for individuals with language disorders, treatment-resistant depressive patients without antidepressants, and recently abstinent smokers. Thus, it is difficult to conclude that this technique was useful for all types of patients, without considering coadjutant pharmacological treatments. In fact, approximately half of the mentally retarded with language disorders presented an increase in irritability after several sessions of tDCS [74]. This is relevant because these participants were minors, which tends to be a sensitive population to pharmacological treatments [81].
With regard to the characteristics of the non-invasive brain stimulation techniques, for tDCS, the application of 1 to 2 mA for 15–30 min seems sufficient to promote the aforementioned alterations in the interpretation of hostile intentions of our opponents. For cTBS and rTMS, it is sufficient to apply 0.5 to 1 Hz for 30 min. Nevertheless, CES did not seem to promote these mood changes. This could be explained by its far location from the PFC. Fortunately, we also consider it important to highlight that none of the studies reported important side-effects, only unusual minimal reactions. Finally, because the effects of these stimulation techniques seem relatively short-lived, it would be advisable to sustain them with single weekly sessions spaced out over time. In this regard, there was no unanimity among the studies included in our review about whether a single session is sufficient to produce changes, or whether several sessions are required. Therefore, it is necessary for further studies to explore this key variable.
Some strengths of this systematic review should be highlighted. First, we considered several multidisciplinary scientific databases. Moreover, it is also important to acknowledge that we considered grey literature and clinical trials, which increased the likelihood of obtaining unpublished research. Second, it is extremely important to reinforce the fact that we established the methodological quality of the manuscripts as an inclusion criterion, thus excluding case studies or weak research designs. Finally, our conclusions were based on several studies with relatively homogeneous samples, which positively impacts the replicability of the results and reinforces the value of our conclusions.
This article also had a few limitations that are important to mention and consider in the interpretation of the results. Because we only searched for studies in specific databases, other databases were omitted that could have increased the range of articles found. Furthermore, there was an unbalanced number of manuscripts, predominantly including studies assessing anger and irritability in normative individuals, but only a few measured aggressive behaviours in violent individuals (e.g., people with mental disorders, inmates, offenders). However, studies assessing whether these techniques are good enough to promote changes had important methodological limitations. This reduced the reliability of our conclusions, especially in those analysing violent and pathological populations, thus reinforcing the need to conduct additional research in this field. Moreover, a large number of studies assessed anger with self-reports, which are subject to distortions such as social desirability and acquiescence biases. Additionally, criminal offenders and inmates might present a conflict of interest on questionnaire responses due to the obtaining of penitentiary benefits. Regarding TAP, although it is a well-known laboratory procedure, it only assesses aggressive behaviour in response to provocation. Therefore, it should be noted as a potential limitation. Furthermore, several manuscripts combined psychotropics with brain stimulation techniques, and so it is difficult to assess the effects of brain stimulation techniques alone. In fact, in these cases, it was very difficult to determine whether therapeutic effects were the result of each technique, or a combination of the two. Moreover, several studies did not offer information about whether patients had taken drugs, even though these patients presented with uncontrollable agitation and irritability. Because this sounded odd, we decided to remove them from our review. Nevertheless, we do not think this affected the conclusions of our study.

5. Conclusions

In summary, the present review demonstrated the importance of considering non-invasive brain stimulation techniques as potential tools to increase the threshold for becoming violent by interfering in the interpretation of threat signals. Moreover, it is important to highlight that we should consider the possibility of combining these treatments with other previously established treatments in order to increase their success. In any case, although these tools seem to be relevant strategies to reduce anger-state, we cannot ignore the study limitations and the need to consider it as a coadjutant treatment to psychotherapy in order to promote lasting changes in violent populations. Future studies should compare the effects of these tools combined with other pharmacological treatments (e.g., sertraline, impipramine, fluoxetine, risperidone) and serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, such as venlafaxine, which not only affect the sertraline system, but also the noradrenergic system in order to find out whether other drugs with potent effects on other neurotransmitter systems present greater benefits for anger control.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Á.R.-M.; methodology, Á.R.-M.; writing—original draft preparation, Á.R.-M.; writing—review and editing, Á.R.-M.; S.B. and L.M.-A.; supervision, S.B. and L.M.-A.; funding acquisition, Á.R.-M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Project supported by a 2018 Leonardo Grant for Researchers and Cultural Creators, BBVA Foundation. The Foundation accepts no responsibility for the opinions, statements, and contents included in the project and/or the results thereof, which are entirely the responsibility of the authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Glenn, A.L.; Raine, A. Neurocriminology: Implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. McGuire, J. A review of effective interventions for reducing aggression and violence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2008, 363, 2577–2597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Moya-Albiol, L.; Sariñana-González, P.; Vitoria-Estruch, S.; Romero-Martínez, Á. La neurocriminología como disciplina aplicada emergente. Vox Juris 2017, 33, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Brunner, F.; Neumann, I.; Yoon, D.; Rettenberger, M.; Stück, E.; Briken, P. Determinants of dropout from correctional offender treatment. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Romero-Martínez, Á.; González, M.; Lila, M.; Gracia, E.; Martí-Bonmatí, L.; Alberich-Bayarri, Á.; Maldonado-Puig, R.; Ten-Esteve, A.; Moya-Albiol, L. The brain resting-state functional connectivity underlying violence proneness: Is it a reliable marker for neurocriminology? A systematic review. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Humble, F.; Berk, M. Pharmacological management of aggression and violence. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 2003, 18, 423–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Cascade, E.; Kalali, A.H.; Kennedy, S.H. Real-world data on SSRI antidepressant side effects. Psychiatry 2009, 6, 16. [Google Scholar]
  8. Ferguson, J.M. SSRI antidepressant medications: Adverse effects and tolerability. Primary Care Companion J. Clin. Psychiatry 2001, 3, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Stamp, F. Psychiatric Treatment of Violent Offenders in Prison. Available online: https://aic.gov.au/sites/default/files/publications/proceedings/downloads/19-stamp.pdf (accessed on 28 January 2020).
  10. Chung, S.W.; Hoy, K.E.; Fitzgerald, P.B. Theta-burst stimulation: A new form of TMS treatment for depression? Depress. Anxiety 2015, 32, 182–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Cirillo, P.; Gold, A.K.; Nardi, A.E.; Ornelas, A.C.; Nierenberg, A.A.; Camprodon, J.; Kinrys, G. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in anxiety and trauma-related disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Behav. 2019, 9, e01284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cocchi, L.; Zalesky, A.; Nott, Z.; Whybird, G.; Fitzgerald, P.B.; Breakspear, M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A focus on network mechanisms and state dependence. NeuroImage Clin. 2018, 19, 661–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Kirsch, D.L.; Nichols, F. Cranial electrotherapy stimulation for treatment of anxiety, depression, and insomnia. Psychiatric Clin. 2013, 36, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Perera, T.; George, M.S.; Grammer, G.; Janicak, P.G.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Wirecki, T.S. The clinical TMS society consensus review and treatment recommendations for TMS therapy for major depressive disorder. Brain Stimul. 2016, 9, 336–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Gaynes, B.N.; Lloyd, S.W.; Lux, L.; Gartlehner, G.; Hansen, R.A.; Brode, S.; Jonas, D.E.; Swinson Evans, T.; Viswanathan, M.; Lohr, K.N. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin Psychiatry 2014, 75, 477–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Philip, N.S.; Nelson, B.G.; Frohlich, F.; Lim, K.O.; Widge, A.S.; Carpenter, L.L. Low-intensity transcranial current stimulation in psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 2017, 174, 628–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Hoogendam, J.M.; Ramakers, G.M.; Di Lazzaro, V. Physiology of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human brain. Brain Stimul. 2010, 3, 95–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Kirimoto, H.; Tamaki, H.; Otsuru, N.; Yamashiro, K.; Onishi, H.; Nojima, I.; Oliviero, A. Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation over the primary motor cortex induces plastic changes in cortical nociceptive processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Suppa, A.; Huang, Y.Z.; Funke, K.; Ridding, M.C.; Cheeran, B.; Di Lazzaro, V.; Ziemann, U.; Rothwell, J.C. Ten years of theta burst stimulation in humans: Established knowledge, unknowns and prospects. Brain Stimul. 2016, 9, 323–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Harmon-Jones, E.; Sigelman, J. State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression. J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 2001, 80, 797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Depue, R.A.; Iacono, W.G. Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1989, 40, 457–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Liberati, A.; Altman, D.G.; Tetzlaff, J.; Mulrow, C.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Ioannidis, J.P.; Clarke, M.; Devereaux, P.J.; Kleijnen, J.; Moher, D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Romero-Martínez, Á.; Hidalgo-Moreno, G.; Moya-Albiol, L. Neuropsychological consequences of chronic stress: The case of informal caregivers. Aging Ment. Health 2018, 1, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Schutter, D.J.; van Honk, J.; d’Alfonso, A.A.; Postma, A.; de Haan, E.H. Effects of slow rTMS at the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on EEG asymmetry and mood. Neuroreport 2001, 12, 445–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Grisaru, N.; Bruno, R.; Pridmore, S. Effect on the emotions of healthy individuals of slow repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation applied to the prefrontal cortex. J. ECT 2001, 17, 184–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Jenkins, J.; Shajahan, P.M.; Lappin, J.M.; Ebmeier, K.P. Right and left prefrontal transcranial magnetic stimulation at 1 Hz does not affect mood in healthy volunteers. BMC Psychiatry 2002, 2, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Schutter, D.J.; van Honk, J. Increased positive emotional memory after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the orbitofrontal cortex. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2006, 31, 101. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hofman, D.; Schutter, D.J. Inside the wire: Aggression and functional interhemispheric connectivity in the human brain. Psychophysiology 2009, 46, 1054–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Schaller, G.; Lenz, B.; Friedrich, K.; Dygon, D.; Richter-Schmidinger, T.; Jacobi, A.; Mueller, S.E.; Maihöfner, C.; Sperling, W.; Kornhuber, J. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation influences mood in healthy male volunteers. J. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 45, 1178–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Baeken, C.; Leyman, L.; De Raedt, R.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; D’haenen, H. Lack of impact of repetitive high frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on mood in healthy female subjects. J. Affect. Disord. 2006, 90, 63–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Baeken, C.; Leyman, L.; De Raedt, R.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; D’haenen, H. Left and right high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex does not affect mood in female volunteers. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008, 119, 568–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Koenigs, M.; Ukueberuwa, D.; Campion, P.; Grafman, J.; Wassermann, E. Bilateral frontal transcranial direct current stimulation: Failure to replicate classic findings in healthy subjects. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009, 120, 80–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Leyman, L.; De Raedt, R.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; Baeken, C. Influence of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on the inhibition of emotional information in healthy volunteers. Psychol. Med. 2009, 39, 1019–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Baeken, C.; De Raedt, R.; Van Schuerbeek, P.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; De Mey, J.; Bossuyt, A.; Luypaert, R. Right prefrontal HF-rTMS attenuates right amygdala processing of negatively valenced emotional stimuli in healthy females. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 214, 450–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Baeken, C.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; De Raedt, R. Baseline ‘state anxiety’influences HPA-axis sensitivity to one sham-controlled HF-rTMS session applied to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011, 36, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Baeken, C.; Schrijvers, D.L.; Sabbe, B.G.C.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; De Raedt, R. Impact of one HF-rTMS session on fine motor function in right-handed healthy female subjects: A comparison of stimulation over the left versus the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychobiology 2012, 65, 96–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Baumgartner, T.; Schiller, B.; Rieskamp, J.; Gianotti, L.R.; Knoch, D. Diminishing parochialism in intergroup conflict by disrupting the right temporo-parietal junction. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013, 9, 653–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Baeken, C.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; Remue, J.; Rossi, V.; Schiettecatte, J.; Anckaert, E.; De Raedt, R. One left dorsolateral prefrontal cortical HF-rTMS session attenuates HPA-system sensitivity to critical feedback in healthy females. Neuropsychologia 2014, 57, 112–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Moulier, V.; Gaudeau-Bosma, C.; Isaac, C.; Allard, A.C.; Bouaziz, N.; Sidhoumi, D.; Braha-Zeitoun, S.; Benadhira, R.; Thomas, F.; Januel, D. Effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on mood in healthy subjects. Socioaffective Neurosci. Psychol. 2016, 6, 29672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Iyer, M.B.; Mattu, U.; Grafman, J.; Lomarev, M.; Sato, S.; Wassermann, E.M. Safety and cognitive effect of frontal DC brain polarization in healthy individuals. Neurology 2005, 64, 872–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hortensius, R.; Schutter, D.J.; Harmon-Jones, E. When anger leads to aggression: Induction of relative left frontal cortical activity with transcranial direct current stimulation increases the anger–aggression relationship. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2011, 7, 342–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Plazier, M.; Joos, K.; Vanneste SOst, J.; De Ridder, D. Bifrontal and bioccipital transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) does not induce mood changes in healthy volunteers: A placebo-controlled study. Brain Stimul. 2012, 5, 454–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Motohashi, N.; Yamaguchi, M.; Fujii, T.; Kitahara, Y. Mood and cognitive function following repeated transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy volunteers: A preliminary report. Neurosci. Res. 2013, 77, 64–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kelley, N.J.; Hortensius, R.; Harmon-Jones, E. When anger leads to rumination: Induction of relative right frontal cortical activity with transcranial direct current stimulation increases anger-related rumination. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 475–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. McIntire, L.K.; McKinly, R.A.; Goodyear, C.; Nelson, J. A comparison of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation and caffeine on vigilance and cognitive performance during extended wakefulness. Brain Stimul. 2014, 7, 499–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Vitor-Costa, M.; Okuno, N.M.; Bortolotti, H.; Bertollo, M.; Boggio, P.S.; Fregni, F.; Altimari, L.R. Improving cycling performance: Transcranial direct current stimulation increases time to exhaustion in cycling. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0144916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Riva, P.; Romero-Lauro, L.J.; DeWall, C.N.; Chester, D.S.; Bushman, B.J. Reducing aggressive responses to social exclusion using transcranial direct current stimulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015, 10, 352–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Dambacher, F.; Schuhmann, T.; Lobbestael, J.; Arntz, A.; Brugman, S.; Sack, A.T. Reducing proactive aggression through non-invasive brain stimulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015, 10, 1303–1309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. De Putter, L.M.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; Baeken, C.; De Raedt, R.; Koster, E.H. Combining tDCS and working memory training to down regulate state rumination: A single-session double blind sham-controlled trial. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2015, 39, 754–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. De Raedt, R.; Remue, J.; Loeys, T.; Hooley, J.M.; Baeken, C. The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex on implicit self-esteem is mediated by rumination after criticism. Behav. Res. Ther. 2017, 99, 138–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. McIntire, L.K.; McKinley, R.A.; Nelson, J.M.; Goodyear, C. Transcranial direct current stimulation versus caffeine as a fatigue countermeasure. Brain Stimul. 2017, 10, 1070–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Vanderhasselt, M.A.; Sanchez, A.; Josephy, H.; Baeken, C.; Brunoni, A.R.; De Raedt, R. Anodal tDCS over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates cognitive processing of emotional information as a function of trait rumination in healthy volunteers. Biol. Psychol. 2017, 123, 111–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Choy, O.; Raine, A.; Hamilton, R.H. Stimulation of the prefrontal cortex reduces intentions to commit aggression: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, stratified, parallel-group trial. J. Neurosci. 2018, 38, 6505–6512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Gallucci, A.; Riva, P.; Lauro, L.J.R.; Bushman, B.J. Stimulating the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) modulates frustration-induced aggression: A tDCS experiment. Brain Stimul. 2020, 13, 302–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Valenzuela, P.L.; Amo, C.; Sánchez-Martínez, G.; Torrontegi, E.; Vázquez-Carrión, J.; Montalvo, Z.; Lucia, A.; de la Villa, P. Enhancement of Mood but not Performance in Elite Athletes with Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 2019, 14, 310–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Roh, H.T.; So, W.Y. Cranial electrotherapy stimulation affects mood state but not levels of peripheral neurotrophic factors or hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis regulation. Technol. Health Care 2017, 25, 403–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Demirtas-Tatlidede, A.; Freitas, C.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Schmahmann, J.D. Modulatory effects of theta burst stimulation on cerebellar nonsomatic functions. Cerebellum 2011, 10, 495–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Sheffield, A.; Ahn, S.; Alagapan, S.; Fröhlich, F. Modulating neural oscillations by transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A crossover, double-blind, sham-controlled pilot study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2019, 49, 250–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Perach-Barzilay, N.; Tauber, A.; Klein, E.; Chistyakov, A.; Ne’eman, R.; Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. Asymmetry in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and aggressive behavior: A continuous theta-burst magnetic stimulation study. Soc. Neurosci. 2013, 8, 178–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. De Dreu, C.K.; Kret, M.E.; Sligte, I.G. Modulating prefrontal control in humans reveals distinct pathways to competitive success and collective waste. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016, 11, 1236–1244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Dambacher, F.; Schuhmann, T.; Lobbestael, J.; Arntz, A.; Brugman, S.; Sack, A.T. No effects of bilateral tDCS over inferior frontal gyrus on response inhibition and aggression. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0132170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Riva, P.; Gabbiadini, A.; Lauro, L.J.R.; Andrighetto, L.; Volpato, C.; Bushman, B.J. Neuromodulation can reduce aggressive behavior elicited by violent video games. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2017, 17, 452–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Gilam, G.; Abend, R.; Gurevitch, G.; Erdman, A.; Baker, H.; Ben-Zion, Z.; Hendler, T. Attenuating anger and aggression with neuromodulation of the vmPFC: A simultaneous tDCS-fMRI study. Cortex 2018, 109, 156–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Chen, C.Y. Right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in proactive and reactive aggression: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. NeuroReport 2018, 29, 1509–1515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Dedoncker, J.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; Remue, J.; De Witte, S.; Wu, G.R.; Hooley, J.M.; De Raedt, R.; Baeken, C. Prefrontal TDCS attenuates medial prefrontal connectivity upon being criticized in individuals scoring high on perceived criticism. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019, 13, 1060–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Molero-Chamizo, A.; Riquel, R.M.; Moriana, J.A.; Nitsche, M.A.; Rivera-Urbina, G.N. Bilateral prefrontal cortex anodal tDCS effects on self-reported aggressiveness in imprisoned violent offenders. Neuroscience 2019, 397, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Baruth, J.M.; Casanova, M.F.; El-Baz, A.; Horrell, T.; Mathai, G.; Sears, L.; Sokhadze, E. Low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation modulates evoked-gamma frequency oscillations in autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurother. 2010, 14, 179–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Casanova, M.F.; Baruth, J.M.; El-Baz, A.; Tasman, A.; Sears, L.; Sokhadze, E. Repetitive transcanial magnetic stimulation (RTMS) modulates event-related potential (ERP) indices of attention in autism. Transl. Neurosci. 2012, 3, 170–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Casanova, M.F.; Hensley, M.K.; Sokhadze, E.M.; El-Baz, A.S.; Wang, Y.; Li, X.; Sears, L. Effects of weekly low-frequency rTMS on autonomic measures in children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Sokhadze, E.M.; El-Baz, A.S.; Sears, L.L.; Opris, I.; Casanova, M.F. rTMS neuromodulation improves electrocortical functional measures of information processing and behavioral responses in autism. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Wang, Y.; Hensley, M.K.; Tasman, A.; Sears, L.; Casanova, M.F.; Sokhadze, E.M. Heart rate variability and skin conductance during repetitive TMS course in children with autism. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2016, 41, 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Baeken, C.; De Raedt, R.; Leyman, L.; Schiettecatte, J.; Kaufman, L.; Poppe, K.; Vanderhasselt, M.A.; Anckaert, E.; Bossuyt, A. The impact of one HF-rTMS session on mood and salivary cortisol in treatment resistant unipolar melancholic depressed patients. J. Affect. Disord. 2009, 113, 100–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Xu, J.; Fregni, F.; Brody, A.L.; Rahman, A.S. Transcranial direct current stimulation reduces negative affect but not cigarette craving in overnight abstinent smokers. Front. Psychiatry 2013, 4, 112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Andrade, A.C.; Magnavita, G.M.; Allegro, J.V.B.N.; Neto, C.E.B.P.; Lucena, R.D.C.S.; Fregni, F. Feasibility of transcranial direct current stimulation use in children aged 5 to 12 years. J. Child. Neurol. 2014, 29, 1360–1365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Smith, R.B.; Tiberi, A.; Marshall, J. The use of cranial electrotherapy stimulation in the treatment of closed-head-injured patients. Brain Injury 1994, 8, 357–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Wu, Y.; Xu, W.; Liu, X.; Xu, Q.; Tang, L.; Wu, S. Adjunctive treatment with high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for the behavioral and psychological symptoms of patients with Alzheimer’s disease: A randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled study. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 2015, 27, 280. [Google Scholar]
  77. Sun, W.; Mao, W.; Meng, X.; Wang, D.; Qiao, L.; Tao, W.; Li, L.; Jia, X.; Han, C.; Fu, M.; et al. Low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of refractory partial epilepsy: A controlled clinical study. Epilepsia 2012, 53, 1782–1789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Hansbauer, M.; Wobrock, T.; Kunze, B.; Langguth, B.; Landgrebe, M.; Eichhammer, P.; Frank, E.; Cordes, J.; Wölwer, W.; Winterer, G.; et al. Efficacy of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on PANSS factors in schizophrenia with predominant negative symptoms–Results from an exploratory re-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 263, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Baxter, M.G.; Croxson, P.L. Behavioral control by the orbital prefrontal cortex: Reversal of fortune. Nat. Neurosci. 2013, 16, 984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Yang, Y.; Raine, A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2009, 174, 81–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Batchelor, H.K.; Marriott, J.F. Paediatric pharmacokinetics: Key considerations. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2015, 79, 395–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Flow chart of literature search with reasons for exclusion.
Figure 1. Flow chart of literature search with reasons for exclusion.
Jcm 09 00882 g001
Table 1. Main sociodemographic characteristics and details about the participants in each study, the main results, and the assessment methods used.
Table 1. Main sociodemographic characteristics and details about the participants in each study, the main results, and the assessment methods used.
AuthorsSample Age, Gender and HandednessBrain StructuresBrain StimulationResearch DesignMain Results (Anger)
Normative and healthy sample
Self-reports
Schutter et al., [24]1228.4 ± 8.90Right DLPFCrTMS
1 Hz 20 min
Single blind, sham-controlledSTAS, absence differences
67% men and 33% women Single session
Right-handed
Grisaru et al., [25]1840.5 ± 11.60Left and right PFCrTMS
1 Hz
Randomized, sham-controlledVAS, absence differences
39% men and 61% womenFour days
Right-handed
Jenkins et al., [26]1924.6 ± 5.30 Left and right DLPFCrTMS
1 Hz
Two sessions spaced 2 weeks
Pseudo-randomizationPOMS, absence differences
47% men and 53% women
-
Schutter et al., [27]12From 18 to 25Left OFCrTMS 1 Hz 20 minDouble-blind, sham-controlledPOMS and VAS, absence differences
50% men and 50% womenSingle session
Right-handed
Hofman et al., [28]2021.0 ± 1.81Right frontal cortexrTMS
0.18 ± 0.02 Hz
Single session
Pre-post designHigher left-to-right transcallosal inhibition associated higher AQ score
10% men and 90% women
Right-handed
Schaller et al., [29]3824.0 ± 2.77 Left DLPFCrTMSDouble-blind, sham-controlledBDI, decrease in irritability
100% men25 HzVAS, absence differences
Right-handed9 sessions/consec. days
Baeken et al., [30]2824.68 ± 5.85Left DLPFCrTMS
10 Hz 20 min
Single session
Single-blind, sham-controlledPOMS and VAS, absence differences
100% women
Right-handed
Baeken et al., [31]2725.2 ± 5.00Left DLPFCrTMS
10 Hz 20 min
Single session
Single-blind, sham-controlledPOMS and VAS, absence differences
100% women
Right-handed
Koenigs et al., [32]2125.6 ± 5.8Bilateral frontal cortextDCS anodal and cathodal
2.5 mA 35 min
Double-blind, sham-controlledPOMS, absence differences
57% men and 43% womenSingle session
-
Leyman et al., [33]1821.1 ± 1.45Left and right DLPFCrTMS
10 Hz
Two sessions, spaced 1 week
Single-blind, sham-controlledVAS, absence differences
100% women
Right-handed
Baeken et al., [34]2023.30 ± 2.94Left and right DLPFCrTMS
10 Hz 20 min
Single-blind, randomizedPOMS, absence differences
100% womenSingle session
-
Baeken et al., [35]2422.29 ± 2.58Right DLPFCrTMS
10 Hz 20 min
Single-blind, sham-controlledSelf-reported anger, absence differences
100% womenSingle session
Right-handed
Baeken et al., [36]3621.20 ± 1.44Right and left DLPFCrTMS
10 Hz 20 min
Single-blind, randomized, sham-controlledSelf-reported anger, absence differences
100% womenSingle session
Right-handed
Baumgartner et al., [37]3624.3 ± 4.2Left temporo-parietal junctionrTMS
1 Hz 20 min
Single session
Randomized, sham-controlledSelf-reported anger
100% men
Right-handed
Baeken et al., [38]301.53 ± 2.85Left DLPFCrTMS
20 Hz 20 min
Single session
Single-blind, sham-controlledSelf-reported anger, not mediate changes
100% women
Right-handed
Moulier et al., [39]2033.7 ± 12.2Left DLPFCrTMS 10 Hz
10 sessions/15 min/2 weeks
Double blind, Sham-controlledVAS, absence differences
60% men and 40% women
Right-handed
Iyer et al., [40]10337.5 ± 12.9Left PFCtDCS anodal and cathodal
1–2 mA 20 min
Single-blind, sham-controlledVAS, absence differences
46% men and 54% womenSingle session
Right-handed
Hortensius et al., [41]80-Frontal cortextDCS
2 mA 15 min
Single session
Double blind, randomized, sham-controlledSelf-reported anger, absence differences
50% men and 50% women Right-handed
Plazier et al., [42]1721.47 ± 0.91Right (anodal) and left (cathodal) DLPFC and occipitaltDCS
1.5 mA 20 min
Double blind, randomized, sham-controlledSelf-reported anger, absence differences
100% menSingle session
-
Motohashi et al., [43]1222 ± 2.2Left DLPFCtDCS
1 mA 4-daily 20 min
Single-blind, sham-controlledPOMS, absence differences
100% menFour days
83% right-handed
Kelley et al., [44]90-Left and right PFCtDCS
2 mA 15 min
Single session
Double-blind, sham-controlled Self-reported anger, absence differences
33% men and 67% women
Right-handed
McIntire et al., [45]3029.3 ± 3.4DLPFC tDCS (anodal) + caffeine
2 mA 30 min
Randomized, sham-controlledPOMS and VAS, absence differences
73% men and 26% womenSingle session
Right-handed
Vitor-Costa et al., [46]1126 ± 4Primary motor cortextDCS
2 mA 30 min
Three days, spaced 48 h
Single-blind, sham-controlledSelf-reported anger, not mediate changes
100% men
-
Riva et al., [47]8023.06 ± 4.36Right VLPFCtDCS
1.5 mA 20 min
Single session
Randomized, sham-controlledSTAS, absence differences
21% men and 79% women
-
Dambacher et al., [48]6421.89 ± 3.26Inferior frontal cortextDCS
1–2 mA 21.75 min
Randomized, sham-controlledRPQ, absence differences
61% men and 39% womenSingle session
-
De Putter et al., [49]6623.09 ± 5.03DLPFCtDCS
2 mA 25 min
Double blind, Sham-controlled POMS, absence differences
20% men and 80% womenSingle session
-
De Raedt et al., [50]3222.6 ± 2.3DLPFCtDCS (anodal)
1.5 mA 20 min
Single-blind, sham-controlledSTAS, absence differences
100% womenSingle session
Right-handed
McIntire et al., [51]5027 ± 5Left (anodal) and right (cathodal) DLPFCtDCS + caffeine
2 mA
Random, sham-controlledPOMS, absence differences
72% men and 28% women36 h
-
Vanderhasselt et al., [52]3523.40 ± 4.43Right DLPFCtDCS (anodal)
2 mA 20 min
Single-blind, sham-controlledVAS, absence differences
31% men and 69% womenSingle session
Right-handed
Choy et al., [53]8120 years Bilateral DLPFCtDCS (anodal)
2 mA 20 min
Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Stratified, Parallel-Group TrialIncreases activation PFC less desire to commit physical and sexual assault (hypothetical vignettes/scenarios)
44% men and 56% womenTwo sessions
-
Gallucci et al., [54]9022.27 ± 2.46VLPFCtDCS (anodal)
1.5 mA 20 min
Double blind, randomized placebo-controlled design; sham-controlledSTAS, absence differences
50% men and 50% womenSingle session
-
Valenzuela et al., [55]827 ± 2Left primary motor cortextDCS (anodal)
2 mA 20 min
Double-blind, cross-over, sham-controlledBMS, absence differences
100% malesSingle session
-
Roh et al., [56]5054.8 ± 2.8 Earlobes of patientsCES
0.5 Hz 20 min
3 times/week; 8 weeks
Cross-over, sham-controlledPOMS, absence differences
100% women
-
Demirtas-Tatlidede et al., [57]1228.8 ± 9.94Vermis and cerebellar hemispheresiTBS
10 burst/session
Three sessions
RandomizedPOMS, absence differences
50% men and 50% women
Right-handed
Sheffield et al., [58]2426.54 ± 12.28Left or right PFC (frontal alpha asymmetry)tSMS
Single session
Double-blind, sham-controlledAQ, absence relationship cortical changes and AQ score
54% men and 46% women
Right-handed
Laboratory tasks
Perach-Barzilay et al., [59]1628 ± 4.68Left DLPFCcTBS
5 Hz/50 bursts
Single session
Randomized placebo-controlled design; sham-controlledSOP, stimulation left DLPFC increased reactive and proactive aggression
88% men and 12% women Right-handed
De Dreu et al., [60]1825.16 ± 2.00Right inferior frontal gyrusTBS
Three sessions
Double-blind, sham-controlled TAP, High activation entailed less aggression
100% men
-
Hortensius et al., [41]80-Frontal cortextDCS
2 mA 15 min
Single session
Double blind, randomized placebo-controlled design; sham-controlledTAP, left frontal activity entailed high aggression after provocation
50% men and 50% women Right-handed
Riva et al., [47]8023.06 ± 4.36Right VLPFCtDCS
1.5 mA 20 min
Single session
Randomized, sham-controlledTAP, Anodal stimulation right VLPFC entailed less aggression in socially excluded participants after videogame exposure
21% men and 79% women
-
Dambacher et al., [48]6421.89 ± 3.26Bilateral inferior frontal cortextDCS
1.5 mA 21.75 min
Single session
Randomized, sham-controlledTAP, absence differences
61% men and 39% women
-
Dambacher et al., [61]4322.14 ± 2.00Right DLPFCtDCS
2 mA (20 phases) 750 s
Randomized placebo-controlled design sham-controlledTAP, right hemispheric dominance reduced proactive aggression in men
47% men and 53% womenSingle session
-
Riva et al., [62]7921.73 ± 2.38 Right VLPFCtDCS (anodal)
1.5 mA 20 min
Randomized placebo-controlled design; sham-controlledTAP, Lower levels of aggressive behaviour
52% men and 48% womenSingle session
-
Gilam et al., [63]2526.16 ± 3.63Bilateral VMPFCtDCS (anodal)Double-blind, sham-controlledIncreased activation entailed less self-reported anger after provocation
40% men and 60% women1.2 mA 22 min
Two sessions
-
Chen et al., [64]3220–22 years Right VLPFCtDCS
2 mA 20 s
Randomized, sham-controlledTAP, Reduction in proactive and reactive aggression
50% men and 50% womenSingle session
-
Gallucci et al., [54]9022.27 ± 2.46Left VLPFCtDCS (anodal)
1.5 mA 20 min
Double-blind, randomized sham-controlledLeft VLPFC increased aggression.
50% men and 50% womenSingle sessionMales were more aggressive than females
-
Dedoncker et al., [65]4122.9 ± 2.61Left DLPFCtDCS (anodal)
1.5 mA 20 min
Randomized sham-controlledVAS, absence changes
100% femalesSingle session
Right-handed
Violent individuals (inmates)
Molero-Chamizo et al., [66]4136.2 ± 12.3Bilateral PFCtDCS (anodal)Single-blind, sham-controlledAQ, murders experienced reductions in the physical and verbal aggression
100% men1.5 mA 15 min
-3 sessions/consec. days
Pathological conditions
Autism spectrum disorders
Baruth et al., [67]2513.9 ± 5.3Bilateral DLPFCrTMSRandomized-controlled (waiting list)ABC, reductions in irritability
84% men and 16% women1 Hz
-12 sessions 30 min
12 sessions/weeks
Casanova et al., [68]4513.0 ± 2.7Bilateral DLPFCrTMSRandomized-controlled (waiting list)ABC, reductions in irritability
87% men and 13% women1 Hz 30 min
-12 sessions/weeks
Casanova et al., [69]1813.1 ± 2.2Bilateral DLPFCrTMSPre-post designABC, reductions in irritability
78% men and 12% women0.5 Hz 30 min
-18 weeks/sessions
Sokhadze et al., [70]5414.5 ± 2.9Bilateral DLPFCrTMSRandomized-controlled (waiting list)ABC, reductions in irritability
81% men and 19% women1 Hz 30 min
-18 weeks/sessions
Wang et al., [71]3312.9 ± 3.8Bilateral DLPFCrTMSPre-post designABC, reductions in irritability
84% men and 16% women0.5 Hz 30 min
-12 weeks/sessions
Unipolar depressed patients of the melancholic subtype (free drugs)
Baeken et al., [72]2044.3 ± 10.6Left DLPFCrTMS activeSingle-blind, sham-controlledPOMS, absence differences
35% men and 65% female10 Hz 20 min
Right-handedSingle session
Abstinent smokers
Xu et al., [73]2445 ± 7.6Left DLPFC (anodal) and right supraorbital area (cathodal)tDCS (anodal)
2 mA 20 min
Single-blind, sham-controlledPOMS, absence differences
87% men and 13% womenTwo sessions
-
Language disorders
Andrade et al., [74]14From 5 to 12Anode (Broca area (mid-left inferior frontal gyrus) and cathode right supraorbital area.tDCSPre-post design35.7% increased irritability: severe (14.3%), moderate (14.3%) and mild (7.1%).
71% men and 29% women10 sessions, 2 days interval
-
Closed-head injury
Smith et al., [75]21-Earlobes of patientsCESDouble-blind, sham-controlledPOMS, reductions anger
100 Hz
4 days/week for 3 weeks
Alzheimer
Wu et al., [76]52From 70 to 80Left DLPFCrTMS + low dose risperidoneDouble-blind, sham-controlledBEHAVE-AD, reductions aggressiveness
40% men and 60% women20 Hz
-5 sessions/week for 4 weeks
Refractory partial epilepsy
Sun et al., [77]6021 years (average)EpileptogenicrTMS + antiepileptic drugs (unchanged dose)Single-blind, sham-controlledSCL-90-R, absence changes
68% men and 32% womenfocus0.5 Hz
- Daily for 3 weeks
Schizophrenia
Hansbauer et al., [78]14636 years (average)Left DLPFCrTMSDouble-blind, sham-controlledPANSS, absence changes
75% men and 25% women10 Hz
82% righ-handed5 sessions/week for 3 weeks
Prefrontal cortex (PFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), cranial electrotherapy stimulation (CES), theta burst stimulation (TBS), intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS), transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS), State-Trait Anger Scale (STAS), Profile of Mood States (POMS), Visual Analogue Scale (VAS), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Buss–Perry Aggression Questionnaire (AQ), Reactive–Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ), Brunel Mood Scale (BMS), Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC), Taylor Aggression Paradigm (TAP), Social Orientation Paradigm (SOP), Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease Rating Scale (BEHAVE-AD), Symptom Checklist-90-R (SCL-90-R), Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Romero-Martínez, Á.; Bressanutti, S.; Moya-Albiol, L. A Systematic Review of the Effectiveness of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation Techniques to Reduce Violence Proneness by Interfering in Anger and Irritability. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 882. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9030882

AMA Style

Romero-Martínez Á, Bressanutti S, Moya-Albiol L. A Systematic Review of the Effectiveness of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation Techniques to Reduce Violence Proneness by Interfering in Anger and Irritability. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2020; 9(3):882. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9030882

Chicago/Turabian Style

Romero-Martínez, Ángel, Sara Bressanutti, and Luis Moya-Albiol. 2020. "A Systematic Review of the Effectiveness of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation Techniques to Reduce Violence Proneness by Interfering in Anger and Irritability" Journal of Clinical Medicine 9, no. 3: 882. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9030882

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop