Next Article in Journal
Effect of Bifidobacterium bifidum G9-1 on the Intestinal Environment and Diarrhea-Predominant Irritable Bowel Syndrome (IBS-D)-like Symptoms in Patients with Quiescent Crohn’s Disease: A Prospective Pilot Study
Previous Article in Journal
COPD Exacerbation: Why It Is Important to Avoid ICU Admission
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Differences in Selective Attention and Inhibitory Control in Patients with Major Depressive Disorder and Healthy Controls Who Do Not Engage in Sufficient Physical Activity

1
Department for Sport, Exercise and Health, University of Basel, 4052 Basel, Switzerland
2
Psychiatric Clinic Sonnenhalde, 4125 Riehen, Switzerland
3
Adult Psychiatric Clinics (UPKE), University of Basel, 4002 Basel, Switzerland
4
Sleep Disorders Research Center, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah 6719851115, Iran
5
Substance Abuse Prevention Research Center, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah 6715847141, Iran
6
School of Medicine, Tehran University of Medical Sciences (TUMS), Tehran 1419733141, Iran
7
Department of Intervention Research in Exercise Training, German Sport University Cologne, 50933 Cologne, Germany
8
Psychiatric Services Solothurn, University of Basel, 4503 Solothurn, Switzerland
9
Private Clinic Wyss, 3053 Münchenbuchsee, Switzerland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12(10), 3370; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12103370
Submission received: 3 April 2023 / Revised: 3 May 2023 / Accepted: 5 May 2023 / Published: 9 May 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Mental Health)

Abstract

:
Background: Patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) are characterized by neurocognitive impairments and show deficits in various cognitive performance indicators, including executive function. We examined whether sustained attention and inhibitory control differ between patients with MDD and healthy controls, and whether differences exist between patients with mild, moderate, and severe depression. Methods: Clinical in-patients (N = 212) aged 18–65 years with a current diagnosis of MDD and 128 healthy controls were recruited. Depression severity was assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory, and sustained attention and inhibitory control were assessed using the oddball and flanker tasks. The use of these tasks promises insights into executive function in depressive patients that are not biased by verbal skills. Group differences were tested via analyses of covariance. Results: Patients with MDD showed slower reaction times in both the oddball and flanker task, independent of the executive demands of the trial types. Younger participants achieved shorter reaction times in both inhibitory control tasks. After correcting for age, education, smoking, BMI, and nationality, only differences in reaction times in the oddball task were statistically significant. In contrast, reaction times were not sensitive to the symptom severity of depression. Conclusion: Our results corroborate deficits in basic information processing and specific impairments in higher-order cognitive processes in MDD patients. As difficulties in executive function underlie problems in planning, initiating, and completing goal-directed activities, they may jeopardize in-patient treatment and contribute to the recurrent nature of depression.

1. Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the most prevalent mental disorders worldwide [1]. In 2017, more than 320 million people were affected [2]. Estimates of lifetime prevalence range between 2 and 30%, dependent on geographic region [3,4,5,6]. During the COVID-19 pandemic, increases in prevalence and disease burden were observed across the world [7]. MDD is associated with high societal costs due to direct health care costs [8], as well as lost work productivity [9]. MDD is often recurrent and relapse is a frequent phenomenon [10]; almost 50% of patients with a first episode of MDD relapse within 2 years after the first episode, and almost 80% relapse during their lifetime [11]. As a consequence, depression often takes a chronic course [12]. MDD contributes to poor quality of life and social adjustment [13,14] and is seen as a leading cause of disability [15]. MDD is highly comorbid with other physical diseases [16] and strongly contributes to the number of years lived with severe impairment [17]. Ultimately, MDD leads to a marked reduction in life expectancy [18].
Nevertheless, physical impairments are not the only way in which depression impacts patients’ lives [19]. Cognitive deficits are also important to understand and explain the impairment of patients with MDD and are likely to be involved in the etiology of the disorder [20]. Depression in mild cognitive impairment is also associated with progression to Alzheimer’s disease [21,22,23]. Difficulties with concentration and decision making have been identified as diagnostic criteria of MDD [24], but deficits seem to extend beyond these core symptoms [25]. Meanwhile, there is solid evidence that patients with MDD are characterized by neurocognitive impairments and show deficits in various cognitive performance indicators, such as attention, memory, problem solving, and processing of emotional information [26,27]. Some scholars have suggested that MDD is associated with a general depletion in cognitive resources [28], whereas others have argued that patients with MDD have sufficient resources but have difficulties with resource allocation and/or the initiation of efficient cognitive strategies [29,30]. Cognitive impairments have been observed in both bipolar [31] and unipolar depression [32].
One form of how cognitive impairments can manifest is in executive function deficits. Executive functions represent a multi-dimensional construct that comprises a set of different (domain-general) cognitive processes relevant for governing goal-directed behavior, including the ability to formulate goals, plan and perform effectively, sustain attention, and regulate emotions [33,34]. Executive functions are especially relevant in response to non-routine situations and are involved in the regulation of various non-executive cognitive processes (e.g., perception, motor responses). Based on the framework provided by Miyake and Friedman [35], inhibitory control is considered a common component of executive function. Inhibitory control encompasses the ability to selectively attend and resist distracting information, thus building on top-down attentional processes [34]. The current body of evidence suggests that selective and sustained attention are both impaired in MDD, which affects behavior in daily life and contributes to a negative attentional bias [36]. The latter describes the tendency of depressed individuals to focus on negative stimuli, and this tendency has a predictive value for the development of depression severity [37]. Executive functions are strongly regulated by prefrontal cortex activity [38]. Thus, executive function deficits have been linked to lesions in the frontal brain regions [39]. These processes are often assessed with laboratory cognitive or neuropsychological tasks [40].
In a recent systematic review, deficits in people with depression were observed in executive subcomponents in 25 of 28 studies [39]. For several reasons, this finding is critical: First, in depressed patients, deficits in executive functions have negative effects on other life domains, including occupational performance, relationships with family members, and life satisfaction [41,42]. Second, empirical evidence suggests that although symptom recovery is paralleled with cognitive improvement, cognitive and executive function deficits can persist even after MDD patients are in remission [43,44]. Third, depressed patients with poor executive dysfunction also show slower and poorer responses to antidepressant treatment [45] and are more vulnerable to relapse [46,47]. Accordingly, executive functions have been proposed as an important target for therapy [25].
In the present study, new data are presented on the relationship between depression and the inhibitory aspects of executive function. The analyses are based on baseline data of a randomized controlled trial which aimed at investigating the effects of a 12-month individually tailored physical activity counseling program on physical activity behavior, fitness, depressive symptom severity, psychological functioning (including anxiety, stress, quality of life, and cognitive function), and cardiovascular risk in a relatively large sample of in-patients with MDD and healthy (non-depressed) controls [48]. As markers of cognitive function, two objective computer-based tests were carried out to assess sustained attention and inhibitory control (oddball and flanker paradigm). The inclusion of these variables was considered important for several reasons. First, previous studies showed that physical activity and cognitive function are interrelated in reciprocal ways [49,50], and executive function can improve as a result of participation in physical activity and exercise training [51,52,53]. However, while these associations are well documented in non-clinical samples, research with psychiatric samples is scarce [54,55,56]. Second, previous studies have shown that self-regulatory control can have an important impact on adherence to physical activity and exercise programs [49,50]. Accordingly, previous studies showed that specific networks and brain areas that are closely involved in inhibitory control are linked to exercise adherence [57,58,59,60]. Therefore, it has been argued that neuropsychological and neurophysiological tests may help predict who is more or less likely to adhere to an intervention, which in turn can help practitioners provide alternative and individualized interventions to individuals with poor expected adherence [61]. However, little is known so far whether such tests have predictive power in psychiatric patients, although it is well known that these patients might experiences difficulties in self-regulatory control [62] and that regular engagement in physical activity is essential to obtain the full benefits associated with a physically active lifestyle [63,64,65]. Third, we decided to focus on the inhibitory aspects of executive function because some authors argued that deficits in executive function in depressed people may be secondary to a primary deficit in attention [66]. This assumption is due to the notion that basic cognitive processes (such as adequate attention) are needed for proper functioning of executive processes. Previous meta-analyses in patients with depression also showed that the strongest deficits appear in tasks that require inhibitory control [67,68].
In the present paper, two research questions were addressed: First, we wanted to find out whether sustained attention and inhibitory control differ in patients with MDD and healthy (non-depressed) controls. Because insufficient physical activity was an inclusion criterion for depressed patients (<150 min of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity per week), healthy controls also had to be insufficiently physically active. Second, we wanted to find out whether sustained attention and inhibitory control differ within our patient sample based on their depressive symptom severity (mild, moderate, severe).
Our study adds to the current literature in several ways: First, existing evidence is based on relatively small samples with typically 20–50 patients [19,39]. Second, few studies have used socio-demographically matched controls to rule out testing effects or other sources of bias [69,70]. Third, different parameters were used to assess executive function in previous research [39]. When measuring inhibitory control, previous investigations have heavily relied on the Stroop Color–Word test [67], despite the criticism that performance on this task is influenced by reading skills [71]. The use of the oddball and flanker tasks promises insights into executive function and attentional processes in depressive patients that are not biased by verbal skills.
Based on the previous literature, three hypotheses were formulated. Firstly (and most generally), we expected poorer performances in sustained attention and inhibitory control in depressed patients compared to healthy controls (Hypothesis 1) [19,39]. Secondly (and more specifically), researchers have suggested two major mechanisms to explain cognitive deficits in MDD, namely the information processing hypothesis and the cognitive effort hypothesis [19,72]. The information processing hypothesis assumes that information processing is slowed down among depressed people, which interferes with higher cognitive function [73,74]. In contrast, the cognitive effort hypothesis suggests that executive dysfunction among depressed patients is not only due to a generalized slowing of information, but rather it also reflects a specific executive deficit. Here, a distinction is made between the execution of automatic tasks that do not require attention and tasks that demand more attentional resources for successful completion [75,76]. As shown in a recent systematic review of 37 studies, both hypotheses are supported by empirical data, showing that compared to healthy controls, both psychomotor slowing and specific executive function deficits are observed in depressed patients [19]. Based on this knowledge, our second hypothesis was that differences will not only appear in indicators that require limited inhibitory control (responses to frequent and congruent stimuli) but also manifest in more non-automatic, effortful tasks (responses to rare and incongruent stimuli) (Hypothesis 2). Thirdly, it has been hypothesized that executive function may vary according to the level of depressive symptom severity [77]. In line with this notion, we expected stronger executive function impairments in sustained attention and inhibitory control among patients with stronger symptom severity [78,79] (Hypothesis 3).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design

In the present paper, we present baseline data from a multi-centric, two-arm randomized controlled trial (RCT) including an intervention group (extended personalized physical activity and exercise counseling program) and a placebo control group (general instructions about health-enhancing physical activity), allocation concealment, single blinding, and intention-to-treat analysis. The study is a cooperation between four Swiss psychiatric clinics (two public, two private) and the Department of Sport, Exercise and Health of the University of Basel [48].

2.2. Participants

Clinical in-patients aged 18–65 years with a current diagnosis of major depression (ICD-10) were recruited between June 2019 and October 2021. For patient screening, a structured clinical interview was carried out by a psychiatrist to ensure that all participants fulfilled the ICD-10 diagnosis for a single episode (F32), recurrent MDD (F33), or bipolar disorder type II (F31-II). The duration of the current depressive episode, number of previous depressive episodes, and psychiatric and somatic comorbidities were also assessed during the process. Symptom severity was assessed using the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD17) [80] by a trained staff member, while patients completed the 21-item Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [81]. Level of physical activity during the last week before entering the clinic was measured with the short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) [82]. The inclusion criteria for patients were as follows: (a) women and men, 18–65 years of age; (b) presence of MDD according to the ICD-10 diagnostic criteria; (c) BDI-II ≥ 17 (at least borderline clinical depression); (d) currently not meeting the ACSM physical activity recommendations (IPAQ < 150 min/week of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity); (e) written informed consent; and (f) ability to speak and read German. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) presence of history of bipolar disorder type I (F31 type I), history of schizophrenia, or history of schizoaffective disorder; (b) current active alcohol or drug abuse or dependence; (c) any significant medical condition that contraindicates safe participation in physical activity; (d) active suicidal intent; € evidence of significant cardiovascular, neuromuscular, or endocrine disorders limiting regular physical activity, or medical contraindications to physical activity indicated by the Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q) [83]; and (f) inability to speak and read German.
To compare baseline differences in the primary and secondary outcomes between patients and healthy controls, we attempted to achieve an age- and gender-matched sample of 167 participants through advertisements in newspapers and through word-of-mouth recommendation. The inclusion criteria for healthy controls were as follows: (a) women and men; (b) 18–65 years of age; (c) HAMD17 ≤ 7; (d) BDI-II ≤ 13; (e) currently not meeting the ACSM physical activity recommendations (IPAQ < 150 min/week of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity); (f) written informed consent; and (g) ability to speak and read German.
In total, we were able to recruit 244 patients and 151 healthy controls who were willing to take part in the study. In total, 212 patients and 128 healthy controls fulfilled all inclusion criteria and had valid data for the oddball and flanker tasks at baseline. The initial power calculation was conducted in light of the expected effect on the primary outcome of the RCT [48]. However, a posteriori power analysis (using G*Power 3.1) showed that our sample was sufficiently powered to detect a small-to-medium effect. Thus, for an ANCOVA with fixed effects, 2 groups, 5 covariates, alpha error = 0.05, and power = 0.80, our sample size allowed us to detect an effect of f = 0.153.

2.3. Ethical Considerations

Ethical approval was obtained from the “Ethikkommission Nordwest- und Zentralschweiz” (ref approval no. 2018-00976) and all procedures were in line with the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The intervention study was registered in the WHO trial register (trial number: ISRCTN10469580). Participants were informed about the general goals of the study, and informed written consent was collected before study entry. Participants were informed that the participation in the study was voluntary and that withdrawal or discontinuation was possible at any time.

2.4. Data Assessment and Measures

Screening of patients took place in the first week after admission to in-patient treatment, and baseline data assessment was carried out after 2–3 weeks after admission. Screening and data assessment was combined in the healthy controls and took place at two of the four clinics. Data assessment procedures were identical for the patients and healthy controls.

2.4.1. Depression Severity

Self-reported depression severity was measured using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [81]. The BDI-II is a 21-item tool, frequently used to assess symptoms of unipolar depression, such as affective, behavioral, cognitive, and somatic symptoms (e.g., “I am so unhappy/sad that I can’t stand it”). Responses were given on a 4-point scale (from 0 to 3). Total BDI-II scores range from 0 to 63, with higher scores reflecting increasing levels of depressive symptomatology. Evidence for the reliability and validity of this measure has been reported previously [84]. Three levels of depression severity were differentiated: mild depression (BDI-II = 0–19); moderate depression (BDI-II = 20–28); and severe depression (BDI-II = 29–63) [85]. Internal consistency of the BDI-II in the present sample was satisfactory (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.94).

2.4.2. Sustained Attention and Inhibitory Control

With regard to cognitive function, we administered computer-based versions of the oddball and flanker tasks with E-Prime 3.0 (PST, USA) to assess inhibitory control. Both tasks are reliable, well-recognized neuropsychological tests for assessing executive function and selective attention in particular [34,86,87,88]. According to the study protocol [48], we also intended to use the 2-back test to assess working memory. However, initial assessments showed that participants had difficulties understanding and focusing on the task. Due to time constraints and to prevent dropouts, we removed the 2-back test from the test battery. This did not affect performance in the other tasks, given that the 2-back test was always applied last in the testing sequence.
The oddball paradigm [89] required participants to press different buttons to deviants (70%) and targets (30%). The letters “X” and “O” were used as visual stimuli and their allocation to the trial type was counterbalanced across the test blocks. Following an inter-trial interval that varied randomly between 800 and 1500 ms, visual stimuli were presented for 250 ms. Responses were collected within 1050 ms following stimulus onset.
For the flanker task [90], participants were instructed to respond to the direction of a centrally presented arrow and to ignore the flanking arrows, which either pointed in the same (congruent trials) or opposite direction (incongruent trials). Both trial types were equiprobable and followed a randomized presentation order. After an inter-trial interval with a random variation between 800 and 1300 ms, visual stimuli were shown for 350 ms, and responses were allowed within 1600 ms. In both the oddball and flanker task, participants completed a practice block and two subsequent test blocks with forty trials each [34,91]. Separately, for each task and trial type, reaction time (on response-correct trials) and accuracy were extracted for statistical analyses. Additionally, the interference score was calculated in the flanker task by subtracting reaction time on congruent trials from the reaction time of incongruent trials. To be included in the data analyses, participants had to achieve accuracy rates, in both tasks, higher than 50%.

2.4.3. Covariates

Participants were asked to report their sex, age, language, nationality, marital status, level of education, employment (rate) prior to hospitalization, years of job experience, smoking status, and whether they had children living at home. Additionally, information about the duration of the current depressive episode, number of prior depressive episodes, age of onset of depression, and current medication was assessed via clinical interview in the patient group.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Information about sample characteristics is presented in form of M, SD, %, and frequencies. Descriptive statistics (M, SD) for the different sustained attention and inhibitory control measures are reported separately for the total sample, patients vs. healthy controls, and patients with different depression severity. For the oddball paradigm, the following indicators were considered: accuracy and reaction time for frequent and rare trials. For the flanker task, the indicators were as follows: accuracy and reaction time for congruent and incongruent trials, as well as interference score.
Between-group differences were first tested via univariate analyses of (co)variance with the group (e.g., patients vs. controls) as a fixed factor and the executive function measures as dependent variables. In the case of between-group differences in covariates, these variables were controlled in the ANCOVA. In addition, accuracy was used as a further covariate when reaction times were used as outcomes. If more than two groups were combined, Bonferroni post-hoc tests were applied to further explore which groups differ from each other. In order to consider the notion that accuracy and reaction times in frequent/rare and congruent/incongruent trials might be linked, we further carried out repeated-measures ANCOVA, with the group as a fixed factor, frequent/rare (or congruent/incongruent) as a within-subject factor, and the interaction term between these two factors. All analyses were calculated with SPSS 28 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA), and the level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 across all analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics of Patients and Healthy Controls

The characteristics of the total sample, patients with MDD, and healthy controls are shown in Table 1. Sex was equally distributed across groups (54.4% women in the total sample). The mean age was 39.38 years. The majority of the sample spoke German as their first language (87.9%), were Swiss citizens (75.0%), and single (69.4%). Approximately half of the sample reported higher education (47.6%), and one third (31.8%) identified as smokers. Among patients, 34.0 percent had an F32 diagnosis (first episode) and 64.6% had recurrent MDD (F33 diagnosis). At baseline (2–3 after admission to the hospital), 43.4% (n = 92) of the patients reported mild symptom severity, whereas 34.0% (n = 72) reported moderate depression and 22.6% (n = 48) reported severe depression. Moreover, 89.2% of the patients took antidepressants. The healthy controls were younger than the patients, despite our efforts to achieve an age-matched control sample. As shown in Table 1, the patients were more likely to be Swiss, to smoke, and to have higher body weight/BMI, whereas the controls more often reported higher education. The patients also had more years of job experience (mainly because they were older).

3.2. Sustained Attention and Inhibitory Control in Patients and Healthy Controls

Table 2 shows that the patients and controls did not differ with regard to accuracy in the two computer-based tasks. Without controlling for covariates, patients differed in reaction time both in response to frequent and rare stimuli in the oddball task. These differences remained after controlling for age, level of education, smoking, BMI, and nationality. The findings showed that patients had longer reaction times for both frequent and rare stimuli. Age was the only significant covariate, showing that younger participants had shorter reaction times (frequent: p < 0.001, η2 = 0.116; rare: p < 0.001, η2 = 0.087). After controlling for accuracy and covariates, repeated-measures ANCOVA further showed that reaction times did not differ in response to frequent and rare stimuli (Wilks’ lambda, F(1321) = 1.41, p = 0.236), that patients had lower reaction times (Wilks’ lambda, F(1321) = 6.00, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.018), and that no interaction existed between type of stimulus and group (Wilks’ lambda, F(1321) = 0.244, p = 0.622).
In the flanker task, significant differences were found with regard to reaction time in response to congruent and incongruent stimuli. Again, longer reaction times were found in patients compared to controls. In the controlled model, these differences disappeared, mainly because age explained substantial levels of additional variance (congruent: p < 0.001, η2 = 0.102; incongruent: p < 0.001, η2 = 0.092), showing that younger participants reacted faster to both types of stimuli. None of the other covariates were associated with the flanker task outcomes. After controlling for accuracy and covariates, the between-group differences reappeared in the repeated-measures ANCOVA, showing that patients had longer reaction times than healthy controls (Wilks’ lambda, F(1321) = 4.26, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.013) but that reaction times in response to frequent and rare stimuli were similar (Wilks’ lambda, F(1321) = 0.83, p = 0.362), and that there was no interaction between group and type of stimulus (Wilks’ lambda, F(1321) = 0.00, p = 0.985).

3.3. Differences between Patients with Different Symptom Severity

Table 3 shows that patients with mild, moderate, and severe depression did not differ from each other in any of the executive function measures, either with or without the inclusion of covariates. Similarly, in the repeated-measures ANCOVA, no significant main effects were found for type of stimulus or group. Similarly, the interaction terms remained non-significant for both the oddball and flanker task.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this paper was to examine whether sustained attention and inhibitory control differ between patients with MDD and healthy controls, and whether differences exist between patients with mild, moderate, and severe depression. Our data show that patients with depression show slower reaction times in both the oddball and flanker task, independent of the executive demands of the trial types. While performance on both tasks differed between groups, reaction times were not sensitive to the symptom severity of depression.
In the present article, we formulated three hypotheses, and each of these are now briefly discussed. Our first and most general hypothesis that patients have poorer performances in executive function than healthy controls was supported. Patients with depression showed slower reaction times on both the oddball and flanker Task (for the flanker task, this result was present only in the uncontrolled analysis), indicating a delay in specific cognitive processes. This effect was not influenced by a speed–accuracy trade-off, given that accuracy was not sensitive to depression. Our findings support previous investigations in which depression was associated with reduced processing speed in various executive function measures [70], deficits in selective attention [92,93], and inhibition [93,94]. These relations have been observed both in the acute phase of the illness and in phases of recovery [76,95,96,97,98]. Possible mechanisms underlying these associations are differences between patients and healthy controls in neurotransmitters, such as dopamine, histamine, or norepinephrine (and associated pathways to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), which are important for executive functioning [25]. Prior research has also pointed towards structural and functional abnormalities in prefrontal cortex and connecting subcortical regions, such as the hippocampus, in patients with MDD [99,100]. Because most of the previous studies were based on variants of the Stroop task (e.g., [72,95,97,101,102,103,104]), our findings indicate a generalization of impaired performance to other computer-based tasks that tap into executive function. Knowing that executive function and related attentional processes are essential for building capabilities to make individuals independent, creative, and socially integrated [39,105], impairments in this domain are important targets for intervention. Not surprisingly, therefore, specific types of psychotherapy have been suggested to improve executive function among people with depression, including problem-solving therapy [106], cognitive remediation treatment [107], or cognitive enhancement therapy [108]. It should also be noted that the strength of the differences decreased after inclusion of covariates, which was mainly due to the fact that age explained substantial amounts of additional variance. This is in line with prior investigation showing that in adults, executive function generally declines with increasing age [109,110], an effect that was also observed in people with depression [78,96].
Support was also found for our second hypothesis. Thus, patients with depression showed slower reaction times on trials with lower and higher executive demands. Consequently, depression also manifests in more non-automatic and effortful tasks. This may be due to a general slowing of information processing that also complicates performance on tasks requiring higher-order cognitive functions, which rely on overlapping cognitive processes. Previous findings indicate that information processing and executive function are linked, but that information processing is more sensitive to age [111]. Recalling that controlling for age did not reduce the effect size of depression on trials with low and high executive demands on the oddball task, this argues against the idea that impairments in executive function are due to shared cognitive processes. In contrast, the pattern of findings lends further support for specific deficits in executive function and impairments in selective attention in particular [19].
Our third hypothesis was not supported by our findings as the severity of depressive symptoms did not vary as a function of performance on the oddball and flanker Task. This is at odds with previous studies, which pointed towards lower impairments in subclinical participants compared to clinically depressed patients [78,79]. However, this relation was not reported consistently in previous studies [43,96,104]. This finding also contrasts with previously published results from the PACINPAT study, in which we observed that patients with more severe depression self-report a lower intention to exercise, fewer implantation intentions, more physical activity barriers, and more difficulties in dealing with behavioral obstacles [112]. However, it should be noted that laboratory measures of inhibitory control and self-reported measures of self-control do not necessarily exceed low correlations [94,98,113,114]. While this led some researchers to conclude that computer-based neuropsychological tests have limited value as measures of domain-general inhibitory control [113,115,116], others have recommended the use of both objective and subjective measures, such as the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Adult version (BRIEF-A) [117] together to optimize screening [118].
The major strengths of our study are the relatively large sample size (one of the largest samples hitherto assessed to examine the association between depression and executive functions) [19], the use of two tests that have rarely been used in depression research [101,119], and the inclusion of important covariates. Particularly, when comparing patients with mild, moderate, and severe depression, it seemed important to control for depression history because higher impairments were observed in people with recurrent episodes compared to first-episode patients [79,120]. Similarly, we controlled our analyses for antidepressant medication as the impact of medication on cognitive performance is currently a matter of academic debate [121,122]. In line with a recent meta-analysis [122], overall antidepressant treatment was not associated with executive function in the present sample (data not shown). A limitation can be seen in the fact that our results are based on cross-sectional data. Thus, while depression may lead to deficits in executive function via neurophysiological pathways, cognitive vulnerability models of depression [123] have argued that deficits in executive functions may increase the risk of becoming depressed. Thus, attenuated inhibition of negative stimuli in combination with increased activation of subcortical processing regions can contribute to biased attention, distorted cognitions, rumination tendency, and emotional dysregulation caused by the inability to reappraise negative thoughts [124,125,126,127,128]. Additionally, because almost all patients in our sample had an F32 and F33 diagnosis, the findings of our present study cannot be generalized to some other forms of depression. For instance, worse performances in the Stroop task were observed as psychotic compared to non-psychotic depression [129], and poorer performances were found in a meta-analytic study for attention and problem solving in melancholic compared to non-melancholic depression [130]. We also acknowledge that due to the fact that (a) in-patients knew that they would take part in a trial designed to compare two different physical activity counseling approaches, and (b) a key ethical requirement is that participation in scientific studies is voluntary, we cannot fully rule out that in-patients had a more positive attitude towards physical activity than the controls. However, exercise intention was not associated with any of the executive function measures in the present sample (p > 0.05). Finally, it can be criticized that we did not use a complete battery of neuropsychological tests in our study, only looking at sustained attention and inhibitory control as two specific types of executive function. However, a meta-analysis yielded similar effect sizes for different executive function tasks in people with depression compared to controls, with d = 0.58 for inhibition (k = 48); d = 0.47 for shifting (k = 69); d = 0.57 for updating (k = 10); d = 0.45 for verbal working memory (k = 39); d = 0.45 for visuospatial working memory (k = 23), d = 0.38 for planning (k = 17); d = 0.55 for verbal fluency (k = 46); and d = 0.33 for processing speed (k = 23). Finally, as highlighted in the introduction, inhibitory control seems to reflect general aspects that are common to all other types of executive function [40,98].
From a practical point of view, identification of patients who have reduced sustained attention and inhibitory control, and thus may be more vulnerable to relapse, may facilitate targeted and individually tailored preventive measures to make interventions more effective [98,131]. Without adequate executive function skills, forms of therapy that rely on executive function skills might be less effective [114]. In the realm of our intervention trial, our further analyses will reveal whether patients with higher inhibitory control show better adherence to our lifestyle intervention, whether high adherence mediates the effect of the intervention of physical activity, and whether patients with higher inhibitory control are more likely to show an adequate response to treatment in general.

5. Conclusions

Compared to healthy controls, patients with major depression undergoing in-patient treatment show slower reaction time to cognitive tasks that require inhibitory control and sustained attention. This pattern is independent of the executive function demand, suggesting deficits in basic information processing and specific impairments in higher-order cognitive processes. As difficulties in executive function underlie problems in planning, initiating, and completing goal-directed activities, they may jeopardize in-patient treatment and contribute to the recurrent nature of depression.

Author Contributions

J.B., M.H., C.I., U.E.L., S.M., T.M., A.O. and N.S.-K. supported the patient screening and recruitment processes on the four study sites. R.C. and J.-N.K. recruited the participants and collected the data. R.C., S.B., L.D., A.E., O.F., L.Z. and S.L. offered thematic support. S.L. was responsible for the selection and programming of the neuropsychological tests, as well as data processing. M.G. and S.L. were responsible for the data analyses. M.G. conceptualized the manuscript and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All listed co-authors read, contributed to, and approved the final manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The PACINPAT trial is funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant number: 321003B-179353).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical approval was obtained from the “Ethikkommission Nordwest- und Zentralschweiz” (ref approval no. 2018-00976) and all procedures were in line with the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The intervention study was registered in the WHO trial register (trial number: ISRCTN10469580).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available on request due to restrictions.

Acknowledgments

We thank all patients and staff members of the four clinics for their contribution to the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interests.

References

  1. Kessler, R.C.; Petukhova, M.; Sampson, N.A.; Zaslavsky, A.M.; Wittchen, H.U. Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2012, 21, 169–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. WHO. Global Health Estimates: Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  3. Andrade, L.; Caraveo-Anduaga, J.J.; Berglund, P.; Bijl, R.V.; de Graaf, R.; Vollebergh, W.; Dragomierecka, E.; Kohn, R.; Keller, M.; Kessler, R.C. The epidemiology of major depressive episodes: Results from the International Consortium of Psychiatric Epidemiology (ICPE) Surveys. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2003, 12, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hasin, D.S.; Sarvet, A.L.; Meyers, J.L.; Saha, T.D.; Ruan, W.J.; Stohl, M.; Grant, B.F. Epidemiology of adult DSM-5 Major Depressive Disorder and its specifiers in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry 2018, 75, 336–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Gutierrez-Rojas, L.; Porras-Segovia, A.; Dunne, H.; Andrade-Gonzalez, N.; Cervilla, J.A. Prevalence and correlates of major depressive disorder: A systematic review. Braz. J. Psychiatry 2020, 42, 657–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Richards, D. Prevalence and clinical course of depression: A review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011, 31, 1117–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Santomauro, D.F.; Herrera, A.M.M.; Shadid, J.; Zheng, P.; Ashbaugh, C.; Pigott, D.M.; Abbafati, C.; Adolph, C.; Amlag, J.O.; Aravkin, A.Y. Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet 2021, 398, 1700–1712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ekman, M.; Granstrom, O.; Omerov, S.; Jacob, J.; Landen, M. The societal cost of depression: Evidence from 10,000 Swedish patients in psychiatric care. J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 150, 790–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Evans, V.C.; Chan, S.S.L.; Iverson, G.L.; Bond, D.J.; Yatham, L.N.; Lam, R.W. Systematic review of neurocognition and occupational functioning in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatry 2013, 3, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hardeveld, F.; Spijker, J.; de Graaf, R.; Nolen, W.A.; Beekman, A.T.F. Recurrence of major depressive disorder and its predictors in the general population: Results from The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). Psychol. Med. 2013, 43, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mueller, T.I.; Leon, A.C.; Keller, M.B.; Solomon, D.A.; Endicott, J.; Coryell, W.; Warshaw, M.; Maser, J.D. Recurrence after recovery from major depressive disorder during 15 years of observational follow-up. Am. J. Psychiatry 1999, 156, 1000–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Rush, A.J. Chronic major depression: A review and update. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2001, 62, 3–4. [Google Scholar]
  13. Rapaport, M.H.; Clary, C.; Fayyad, R.; Endicott, J. Quality-of-life impairment in depressive and anxiety disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 2005, 162, 1171–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Trivedi, M.H.; Morris, D.W.; Wisniewski, S.R.; Lesser, I.; Nierenberg, A.A.; Daly, E.; Kurian, B.T.; Gaynes, B.N.; Balasubramani, G.K.; Rush, A.J. Increase in work productivity of depressed individuals with improvement in depressive symptom severity. Am. J. Psychiatry 2013, 170, 633–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bromet, E.; Andrade, L.H.; Hwang, I.; Sampson, N.A.; Alonso, J.; de Girolamo, G.; de Graaf, R.; Demyttenaere, K.; Hu, C.; Iwata, N. Cross-national epidemiology of DSM-IV major depressive episode. BMC Med. 2011, 9, 90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gerber, M.; Claussen, M.C.; Cody, R.; Imboden, C.; Ludyga, S.; Scherr, J.; Seifritz, E.; von Känel, R. Cardiovascular disease and excess mortality in depression: Physical activity as a game changer. Dtsch. Z. Sportmed. 2021, 72, 261–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Murray, C.J.L.; Barber, R.M.; Foreman, K.J.; Ozgoren, A.A.; Abd-Allah, F.; Abera, S.F.; Aboyans, V.; Abraham, J.P.; Abubakar, I.; Abu-Raddad, L.J.; et al. Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 306 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 188 countries, 1990–2013: Quantifying the epidemiological transition. Lancet 2015, 386, 2145–2191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Palazidou, E. The neurobiology of depression. Brit. Med. Bull. 2012, 101, 127–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Nuno, L.; Gomez-Benito, J.; Carmona, V.R.; Pino, O. A systematic review of executive function and information processing speed in Major Depression Disorder. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Godard, J.; Baruch, P.; Grondin, S.; Lafleur, M.F. Psychosocial and neurocognitive functioning in unipolar and bipolar depression: A 12-month prospective study. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 196, 145–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Van der Mussele, S.; Fransen, E.; Struyfs, H.; Luyckx, J.; Marien, P.; Saerens, J.; Somers, N.; Goeman, J.; De Deyn, P.; Engelborghs, S. Depression in mild cognitive impairment is associated with progression to Alzheimer’s disease: A longitudinal study. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2014, 42, 1239–1250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Acosta-Baena, N.; Lopera-Gomez, C.M.; Jaramillo-Elorza, M.C.; Velilla-Jimenez, L.; Villegas-Lanau, C.A.; Sepulveda-Falla, D.; Arcos-Burgos, M.; Lopera, F. Early depressive symptoms predict faster dementia progression in autosomal-dominant Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2023, 92, 911–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Donovan, N.J.; Hsu, D.C.; Dagley, A.S.; Schultz, A.P.; Amariglio, R.E.; Mormino, E.C.; Okereke, O.I.; Rentz, D.M.; Johnson, K.A.; Sperling, R.A.; et al. Depressive symptoms and biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease in cognitively normal older adults. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015, 46, 63–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5 (DSM-5), 5th ed.; APA: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  25. DeBattista, C. Executive dysfunction in major depressive disorder. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2005, 5, 79–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Levin, R.L.; Heller, W.; Mohanty, A.; Herrington, J.D.; Miller, G.A. Cognitive deficits in depression and functional specificity of regional brain activity. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2007, 31, 211–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rock, P.L.; Roiser, J.P.; Riedel, W.J.; Blackwell, A.D. Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2014, 44, 2029–2040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Mathews, A.; MacLeod, C. Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 1994, 45, 25–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hertel, P.T.; Gerstle, M. Depressive deficits in forgetting. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 573–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Levens, S.M.; Muhtadie, L.; Gotlib, I.H. Rumination and impaired resource allocation in depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2009, 118, 757–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kozicky, J.M.; Ha, T.H.; Torres, I.J.; Bond, D.J.; Honer, W.G.; Lam, R.W.; Yatham, L.N. Relationship between frontostriatal morphology and executive function deficits in bipolar I disorder following a first manic episode, data from the Systematic Treatment Optimization Program for Early Mania (STOP-EM). Bipolar Disord. 2013, 15, 657–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Trivedi, M.H.; Greer, T.L. Cognitive dysfunction in unipolar depression: Implications for treatment. J. Affect. Disord. 2014, 152, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Elliott, R. Executive functions and their disorders. Brit. Med. Bull. 2003, 65, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Keller, A.S.; Leikauf, J.E.; Holt-Gosselin, B.; Staveland, B.R.; Williams, L.M. Paying attention to attention in depression. Transl. Psychiatry 2019, 9, 279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Beevers, C.G.; Lee, H.J.; Wells, T.T.; Ellis, A.J.; Telch, M.J. Association of predeployment gaze bias for emotion stimuli with later symptoms of PTSD and depression in soldiers deployed in Iraq. Am. J. Psychiatry 2011, 168, 735–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Banich, M.T. Executive function: The search for an integrated account. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 18, 89–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Alves, M.R.P.; Yamamoto, T.; Arias-Carrion, O.; Rocha, N.B.F.; Nardi, A.E.; Machado, S.; Silva, A.C. Executive function impairments in patients with depression. CNS Neurol. Disord. 2014, 13, 1026–1040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Friedman, N.P.; Miyake, A. Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 2017, 86, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Cambridge, O.R.; Knight, M.J.; Mills, N.; Baune, B.T. The clinical relationship between cognitive impairment and psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder: A systematic review. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 269, 157–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Weightman, M.J.; Knight, M.J.; Baune, B.T. A systematic review of the impact of social cognitive deficits on psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder and opportunities for therapeutic intervention. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 274, 195–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Paelecke-Habermann, Y.; Pohl, J.; Leplow, B. Attention and executive functions in remitted major depression patients. J. Affect. Disord. 2005, 89, 125–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Conradi, H.J.; Ormel, J.; de Jonge, P. Presence of individual (residual) symptoms during depressive episodes and periods of remission: A 3-year prospective study. Psychol. Med. 2011, 41, 1165–1174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Dunkin, J.J.; Leuchter, A.F.; Cook, I.A.; Kasl-Godley, J.E.; Abrams, M.; Rosenberg-Thompson, S. Executive dysfunction predicts nonresponse to fluoxetine in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2000, 60, 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Hammar, A.; Ardal, G. Cognitive functioning in major depression—A summary. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2009, 3, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Porter, R.J.; Bourke, C.; Gallagher, P. Neuropsychological impairment in major depression: Its nature, origin and clinical significance. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2007, 41, 115–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gerber, M.; Beck, J.; Brand, S.; Cody, R.; Donath, L.; Eckert, A.; Faude, O.; Fischer, X.; Hatzinger, M.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E.; et al. The impact of life-style Physical Activity Counselling in IN-PATients with major depressive disorders on physical activity, cardiorespiratory fitness, depression, and cardiovascular health risk markers: Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 2019, 20, 367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Audiffren, M.; Andre, N. The exercise-cognition relationship: A virtuous circle. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019, 8, 339–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Cheval, B.; Orsholits, D.; Sieber, S.; Courvoisier, D.; Cullati, S.; Boisgontier, M.P. Relationship Between Decline in Cognitive Resources an Physical Activity. Health Psychol. 2020, 39, 519–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Brand, S.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E.; Pühse, U. Acute effects of moderate aerobic exercise on specific aspects of executive function in different age and fitness groups: A meta-analysis. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 1611–1626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Pühse, U.; Looser, V.-N.; Kamijo, K. Long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals are moderated by sex, exercise type and dose. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 603–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Herrmann, C.; Brand, S.; Pühse, U. Chronic effects of exercise implemented during school-break time on neurophysiological indices of inhibitory control in adolescents. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2018, 10, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Beck, J.; Gerber, M.; Brand, R.; Pühse, U.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E. Executive function performance is reduced during occupational burnout but can recover to the level of healthy controls. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013, 47, 1824–1830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Imboden, C.; Claussen, M.C.; Seifritz, E.; Gerber, M. Physical activity for the treatment and prevention of depression: A rapid review of meta-analyses. Ger. J. Sport Med. 2021, 72, 280–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Imboden, C.; Gerber, M.; Beck, J.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E.; Pühse, U.; Hatzinger, M. Aerobic exercise or stretching as add-on to inpatient treatment of depression: Similar antidepressant effects on depressive symptoms and larger effects on working memory for aerobic exercise alone. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 276, 866–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Morris, T.P.; Burzynska, A.; Voss, M.; Fanning, J.; Salerno, E.A.; Prakash, R.; Gothe, N.P.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Hillman, C.; McAuley, E.; et al. Brain structure and function predict adherence to an exercise intervention in older adults. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2022, 54, 1483–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gujral, S.; Kramer, A.F.; Erickson, K.I. Greater gray matter volume predicts exercise adherence in older adults. Ann. Behav. Med. 2015, 49, S69. [Google Scholar]
  59. Best, J.R.; Chiu, B.K.; Hall, P.A.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Larger lateral prefrontal cortex volume predicts better exercise adherence among older women: Evidence from two exercise training studies. J. Gerontol. 2017, 72, 804–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Krämer, L.; Helmes, A.W.; Bengel, J. Understanding activity limitations in depression: Integrating the concepts of motivation and volition from health psychology into clinical psychology. Eur. Psychol. 2014, 19, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Erickson, K.I.; Creswell, J.D.; Verstynen, T.D.; Gianaros, P.J. Health neuroscience: Defining a new field. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 23, 446–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Krämer, L.V.; Helmes, A.W.; Seelig, H.; Fuchs, R.; Bengel, J. Correlates of reduced exercise behaviour in depression: The role of motivational and volitional deficits. Psychol. Health 2014, 29, 1206–1225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Lindegård, A.; Jonsdottir, I.H.; Börjesson, M.; Lindwall, M.; Gerber, M. Changes in mental health in compliers and non-compliers with physical activity recommendations in patients with stress-related exhaustion. BMC Psychiatry 2015, 15, 272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Hoffman, B.M.; Babyak, M.A.; Craighead, E.; Sherwood, A.; Doraiswamy, P.M.; Coons, M.J.; Blumenthal, J.A. Exercise and pharmacotherapy in patients with major depression: One-year follow-up of the SMILE study. Psychosom. Med. 2011, 73, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Gerber, M.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E.; Pühse, U.; Brand, S. Exercise is medicine for patients with major depressive disorders. But only if the “pill” is taken! Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 1977–1981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Nilsson, J.; Thomas, A.J.; Stevens, L.H.; McAllister-Williams, R.H.; Ferrier, I.N.; Gallagher, P. The interrelationship between attentional and executive deficits in major depressive disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2016, 134, 73–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Bora, E.; Harrison, B.J.; Yucel, M.; Pantelis, C. Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2013, 43, 2017–2026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Snyder, H.R. Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 81–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Xu, G.Y.; Lin, K.G.; Rao, D.P.; Dang, Y.M.; Ouyang, H.Y.; Guo, Y.B.; Ma, J.; Chen, J. Neuropsychological performance in bipolar I, bipolar II and unipolar depression patients: A longitudinal, naturalistic study. J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 136, 328–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Schmid, M.; Hammar, A. Cognitive function in first episode major depressive disorder: Poor inhibition and semantic fluency performance. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 2013, 18, 515–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Protopapas, A.; Archonti, A.; Skaloumbakas, C. Reading ability is negatively related to Stroop interference. Cogn. Psychol. 2007, 54, 251–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Den Hartog, H.M.; Derix, M.M.; Van Bemmel, A.L.; Kremer, B.; Jolles, J. Cognitive functioning in young and middle-aged unmedicated out-patients with major depression, testing the effort and cognitive speed hypotheses. Psychol. Med. 2003, 33, 1443–1451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Egeland, J.; Rund, B.R.; Sundet, K.; Landro, N.I.; Asbjornsen, A.; Lund, A.; Roness, A.; Stordal, K.I.; Hugdahl, K. Attention profile in schizophrenia compared with depression, differential effects of processing speed, selective attention and vigilance. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2003, 108, 276–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Halvorsen, M.; Hoifodt, R.S.; Myrbakk, I.N.; Wang, C.E.A.; Sundet, K.; Eisemann, M.; Waterloo, K. Cognitive function in unipolar major depression: A comparison of currently depressed, previously depressed, and never depressed individuals. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsyc. 2012, 34, 782–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Hammar, A. Automatic and effortful information processing in unipolar major depression. Scand. J. Psychol. 2003, 44, 409–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Hammar, A.; Strand, M.; Ardal, G.; Schmid, M.; Lund, A.; Elliott, R. Testing the cognitive effort hypothesis of cognitive impairment in major depression. Nord. J Psychiatry 2011, 65, 74–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Fossati, P.; Ergis, A.M.; Allilaire, J.F. Executive functioning in unipolar depression: A review. Encephale 2002, 28, 97–107. [Google Scholar]
  78. Epp, A.M.; Dobson, K.S.; Dozois, D.J.A.; Frewen, P.A. A systematic meta-analysis of the Stroop task in depression. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 32, 316–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Vanderhasselt, M.A.; De Raedt, R. Impairments in cognitive control persist during remission from depression and are related to the number of past episodes: An event related potentials study. Biol. Psychol. 2009, 81, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Hamilton, M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 1967, 6, 278–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A.; Carbin, M.G. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 1988, 8, 77–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Craig, C.L.; Marshall, A.L.; Sjöström, M.; Bauman, A.E.; Booth, M.L.; Ainsworth, B.E.; Pratt, M.; Ekelund, U.; Yngve, A.; Sallis, J.F.; et al. International Physical Activity Questionnaire, 12-country reliability and validity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2003, 35, 1381–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Thomas, S.; Reading, J.; Shephard, R.J. Revision of the Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q). Can. J. Sport Sci. 1992, 17, 338–345. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  84. Richter, P.; Werner, J.; Heerlein, A.; Kraus, A.; Sauer, H. On the validity of the Beck Depression Inventory. Psychopathology 1998, 31, 160–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Beck, A.T.; Sheer, R.A.; Brown, G.K. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory—II; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  86. Soveri, A.; Lehtonen, M.; Karlsson, L.C.; Lukasik, K.; Antfolk, J.; Laine, M. Test–retest reliability of five frequently used executive tasks in healthy adults. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2018, 25, 155–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Wöstmann, N.M.; Aichert, D.S.; Costa, A.; Rubia, K.; Möller, H.J.; Ettinger, U. Reliability and plasticity of response inhibition and interference control. Brain Cogn. 2013, 81, 82–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Williams, L.M.; Simms, E.; Clark, C.R.; Paul, R.H.; Rowe, D.; Gordon, E. The test-retest reliability of a standardized neurocognitive and neurophysiological test battery: “Neuromarker”. Int. J. Neurosci. 2005, 115, 1605–1630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Huettel, S.A.; McCarthy, G. What is odd in the oddball task? Prefrontal cortex is activated by dynamic changes in response strategy. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 379–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Eriksen, B.A.; Eriksen, C.W. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Neurophys. 1974, 16, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. García-Larrea, L.; Lukaszewicz, A.C.; Mauguiére, F. Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects. Neuropsychologia 1992, 30, 723–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Wells, T.T.; Beevers, C.G. Biased attention and dysphoria: Manipulating selective attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 24, 719–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Cotrena, C.; Branco, L.D.; Shansis, F.M.; Fonseca, R.P. Executive function impairments in depression and bipolar disorder: Association with functional impairment and quality of life. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 190, 744–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Ajilchi, B.; Nejati, V. Executive functions in students with depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms. Basic Clin. Neurosci. 2017, 8, 223–232. [Google Scholar]
  95. Gohier, B.; Ferracci, L.; Surguladze, S.A.; Lawrence, E.; El Hage, W.; Kefi, M.Z.; Allain, P.; Garre, J.-B.; Le Gall, D. Cognitive inhibition and working memory in unipolar depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2009, 116, 100–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Hammar, A.; Sorensen, L.; Ardal, G.; Oedegaard, K.J.; Kroken, R.; Roness, A.; Lund, A. Enduring cognitive dysfunction in unipolar major depression: A test-retest study using the Stroop paradigm. Scand. J. Psychol. 2010, 51, 304–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Nakano, Y.; Baba, H.; Maeshima, H.; Kitajima, A.; Sakai, Y.; Baba, K.; Suzuki, T.; Mimura, M.; Arai, H. Executive dysfunction in medicated, remitted state of major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2008, 111, 46–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Schmid, M.; Hammar, A. First-episode patients report cognitive difficulties in executive functioning 1 year after initial episode of major depressive disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 667238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Ahdidan, J.; Hviid, L.B.; Chakravarty, M.M.; Ravnkilde, B.; Rosenberg, R.; Rodell, A.; Stodkilde-Jorgensen, H.; Videbech, P. Longitudinal MR study of brain structure and hippocampus volume in major depressive disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2011, 123, 211–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Cheng, Y.Q.; Xu, J.A.; Chai, P.; Li, H.J.; Luo, C.R.; Yang, T.; Li, L.; Shan, B.-C.; Xu, X.-F.; Xu, L. Brain volume alteration and the correlations with the clinical characteristics in drug-naive first-episode MDD patients: A voxel-based morphometry study. Neurosci. Lett. 2010, 480, 30–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Degl’Innocenti, A.; Agren, H.; Backman, L. Executive deficits in major depression. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1998, 97, 182–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Concerto, C.; Lanza, G.; Cantone, M.; Pennisi, M.; Giordano, D.; Spampinato, C.; Ricceri, R.; Pennisi, G.; Aguglia, E.; Bella, R. Different patterns of cortical excitability in major depression and vascular depression: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13, 300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Dalby, R.B.; Frandsen, J.; Chakravarty, M.M.; Ahdidan, J.; Sorensen, L.; Rosenberg, R.; Ostergard, L.; Videbech, P. Correlations between Stroop task performance and white matter lesion measures in late-onset major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 202, 142–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Lampe, I.K.; Sitskoorn, M.M.; Heeren, T.J. Effects of recurrent major depressive disorder on behavior and cognitive function in female depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. 2004, 125, 73–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Buist-Bouwman, M.A.; Ormel, J.; de Graaf, R.; de Jonge, P.; van Sonderen, E.; Alonso, J.; Bruffaerts, R.; Vollebergh, W.A.M.; ESEMed/MHEDEA 2000 investigators. Mediators of the association between depression and role functioning. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2008, 118, 451–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Arean, P.A.; Raue, P.; Mackin, R.S.; Kanellopoulos, D.; McCulloch, C.; Alexopoulos, G.S. Problem-solving therapy and supportive therapy in older adults with major depression and executive dysfunction. Am. J. Psychiatry 2010, 167, 1391–1398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Hagen, B.I.; Lau, B.; Joormann, J.; Smastuen, M.C.; Landro, N.I.; Stubberud, J. Goal management training as a cognitive remediation intervention in depression: A randomized controlled trial. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 275, 268–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Douglas, K.M.; Peckham, A.; Porter, R.; Hammar, A. Cognitive enhancement therapy for mood disorders: A new paradigm? Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2019, 53, 1148–1150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Mrazik, M.; Millis, S.; Drane, D.L. The Oral Trail Making Test: Effects of age and concurrent calidity. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2010, 25, 236–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. MacLeod, C.M. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 163–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Albinet, C.T.; Boucard, G.; Bouquet, C.A.; Audiffren, M. Processing speed and executive functions in cognitive aging: How to disentangle their mutual relationship? Brain Cogn. 2012, 79, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Cody, R.; Beck, J.; Brand, S.; Donath, L.; Eckert, A.; Faude, O.; Hatzinger, M.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E.; Imboden, C.; Kreppke, J.-N.; et al. Depression severity and psychosocial determinants of exercise behavior in in-patients with major depressive disorders. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2022, 63, 102294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Paap, K.R.; Anders-Jefferson, R.; Zimiga, B.; Mason, L.; Mikulinsky, R. Interference scores have inadequate concurrent and convergent validity: Should we stop using the flanker, Simon, and spatial Stroop tasks? Cogn. Res. 2020, 5, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Davalos, D.B.; Pantlin, L.; Crosby, H. Depression and executive dysfunction in young adults: Implications for therapy. J. Depress. Ther. 2015, 1, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Toplak, M.E.; West, R.F.; Stanovich, K.E. Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 131–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Egeland, J.; Lovstad, M.; Norup, A.; Nybo, T.; Persson, B.A.; Rivera, D.; Schanke, A.-K.; Sigurdardottir, S.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C. Questionnaire use among Nordic neuropsychologists: Shift from assessing personality to checking ecological validity of neuropsychological assessments? Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 2017, 48, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Roth, R.M.; Isquith, P.K.; Gioia, G.A. BRIEF-A: Behavior Ratibng Invetory of Executive Function—Adult Version; Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz, FL, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  118. Ott, C.V.; Bjertrup, A.J.; Jensen, J.H.; Ullum, H.; Sjaelland, R.; Purdon, S.E.; Vieta, E.; Kessing, L.V.; Miskowiak, K.W. Screening for cognitive dysfunction in unipolar depression, Validation and evaluation of objective and subjective tools. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 190, 607–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Kindermann, S.S.; Kalayam, B.; Brown, G.G.; Burdick, K.E.; Alexopoulos, G.S. Executive functions and P300 latency in elderly depressed patients and control subjects. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2000, 8, 57–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Preiss, M.; Kucerova, H.; Lukavsky, J.; Stepankova, H.; Sos, P.; Kawaciukova, R. Cognitive deficits in the euthymic phase of unipolar depression. Psychiatry Res. 2009, 169, 235–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Blumberg, M.J.; Vaccarino, S.R.; McInerney, S.J. Procognitive effects of antidepressants and other therapeutic agents in Major Depressive Disorder: A systematic review. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2020, 81, 19r13200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Rosenblat, J.D.; Kakar, R.; McIntyre, R.S. The cognitive effects of antidepressants in Major Depressive Disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016, 19, pyw082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Beck, A.T. Depression, Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects; University of Pennsylvania Press: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
  124. Disner, S.G.; Beevers, C.G.; Haigh, E.A.; Beck, A.T. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011, 12, 467–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Friedman, N.P.; du Pont, A.; Corley, R.P.; Hewitt, J.K. Longitudinal relations between depressive symptoms and executive functions from adolescence to early adulthood: A twin study. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 6, 543–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. De Raedt, R.; Koster, E.H.W. Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: A reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2010, 10, 50–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Joormann, J.; Tanovic, E. Cognitive vulnerability to depression: Examining cognitive control and emotion regulation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015, 4, 86–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Joormann, J.; Gotlib, I.H. Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 24, 281–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Schatzberg, A.F.; Posener, J.A.; DeBattista, C.; Kalehzan, B.M.; Rothschild, A.J.; Shear, P.K. Neuropsychological deficits in psychotic versus nonpsychotic major depression and no mental illness. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 1095–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Zaninotto, L.; Solmi, M.; Veronese, N.; Guglielmo, R.; Ioime, L.; Camardese, G.; Serretti, A. A meta-analysis of cognitive performance in melancholic versus non-melancholic unipolar depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 201, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Snyder, H.R.; Miyake, A.; Hankin, B.L. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Total
Sample
(N = 340)
Patients
with MDD
(n = 212)
Healthy
Controls
(n = 128)
n%n%n%Chi2ϕ
Sex (female)18554.411152.47457.80.960.053
Language (German as first language)29987.918989.211085.90.780.048
Nationality (Swiss)25575.017482.18163.315.04 ***0.210
Marital status (single)23669.415070.88667.20.480.038
Level of education (higher education)16247.67635.88667.231.42 ***0.304
Children living at home (yes)7722.62620.35124.10.640.043
Antidepressant intake (yes)18955.618989.200.0253.350.869
Smoking (yes)10831.88238.72624.111.59 ***0.191
Depression subtype
  Single episode (F32)------7234.0------------
  Recurrent MDD (F33)------13764.6------------
  Bipolar disorder type II (F31-II)------31.4------------
MSDMSDMSDFEta2
Age (in years)39.3813.3941.3212.8336.1613.7212.23 ***0.035
Height (in cm)171.369.31171.649.29170.889.350.540.002
Weight (in kg)77.1920.2480.9621.2470.9516.7520.65 ***0.058
Body mass index (kg/m2)26.125.9827.236.1424.295.2320.21 ***0.057
Employment rate (in hours/week)24.2219.7623.9020.8024.7417.950.150.000
Years of job experience (in years)16.7913.4018.5812.8613.8213.7910.34 ***0.030
Number of children living at home0.230.420.240.430.200.400.640.002
Depressive symptom severity (at baseline)15.0511.9621.6910.254.033.36353.58 ***0.513
Duration of current episode (in weeks) a------39.0151.82------------
Number of prior depressive episodes b------3.156.02------------
Age of onset of depression (in years) c------29.8914.14------------
Notes. MDD = Major depressive disorders. a 32 values missing. b 17 values missing. c 16 values missing. *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and group differences between patients with MDD and healthy controls in executive function measures.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and group differences between patients with MDD and healthy controls in executive function measures.
Total
Sample
(N = 340)
Patients
with MDD
(n = 212)
Healthy
Controls
(n = 128)
UncontrolledControlled for Age, Level of Education, Smoking, BMI, and Nationality
MSDMSDMSDFEta2FEta2
Oddball
  Accuracy: frequent trials96.25.195.95.396.64.81.420.0040.000.000
  Accuracy: rare trials88.211.187.511.189.311.12.30.0070.630.002
  Reaction time: frequent trials (in ms) a379.468.7387.1677.80366.4259.228.10 **0.0236.25 *0.019
  Reaction time: rare trials (in ms) b410.0972.64417.0076.55398.6364.338.27 **0.0246.09 *0.018
Flanker
  Accuracy: congruent trials97.15.696.76.597.83.51.420.0041.550.005
  Accuracy: incongruent trials91.48.990.710.192.60.71.960.0061.710.005
  Reaction time: congruent trials (in ms) c402.5177.68411.4490.01387.7347.885.66 *0.0171.910.006
  Reaction time: incongruent trials (in ms) d445.6391.93456.83108.90427.0647.926.72 **0.0201.860.006
  Interference score43.1141.9245.3950.6739.3420.071.670.0050.230.001
Notes. a Controlled for oddball accuracy for frequent trials. b Controlled for oddball accuracy in rare trials. c Controlled for flanker accuracy in congruent trials. d Controlled for flanker accuracy in incongruent trials. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and group differences between patients with MDD and healthy controls in executive function measures.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and group differences between patients with MDD and healthy controls in executive function measures.
Mild
Depression
(n = 91)
Moderate
Depression
(n = 72)
Severe
Depression
(n = 49)
UncontrolledControlled for Age, Level of Education, Smoking, BMI, Nationality, Depression History (Recurrent/First-Episode), and Antidepressant Medication (Yes/No)
MSDMSDMSDFEta2FEta2
Oddball
  Accuracy: frequent trials95.66.496.30.496.04.70.400.0040.230.002
  Accuracy: rare trials97.311.388.110.886.811.30.230.0020.810.002
  Reaction time: frequent trials (in ms) a386.3260.00389.6379.55385.0485.150.050.0000.860.001
  Reaction time: rare trials (in ms) b418.7070.42419.9182.65409.3979.430.230.0020.910.001
Flanker
  Accuracy: congruent trials96.37.497.93.095.68.12.240.0212.060.020
  Accuracy: incongruent trials90.011.792.76.389.110.72.310.0221.830.018
  Reaction time: congruent trials (in ms) c416.1570.17400.7364.41418.47142.480.280.0030.340.003
  Reaction time: incongruent trials (in ms) d468.57114.89439.7979.36459.91132.090.860.0080.590.006
  Interference score52.4270.7039.0626.2641.4425.091.600.0151.050.011
Notes. a Controlled for oddball accuracy for frequent trials. b Controlled for oddball accuracy in rare trials. c Controlled for flanker accuracy in congruent trials. d Controlled for flanker accuracy in incongruent trials.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gerber, M.; Cody, R.; Beck, J.; Brand, S.; Donath, L.; Eckert, A.; Faude, O.; Hatzinger, M.; Imboden, C.; Kreppke, J.-N.; et al. Differences in Selective Attention and Inhibitory Control in Patients with Major Depressive Disorder and Healthy Controls Who Do Not Engage in Sufficient Physical Activity. J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12, 3370. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12103370

AMA Style

Gerber M, Cody R, Beck J, Brand S, Donath L, Eckert A, Faude O, Hatzinger M, Imboden C, Kreppke J-N, et al. Differences in Selective Attention and Inhibitory Control in Patients with Major Depressive Disorder and Healthy Controls Who Do Not Engage in Sufficient Physical Activity. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2023; 12(10):3370. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12103370

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gerber, Markus, Robyn Cody, Johannes Beck, Serge Brand, Lars Donath, Anne Eckert, Oliver Faude, Martin Hatzinger, Christian Imboden, Jan-Niklas Kreppke, and et al. 2023. "Differences in Selective Attention and Inhibitory Control in Patients with Major Depressive Disorder and Healthy Controls Who Do Not Engage in Sufficient Physical Activity" Journal of Clinical Medicine 12, no. 10: 3370. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12103370

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop