1. Tics Change
Tourette Syndrome (TS) affects approximately 0.5% of the pediatric population in the United States and can be highly debilitating for patients [
1]. Although TS is chronic, tic severity and frequency are far from constant over time. Changes in tics are so typical that they have been part of the definition of TS. The Tourette Syndrome Classification Study Group criteria for definite TS required that “the anatomic location, number, frequency, complexity, type, or severity of tics changes over time.” [
2] Changes in tics are also incorporated into the Diagnostic Confidence Index. The following criteria make up 15% of the DCI: “waxing and waning course,” “some tics have disappeared, some new tics have appeared,” “environment dependent (not only during stress),” “tics voluntarily suppressible,” and “tics are suggestible” [
3].
Much of the research on TS focuses on why some patients develop tics and why others do not, including genetic and environmental factors. However, somewhat less research has focused on what causes tics at any given time point for someone who already has tics. For example, does stress increase tics? If so, why? As Himle et al. put it, “tic variation may in some cases reflect context-dependent interactive learning processes such that once tics [develop], they are shaped and influenced by environmental contingencies.” [
4] Many patients view the circumstances in which TS symptoms transiently worsen or improve as very important [
5]. This topic was covered in a review by Conelea et al. in 2008, but since that time, new research has continued to add to our understanding of how moment-to-moment factors affect tics [
6].
One way to think of how such factors modify tic severity is the ABC model of behavior, which examines antecedents and consequences of any behavior (action), both of which shape how likely that behavior is to occur. For example, if a child hits his sibling, an antecedent might be that the sibling stole his toy, and a consequence might be his parent putting him in time-out. Both the antecedents and the consequences can influence the likelihood that the behavior will be repeated. Different children may have different baseline characteristics that impact their response to particular antecedents and consequences, such as early childhood experiences, anxiety disorders, ADHD, genetic burden, and so on. Even so, Himle et al. found that “tic-exacerbating antecedents and consequences were nearly ubiquitous in a sample of children with chronic tic disorder” [
4].
Understanding these environmental impacts on tics can inform both treatment and parenting strategies. For example, understanding how parental reactions to tics reinforce tics can help parents understand how to best support their children. Additionally, understanding how stress or major life events can trigger increased tic activity may help clinicians understand in what circumstances treatment alterations may be necessary. Finally, understanding the broader context in which their child’s tics occur can help families know when to expect changes in tics. For instance, much research suggests a worsening in tics is common in patients ages 8–10 [
7], possibly due to “more tic-exacerbating consequences” [
4]. This knowledge can aid clinicians in giving anticipatory guidance to parents.
2. Situations That Make Tics Better and Worse: What We Think We Know
According to one survey, the most common factors that patients with TS and their families associated with worsened tics include stress or anxiety, fatigue, holidays, birthdays, and return to school in the fall [
8]. Some patients report that tics become worse in scenarios where they are particularly self-conscious of their tics, noting that “the desire to suppress them tends to make them much worse,” or “my tics get worse in any situation w[h]ere [I] particularly do not want them noticed” [
9]. Additionally, some patients report that any situation in which their tics are acknowledged increases their tics: “talking about my tics … invariably makes me tic much more” [
9]. Patients also reported on situations where they felt their tics were improved, such as being “deep in concentration with something else that [they are] doing” [
9].
However, there is reason to believe that patient self-report measures are not always accurate. In one 2016 study, children were videotaped and surveyed about their tics during five different situations [
10]. Self-reports and videotaped findings were not well correlated, but participants with higher premonitory urges to tic had more accurate self-reports. Additionally, a study that developed a scale to assess the environmental consequences for ticcing found that child-reported data may be more valid than parent-reported data [
11]. These concerns regarding the validity of different types of data are important to keep in mind when evaluating the literature regarding tic severity in different situations. Next, we assess to what extent the data support the perceptions outlined above.
3. “Just the Facts, Ma’am.” What’s the Evidence about What Situations Makes Tics Better and Worse?
3.1. Sleep
A majority of patients feel that fatigue worsens their symptoms [
8,
12]. In a polysomnography-based study, tic severity was positively correlated with the number of nighttime awakenings and negatively correlated with sleep efficiency, suggesting that sleep loss worsens tics [
13]. Additionally, the study found that patients with TS have significantly worsened sleep efficiency than controls [
13]. The surveys also show that many patients with TS exhibit parasomnias such as sleep walking (33%), sleep talking (60%), or night terrors (26%) [
14]. Given that sleep problems are common in TS, they represent an attractive target for treatment, and indeed there are case reports of tics improving after sleep problems improve [
15]. It is possible that sleep loss and fatigue worsen tics primarily due to a decreased ability to manage other tic triggers when fatigued [
16].
3.2. Anxiety
Anxiety is another commonly cited trigger for tics. In one survey of 14 children with TS, the most common noted exacerbating factor was “being upset or anxious” [
12]. A more recent study from Himle et al. looked at antecedents and consequences of tics using the function-based assessment of tics (FBAT) [
4]. In this study, children diagnosed with comorbid internalizing disorders such as anxiety reported more tic-exacerbating antecedents than those without comorbid internalizing disorders. Additionally, the total number of antecedents was correlated with panic and somatic anxiety, and the total number of consequences was correlated with school anxiety and avoidance [
4]. It is possible that children with anxiety disorders have a lower threshold for becoming anxious in response to other triggers, therefore leading to increased tics in response to a greater number of triggers. It is also possible that anxious children are more sensitive to others’ reactions to their tics, thus leading to more perceived consequences for their tics [
4].
A later study of 45 children with TS by Eaton et al. showed that children with higher separation anxiety symptoms had more tics and that children with higher internalizing symptoms experienced more environmental consequences for tics. However, in this study, the environmental consequences themselves accounted for more of the variance in tics than the internalizing symptoms [
17]. This suggests that while anxiety mediates the environmental impact of tics, anxiety alone does not explain differences in tic severity.
Some patients with TS experience so-called “tic attacks,” which are prolonged (>15 min) clusters of tics or tic-like movements. In one case series of 12 children with tic attacks, all children reported comorbid anxiety symptoms [
18]. During the attacks themselves, they reported increased internal focus of attention on tics, suggesting a vicious cycle of anxiety leading to worsened tics and vice versa. In a different study of 75 children with tic disorders and their parents, increased family accommodation was associated with increased anxiety, as well as many other factors, including increased depressive symptoms, externalizing symptoms, aggression, rule-breaking behaviors, and functional impairment from tics [
19]. Interestingly, despite being associated with many negative outcomes, increased family accommodation was not associated with increased tic severity.
However, as Godar and Bortolato [
16] explained, the relationship between anxiety and TS is not clear. Several studies show a greater physiological response to stressors in patients with TS [
20,
21,
22]. However, a recent study found that patients with TS actually showed lower levels of evening cortisol than controls [
22], suggesting that perhaps tics reduce anxiety for patients with TS. Alternatively, it is also possible that the lower evening cortisol markers are simply a marker of chronic stress, which has been shown in multiple studies in non-TS patients [
22,
23,
24,
25,
26].
3.3. Thoughts
Specific types of thoughts have also been associated with increased tics. In one study, 60 patients with TS/CTD were surveyed on their thoughts preceding tics, including three categories: anticipation (that tics may occur), interference (that they may negatively impact activities, self, or appearance), and permission (whether or not the person felt they were allowed to tic) [
27]. The participants reported that all three categories of thoughts led to tics. Importantly, the interference and anticipation subscales of the Thinking About Tics inventory (THAT) were reduced in participants after cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). Godar and Bortolato suggest that perhaps boredom leads to an increased focus on interoceptive stimuli, leading to increased tics [
16].
The impact of external variables on tic frequency has long been noted. For instance, a 1978 longitudinal study of one patient noted that stressful life events overcame any positive medication effects from haloperidol [
28]. In one study from 2003, 76 adults with either habit disorders or chronic tic disorders kept a daily diary of which activities were associated with tics [
29]. While there were some trends in which types of activities were low or high risk for triggering tics, it was also evident that different people reacted differently to the same situations. For instance, 45% of patients with chronic tic disorders found passive attendance (i.e., watching TV or attending a hockey game) to be low-risk, while 38% found it to be high-risk. Overall, high-risk activities included socialization, waiting, and being in transit, while low-risk activities included studying and physical activity. People associated high-risk activities with tenseness, boredom, dissatisfaction, and disinterest. A Himle et al. study from 2014 showed similar results, with the most commonly noted antecedents for tics being TV or video games, home after school, homework, school classrooms, and public places [
4]. This study also helped provide evidence that the consequences that occur after tics can help to explain setting effects. For instance, more attention after tics occurred 82% of the time at home after school as compared to only 37% of the time during sports/physical activity. This difference in the attention paid to tics may partially explain the common sentiment that tics are worsened at home after school versus during physical activity.
3.4. Stress
Nearly all patients report that stress worsens their tics [
8]. However, analyzing the relationship between stress and tics is challenging, both because stress is multifactorial and because different people can react to the same stressor very differently depending on a variety of factors [
16].
When measured in the laboratory setting, several studies have actually shown that tics decrease during stressful tasks. In one study of eight children with co-occurring tic and anxiety disorders, children’s heart rates and tic frequency were measured while completing stress-inducing tasks such as public speaking and discussion of family conflict [
30]. They found no correlation between higher heart rate (indicating physiologic arousal) and tic frequency and, in fact, found that tic frequency decreased with higher heart rates during the speech task. In another study of 31 children and adolescents with tic disorders, tic frequency decreased while participants completed the Trier Social Stress Test, even though physiological markers of stress were present [
31]. However, the techniques used to induce stress in these participants were task-based, and it is possible that the tasks themselves influenced the participants’ tics as they constituted a form of distraction.
While it is not clear that laboratory simulations of stress increase tic frequency, there is some evidence that this type of stress may impair a patient’s ability to suppress tics. In one study, 10 adolescents with TS attempted to suppress tics during stressful and non-stressful conditions [
32]. Although there was no difference in tic frequency between the baseline stressful and non-stressful conditions, the participants were less able to suppress tics during stressful conditions. This result may explain the widespread perception that stress worsens tics. As far as treatments are concerned, relaxation therapy appears to be ineffective as monotherapy, again suggesting that stress per se plays only a limited role in the overall TS picture [
33].
In general, laboratory conditions are best able to simulate short-term, task-based stressors, but many patients use the term “stress” to instead refer to stressful life events. In one 2003 study of children with TS and OCD, although children with these conditions scored higher than controls on a clinician-rated measure of psychosocial stressors, the Yale Children’s Global Stress Index (YCGSI), there was no correlation between the YCGSI score and tic severity [
34]. However, there was a correlation between the scores on the self-reported Daily Life Stressor Scale (DLSS) and tic symptoms severity. The YCGSI was a clinician score measuring major life stressors during the past several months, while DLSS was a patient-scored measure of minor life stressors over the past week. These results suggest both that tic severity correlates better with daily life stressors than major life events and that patients are better able to evaluate their stress exposure than clinicians. This is perhaps because different patients can experience the same event as stressful or not stressful based on a wide range of individual factors.
Other studies show similar results. In a prospective longitudinal study from 2004, there was a small but statistically significant correlation between negative minor life events and tic severity on the aggregate level [
35]. However, when the same study looked at the association on the individual level, only a minority of participants demonstrated a significant relationship between the two. A later prospective longitudinal study found that current psychosocial stress predicted future tic severity but not current tic severity [
36]. A third study found a correlation between both major and minor life events and motor tic severity, but not vocal tic severity [
37]. Taking all real-world studies together, it is clear that recent minor life stressors can play a significant role in increasing tic severity, despite the fact that laboratory-induced stressors have not been shown to have the same effect.
3.5. Rewards for Tic Suppression
It has been well established that rewarding patients for tic suppression decreases tics. A common study design to test this is the Tic Suppression Task, first developed by Woods and Himle [
38]. In this task, the participants are placed in front of a computer or machine that they are told is a tic detector. A researcher observes unobtrusively, for instance, through a one-way mirror, to limit the impact of observation. The “tic detector” then rewards patients for tic-free intervals longer than 10 s, such as with poker chips that can be redeemed for money. The participants are observed during the baseline conditions, where they are instructed to tic freely, and suppression conditions, where they are instructed to suppress their tics and told they will be rewarded. Often, these conditions are measured several times in random order.
In one pooled analysis of nine studies using the Tic Suppression Task, 90% of the participants showed at least some suppressibility in response to immediately rewarded tic suppression [
32,
39,
40,
41,
42,
43,
44,
45,
46,
47]. Furthermore, about 70% of the participants showed at least a 50% reduction in tic frequency, and about 20% of the participants showed at least a 90% reduction in tic frequency. Factors associated with better suppressibility included more frequent tics and older age [
48]. The addition of the stimulant dexmethylphenidate decreased tic frequency at baseline but, somewhat surprisingly, did not enhance tic suppression during the reward task [
49].
Further research has demonstrated that the reward in the Tic Suppression Task must be contingent on tic suppression in order to be effective. A common research design includes a baseline condition, a verbal instructions condition, a Differential Reinforcement of Zero-rate Ticcing (Differential Reinforcement of Other behavior: DRO), and Noncontingent Reinforcement (NCR) [
43,
50]. The participants are instructed to suppress tics and receive rewards in both the DRO and NCR conditions. However, in the NCR conditions, the participants receive rewards regardless of the frequency of tics. The DRO condition outperforms both the NCR and verbal instruction conditions [
43,
50].
Importantly, it is possible to create a tic-reducing condition without the participants’ knowledge. In Woods et al. 2009, the participants completed the Tic Suppression Task with baseline, verbal instruction, and DRO conditions [
51]. During the verbal instruction condition, an orange light was illuminated, and during the DRO condition, a purple light was illuminated. No lights were illuminated during the baseline conditions. The lights were not explained to the participants. The participants completed four different training sessions under these conditions. During a fifth session, the lights were illuminated during the corresponding tasks, but no suppression instructions or reinforcement were provided. Despite this, there was a statistically significant decrease in tics when the purple light was illuminated compared with no lights being illuminated. In other words, the participants had been trained without their knowledge to suppress tics when they observed a previously irrelevant stimulus, the purple light. This result has important implications as it can help explain tic variability between different environments, as there is likely differential reinforcement of tics between these different environments. This is the concept behind the functional intervention component of the Comprehensive Behavioral Intervention for Tics (CBIT), which has been tested in large, randomized controlled trials and is the main component of the CBIT-Jr intervention designed for younger children [
52,
53,
54].
3.6. Distraction/Focused Attention
Multiple studies have demonstrated that tic severity worsens transiently while one focuses one’s attention on tics. One study of two children showed that talking about their tics increased tic frequency while talking about other topics did not [
55]. A later study of 12 patients found that tic frequency increased when patients observed themselves ticcing via a mirror but decreased when watching a video of themselves without tics [
56]. The impact of distraction, or focusing attention on something other than tics, is less consistent throughout different studies. Some studies have found that focusing attention away from tics reduces tic frequency [
57]. Others have found that distraction interferes with tic suppression, thus increasing tic frequency during suppression conditions [
58]. Still others have found that distraction does not impair tic suppression but that performance on the distraction task was worse during periods of tic suppression [
42]. This suggests that tic suppression requires significant attention, which is perhaps why some studies show that distraction worsens patients’ ability to suppress their tics and supports patients’ observations that tic suppression can be burdensome and interfere with performance on school exams.
3.7. Being Observed
Studies differ in their findings as to how different types of observation by others affect tic severity. In one study that looked at tic severity at home and in the office as measured by video recordings, the patients had significantly increased tics at home [
59]. The patients were recorded both with and without others in the room, and tic severity was increased when the patient was alone. However, in a similar study conducted by a different group, there was no difference in tic severity between recordings taken at home or in office, and patients had more tics when being overtly observed as compared to covertly observed [
60]. Finally, a third study, which measured tic frequency across five different daily life activities, found that tic frequency was at its lowest when patients were alone [
10]. Thus, the impacts of being observed are not clear.
3.8. Social Media Exposure
Anecdotal reports from clinicians indicate an increase in patients presenting with tics or tic-like symptoms since the COVID-19 pandemic began, and many attribute the increase to content labeled as TS on social media. Often, the symptoms on these social media sites do not represent classic Tourette’s syndrome symptomatology and instead are more consistent with a type of functional neurological disorder [
61,
62,
63,
64,
65]. While there is limited research on how the popularity of such videos on social media has impacted those with pre-existing diagnoses of Tourette’s syndrome or other chronic tic disorders, it is possible that exposure to such content may increase tics in this population as well.
3.9. Social Situations
Several studies have found that tics are worsened in association with some social situations [
4,
37,
66]. For example, one study showed that minor daily life events were associated with increased tics, and the most frequently cited daily life events were those involving relationships with friends [
37]. A different study of patients with chronic tic disorders examined the attention paid by participants to words associated with social threat as compared to benign words [
67]. They found that the CTD participants had increased attention to threat as compared to controls and that increased attention to a threat was associated with higher scores on some measures of tic severity, suggesting that social conflict and sensitivity to a social conflict may worsen tics.
3.10. Music
Musical performance has also been shown to decrease tics. One survey of 183 musicians with TS showed that on a scale of one (drastic symptom worsening) to five (drastic symptom improvement), the participants averaged a 4.45 rating for their symptoms while they performed music [
68]. A different study videotaped eight musicians while performing, thinking about performing, and listening to music [
69]. All three conditions decreased tics from baseline, with the largest improvement coming while performing. Additionally, there was a short-term decrease in tics in the period after they performed music, although the effect was small.
3.11. Exercise
Exercise has also been shown to decrease tic frequency, both during the exercise itself and to a lesser extent in the period immediately following the exercise [
70,
71]. One study also found a significant negative correlation between exercise and obsessive-compulsive behaviors, anxiety, and depression in children with tic disorders [
71]. It is possible that the improvements in these other factors can partially explain the benefit in terms of tic improvement that is seen.
3.12. Diet and Supplements
Many families report that certain food types worsen tics. In one survey of 224 participants with TS regarding dietary habits and tic frequency, there was a significant negative effect on tics for foods containing caffeine, preserving agents, refined sugar, and sweeteners [
72]. However, given that this study was based on participant self-reports, more study on this topic is necessary. The treatment of tics with the addition of certain foods or supplements has also been attempted, such as probiotics, N-acetyl cysteine, and omega-3 fatty acids, but results have generally not shown significant improvements [
73,
74,
75]. One open-label study did show improvement in tics in patients given L-Theanine and Vitamin B6 as compared with a control condition of psychoeducation [
76]. Additionally, an open-label study of magnesium and vitamin B6 showed an improvement in children with TS suffering from clinical exacerbations [
77]. However, further placebo-controlled studies are necessary.
4. Relation to Brain Pathophysiologic Models of Tic Production
Various authors have proposed models of regional brain activity that might produce tics [
16,
78,
79,
80,
81,
82,
83]. Godar and Bortolato most directly address the potential role of immediate environmental effects in their model [
16], but most models do not attempt to make such a link. Few studies directly measure the effects of factors that transiently affect tic severity on regional pharmacology or neuronal activity. Therefore, the available evidence has limited ability to support any one pathophysiological model. Imaging or EEG technology could be used to identify the brain regions activated while exposed to specific antecedents or consequences. In the future, new techniques based on machine learning and computer modeling may help to investigate the explanatory power of specific models given such data.
5. What to Do about It
We can take advantage of these situational effects on tics to improve tic severity and frequency. In other words, if the environment can worsen or improve tic severity, we can exploit such effects to improve the lives of people with tics. To start, most experts recommend creating a “tic-neutral environment” [
84]. This is intended to minimize the antecedents and consequences of tics, thus reducing tics. CBIT and CBIT-Jr are evidence-based treatment modalities for TS that work on this concept [
52,
53,
54]. CBIT focuses primarily on habit reversal training, while CBIT-Jr is adapted for younger patients and focuses heavily on parent–child interactions. Both include a functional assessment and intervention, which is meant to identify and then modify tic antecedents and consequences. For instance, negative parental reactions may be identified as consequences of tics. As an intervention, parents would then be taught to minimize their reactions to tics [
52]. The Tic Accommodation and Reaction Scale is a validated measure to identify environmental antecedents and consequences and could be used to track the effectiveness of these types of treatments [
11].
Patients often resist tic-suppression-based behavioral interventions such as CBIT and exposure and response prevention (ERP) based on perceptions acquired over years of intermittent efforts to suppress tics. Occasional, spontaneous efforts at tic suppression can lead to frustration, which can paradoxically worsen tics or even reinforce ticcing. However, consistent, expert-directed tic suppression is an important part of these treatment modalities.
There is also evidence that treating anxiety disorders, as well as managing less severe anxiety, can be helpful. Relaxation biofeedback has been shown to be potentially helpful in the treatment of tics [
85]. A small pilot study also showed positive results for a combination of neurofeedback combined with imagery training, which may induce a state similar to the state of focused attention, which is often thought to reduce tics [
86]. Other common-sense suggestions have less of an evidence base, such as music therapy, managing fatigue, improving sleep, exercising, engaging in enjoyed activities, and distracting attention from tics. However, these are likely to be helpful as well, and many are good recommendations for all patients whether or not they have a chronic tic disorder.
6. Future Directions
As mentioned above, there are currently many factors for which evidence is mixed regarding their impact on tics. These factors, such as stress, distraction, and observation, would certainly benefit from further research. Other factors have limited research altogether, such as the impacts of social media, dietary changes, parenting styles, sibling roles, and educational environments. New research techniques could also improve the quality of the research on this topic. For example, machine learning, computational modeling, and imaging approaches could be useful in linking behavioral antecedents and consequences to underlying brain mechanisms. Other novel approaches, such as wearable technologies, could also be used to track tic frequency as a method of momentary ecological assessment in the future to help provide more accurate measurements of tics in response to real-world situations.
7. Summary
In summary, many factors can affect the moment-to-moment expression of tics (
Table 1). Variables such as fatigue, anxiety, and certain types of thoughts have all been shown to worsen tics. The impact of other variables has been more variable. Different types of stress have been shown to both increase and decrease tics, and the impact of stress on tics continues to be an active area of research. Distraction and being observed similarly have had mixed impacts. Some areas of research, such as the impact of social media exposure and different foods, would benefit from more extensive research in the future. However, it is clear overall that the environment can significantly impact tic severity, even when the patient is not aware of the impact or is no longer receiving behavioral reinforcement. Additionally, reinforcement for tic suppression has been clearly shown to improve tics and has formed the basis for many common forms of treatment for TS. As our understanding of which factors worsen and improve tics continues to grow, the treatment options for TS should continue to improve as well.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, A.M.I. and K.J.B.; writing—original draft preparation, A.M.I.; writing—review and editing, K.J.B.; supervision, K.J.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
Funding was provided by Washington University Yearlong Research Program Dean’s Fellowship; The Tourette Association of America and its Missouri chapter; the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University in St. Louis.
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.
Acknowledgments
A previous version of this manuscript appeared on OSF Preprints (
https://osf.io/z2ebh (accessed on 1 September 2022)).
Conflicts of Interest
The funder had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results. A.M.I.: No conflicts of interest. K.J.B.: No relevant conflicts of interest.
References
- Scharf, J.M.; Miller, L.L.; Gauvin, C.A.; Alabiso, J.; Mathews, C.A.; Ben-Shlomo, Y. Population prevalence of Tourette syndrome: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Mov. Disord. 2015, 30, 221–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- The Tourette Syndrome Classification Study Group. Definitions and classification of tic disorders. Arch. Neurol. 1993, 50, 1013–1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Robertson, M.M.; Banerjee, S.; Kurlan, R.; Cohen, D.J.; Leckman, J.F.; McMahon, W.; Pauls, D.L.; Sandor, P.; van de Wetering, B.J. The Tourette syndrome diagnostic confidence index: Development and clinical associations. Neurology 1999, 53, 2108–2112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Himle, M.B.; Capriotti, M.R.; Hayes, L.P.; Ramanujam, K.; Scahill, L.; Sukhodolsky, D.G.; Wilhelm, S.; Deckersbach, T.; Peterson, A.L.; Specht, M.W.; et al. Variables Associated with Tic Exacerbation in Children with Chronic Tic Disorders. Behav. Modif. 2014, 38, 163–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Beljaars, D. Compulsive Body Spaces, 1st ed.; Routledge: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2022; p. 188. [Google Scholar]
- Conelea, C.A.; Woods, D.W. The influence of contextual factors on tic expression in Tourette’s syndrome: A review. J. Psychosom. Res. 2008, 65, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bloch, M.H.; Leckman, J.F. Clinical course of Tourette syndrome. J. Psychosom. Res. 2009, 67, 497–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bornstein, R.A.; Stefl, M.E.; Hammond, L. A survey of Tourette syndrome patients and their families: The 1987 Ohio Tourette Survey. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 1990, 2, 275–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turtle, L.; Robertson, M.M. Tics, twitches, tales: The experiences of Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2008, 78, 449–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barnea, M.; Benaroya-Milshtein, N.; Gilboa-Sechtman, E.; Woods, D.W.; Piacentini, J.; Fennig, S.; Apter, A.; Steinberg, T. Subjective versus objective measures of tic severity in Tourette syndrome—The influence of environment. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 242, 204–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Capriotti, M.R.; Piacentini, J.C.; Himle, M.B.; Ricketts, E.J.; Espil, F.M.; Lee, H.J.; Turkel, J.E.; Woods, D.W. Assessing Environmental Consequences of Ticcing in Youth with Chronic Tic Disorders: The Tic Accommodation and Reactions Scale. Child. Health Care 2015, 44, 205–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silva, R.R.; Munoz, D.M.; Barickman, J.; Friedhoff, A.J. Environmental factors and related fluctuation of symptoms in children and adolescents with Tourette’s disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1995, 36, 305–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohrs, S.; Rasch, T.; Altmeyer, S.; Kinkelbur, J.; Kostanecka, T.; Rothenberger, A.; Ruther, E.; Hajak, G. Decreased sleep quality and increased sleep related movements in patients with Tourette’s syndrome. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2001, 70, 192–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Robertson, M.M.; Banerjee, S.; Eapen, V.; Fox-Hiley, P. Obsessive compulsive behaviour and depressive symptoms in young people with Tourette syndrome. A controlled study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2002, 11, 261–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghosh, D.; Rizkala, A.; Khuhro, A. Management of sleep problems helps improve tic control in children with Tourette Syndrome/chronic tic disorder. In Proceedings of the 47th Child Neurology Society Meeting, Chicago, IL, USA, 15–18 October 2018; p. S370. [Google Scholar]
- Godar, S.C.; Bortolato, M. What makes you tic? Translational approaches to study the role of stress and contextual triggers in Tourette syndrome. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 76, 123–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Eaton, C.K.; Jones, A.M.; Gutierrez-Colina, A.M.; Ivey, E.K.; Carlson, O.; Melville, L.; Kardon, P.; Blount, R.L. The Influence of Environmental Consequences and Internalizing Symptoms on Children’s Tic Severity. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2017, 48, 327–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robinson, S.; Hedderly, T. Novel Psychological Formulation and Treatment of “Tic Attacks” in Tourette Syndrome. Front. Pediatr. 2016, 4, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Storch, E.A.; Johnco, C.; McGuire, J.F.; Wu, M.S.; McBride, N.M.; Lewin, A.B.; Murphy, T.K. An initial study of family accommodation in children and adolescents with chronic tic disorders. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 26, 99–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chappell, P.; Riddle, M.; Anderson, G.; Scahill, L.; Hardin, M.; Walker, D.; Cohen, D.; Leckman, J. Enhanced stress responsivity of Tourette syndrome patients undergoing lumbar puncture. Biol. Psychiatry 1994, 36, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chappell, P.; Leckman, J.; Goodman, W.; Bissette, G.; Pauls, D.; Anderson, G.; Riddle, M.; Scahill, L.; McDougle, C.; Cohen, D. Elevated cerebrospinal fluid corticotropin-releasing factor in Tourette’s syndrome: Comparison to obsessive compulsive disorder and normal controls. Biol. Psychiatry 1996, 39, 776–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corbett, B.A.; Mendoza, S.P.; Baym, C.L.; Bunge, S.A.; Levine, S. Examining cortisol rhythmicity and responsivity to stress in children with Tourette syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008, 33, 810–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nickel, C.; Tanca, S.; Kolowos, S.; Pedrosa-Gil, F.; Bachler, E.; Loew, T.H.; Gross, M.; Rother, W.K.; Nickel, M.K. Men with chronic occupational stress benefit from behavioural/psycho-educational group training: A randomized, prospective, controlled trial. Psychol. Med. 2007, 37, 1141–1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nicolson, N.A.; van Diest, R. Salivary cortisol patterns in vital exhaustion. J. Psychosom. Res. 2000, 49, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ockenfels, M.C.; Porter, L.; Smyth, J.; Kirschbaum, C.; Hellhammer, D.H.; Stone, A.A. Effect of chronic stress associated with unemployment on salivary cortisol: Overall cortisol levels, diurnal rhythm, and acute stress reactivity. Psychosom. Med. 1995, 57, 460–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Secunda, S.K.; Cross, C.K.; Koslow, S.; Katz, M.M.; Kocsis, J.; Maas, J.W.; Landis, H. Biochemistry and suicidal behavior in depressed patients. Biol. Psychiatry 1986, 21, 756–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Connor, K.; St-Pierre-Delorme, M.E.; Leclerc, J.; Lavoie, M.; Blais, M.T. Meta-cognitions in tourette syndrome, tic disorders, and body-focused repetitive disorder. Can. J. Psychiatry 2014, 59, 417–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Surwillo, W.W.; Shafii, M.; Barrett, C.L. Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: A 20-month study of the effects of stressful life events and haloperidol on symptom frequency. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1978, 166, 812–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Connor, K.; Brisebois, H.; Brault, M.; Robillard, S.; Loiselle, J. Behavioral activity associated with onset in chronic tic and habit disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 2003, 41, 241–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conelea, C.A.; Ramanujam, K.; Walther, M.R.; Freeman, J.B.; Garcia, A.M. Is There a Relationship Between Tic Frequency and Physiological Arousal? Examination in a Sample of Children With Co-Occurring Tic and Anxiety Disorders. Behav. Modif. 2014, 38, 217–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Buse, J.; Enghardt, S.; Kirschbaum, C.; Ehrlich, S.; Roessner, V. Tic Frequency Decreases during Short-term Psychosocial Stress—An Experimental Study on Children with Tic Disorders. Front. Psychiatry 2016, 7, 84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Conelea, C.A.; Woods, D.W.; Brandt, B.C. The impact of a stress induction task on tic frequencies in youth with Tourette Syndrome. Behav. Res. Ther. 2011, 49, 492–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bergin, A.; Waranch, H.R.; Brown, J.; Carson, K.; Singer, H.S. Relaxation therapy in Tourette syndrome: A pilot study. Pediatr. Neurol. 1998, 18, 136–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Findley, D.B.; Leckman, J.F.; Katsovich, L.; Lin, H.; Zhang, H.; Grantz, H.; Otka, J.; Lombroso, P.J.; King, R.A. Development of the Yale Children’s Global Stress Index (YCGSI) and its application in children and adolescents ith Tourette’s syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2003, 42, 450–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoekstra, P.J.; Steenhuis, M.P.; Kallenberg, C.G.; Minderaa, R.B. Association of small life events with self reports of tic severity in pediatric and adult tic disorder patients: A prospective longitudinal study. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2004, 65, 426–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lin, H.; Katsovich, L.; Ghebremichael, M.; Findley, D.B.; Grantz, H.; Lombroso, P.J.; King, R.A.; Zhang, H.; Leckman, J.F. Psychosocial stress predicts future symptom severities in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome and/or obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2007, 48, 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Steinberg, T.; Shmuel-Baruch, S.; Horesh, N.; Apter, A. Life events and Tourette syndrome. Compr. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 467–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woods, D.W.; Himle, M.B. Creating tic suppression: Comparing the effects of verbal instruction to differential reinforcement. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2004, 37, 417–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Conelea, C.A.; Wellen, B.; Woods, D.W.; Greene, D.J.; Black, K.J.; Specht, M.; Himle, M.B.; Lee, H.J.; Capriotti, M. Patterns and Predictors of Tic Suppressibility in Youth with Tic Disorders. Front. Psychiatry 2018, 9, 188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Capriotti, M.R.; Brandt, B.C.; Turkel, J.E.; Lee, H.J.; Woods, D.W. Negative Reinforcement and Premonitory Urges in Youth With Tourette Syndrome: An Experimental Evaluation. Behav. Modif. 2014, 38, 276–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Capriotti, M.R.; Turkel, J.E.; Johnson, R.A.; Espil, F.M.; Woods, D.W. Comparing fixed-amount and progressive-amount DRO Schedules for tic suppression in youth with chronic tic disorders. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2017, 50, 106–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Conelea, C.A.; Woods, D.W. Examining the impact of distraction on tic suppression in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome. Behav. Res. Ther. 2008, 46, 1193–1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greene, D.J.; Koller, J.M.; Robichaux-Viehoever, A.; Bihun, E.C.; Schlaggar, B.L.; Black, K.J. Reward enhances tic suppression in children within months of tic disorder onset. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015, 11, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Specht, M.W.; Woods, D.W.; Nicotra, C.M.; Kelly, L.M.; Ricketts, E.J.; Conelea, C.A.; Grados, M.A.; Ostrander, R.S.; Walkup, J.T. Effects of tic suppression: Ability to suppress, rebound, negative reinforcement, and habituation to the premonitory urge. Behav. Res. Ther. 2013, 51, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Woods, D.W.; Himle, M.B.; Miltenberger, R.G.; Carr, J.E.; Osmon, D.C.; Karsten, A.M.; Jostad, C.; Bosch, A. Durability, negative impact, and neuropsychological predictors of tic suppression in children with chronic tic disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2008, 36, 237–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Capriotti, M.R.; Brandt, B.C.; Ricketts, E.J.; Espil, F.M.; Woods, D.W. Comparing the effects of differential reinforcement of other behavior and response-cost contingencies on tics in youth with Tourette syndrome. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2012, 45, 251–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Conelea, C.; Frank, M.; Frank, H.; Wiecki, T.; Hana, L.; Pei, R. Role of reinforcement learning in tic expression: Integrating methods from behavioral psychology and computational neuroscience. In Behavioral Research in Tourette Syndrome: Standing on the Shoulders of Neuroscience, Psychiatry, and Neurology, Proceedings of the Annual Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies Conference, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 20–23 November 2014; ABCT: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Ueda, K.; Kim, S.; Greene, D.J.; Black, K.J. Correlates and clinical implications of tic suppressibility. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2021, 8, 112–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lyon, G.J.; Samar, S.M.; Conelea, C.; Trujillo, M.R.; Lipinski, C.M.; Bauer, C.C.; Brandt, B.C.; Kemp, J.J.; Lawrence, Z.E.; Howard, J.; et al. Testing tic suppression: Comparing the effects of dexmethylphenidate to no medication in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Tourette’s disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2010, 20, 283–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Himle, M.B.; Woods, D.W.; Bunaciu, L. Evaluating the role of contingency in differentially reinforced tic suppression. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2008, 41, 285–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Woods, D.W.; Walther, M.R.; Bauer, C.C.; Kemp, J.J.; Conelea, C.A. The development of stimulus control over tics: A potential explanation for contextually-based variability in the symptoms of Tourette syndrome. Behav. Res. Ther. 2009, 47, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piacentini, J.; Woods, D.W.; Scahill, L.; Wilhelm, S.; Peterson, A.L.; Chang, S.; Ginsburg, G.S.; Deckersbach, T.; Dziura, J.; Levi-Pearl, S.; et al. Behavior therapy for children with Tourette disorder: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2010, 303, 1929–1937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bennett, S.M.; Capriotti, M.; Bauer, C.; Chang, S.; Keller, A.E.; Walkup, J.; Woods, D.; Piacentini, J. Development and Open Trial of a Psychosocial Intervention for Young Children With Chronic Tics: The CBIT-JR Study. Behav. Ther. 2020, 51, 659–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilhelm, S.; Peterson, A.L.; Piacentini, J.; Woods, D.W.; Deckersbach, T.; Sukhodolsky, D.G.; Chang, S.; Liu, H.; Dziura, J.; Walkup, J.T.; et al. Randomized trial of behavior therapy for adults with Tourette syndrome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2012, 69, 795–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Woods, D.W.; Watson, T.S.; Wolfe, E.; Twohig, M.P.; Friman, P.C. Analyzing the influence of tic-related talk on vocal and motor tics in children with Tourette’s syndrome. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2001, 34, 353–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brandt, V.C.; Lynn, M.T.; Obst, M.; Brass, M.; Munchau, A. Visual feedback of own tics increases tic frequency in patients with Tourette’s syndrome. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015, 6, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Misirlisoy, E.; Brandt, V.; Ganos, C.; Tubing, J.; Munchau, A.; Haggard, P. The relation between attention and tic generation in Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychology 2015, 29, 658–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herrmann, K.; Sprenger, A.; Baumung, L.; Alvarez-Fischer, D.; Munchau, A.; Brandt, V. Help or hurt? How attention modulates tics under different conditions. Cortex 2019, 120, 471–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goetz, C.G.; Leurgans, S.; Chmura, T.A. Home alone: Methods to maximize tic expression for objective videotape assessments in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Mov. Disord. 2001, 16, 693–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piacentini, J.; Himle, M.B.; Chang, S.; Baruch, D.E.; Buzzella, B.A.; Pearlman, A.; Woods, D.W. Reactivity of tic observation procedures to situation and setting. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2006, 34, 649–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olvera, C.; Stebbins, G.T.; Goetz, C.G.; Kompoliti, K. TikTok Tics: A Pandemic Within a Pandemic. Mov. Disord. Clin. Pract. 2021, 8, 1200–1205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulus, T.; Baumer, T.; Verrel, J.; Weissbach, A.; Roessner, V.; Beste, C.; Munchau, A. Pandemic Tic-like Behaviors Following Social Media Consumption. Mov. Disord. 2021, 36, 2932–2935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hull, M.; Parnes, M. Tics and TikTok: Functional Tics Spread Through Social Media. Mov. Disord. Clin. Pract. 2021, 8, 1248–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zea Vera, A.; Bruce, A.; Garris, J.; Tochen, L.; Bhatia, P.; Lehman, R.K.; Lopez, W.; Wu, S.W.; Gilbert, D.L. The Phenomenology of Tics and Tic-Like Behavior in TikTok. Pediatr. Neurol. 2022, 130, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malaty, I.A.C.; Anderson, S.; Bennett, S.M.; Budman, C.L.; Coffey, B.J.; Coffman, K.A.; Greenberg, E.; McGuire, J.F.; Müller-Vahl, K.R.; Okun, M.S.; et al. Diagnosis and management of functional tic-like phenomena. OSF Preprints 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caurin, B.; Serrano, M.; Fernandez-Alvarez, E.; Campistol, J.; Perez-Duenas, B. Environmental circumstances influencing tic expression in children. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2014, 18, 157–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pile, V.; Robinson, S.; Topor, M.; Hedderly, T.; Lau, J.Y.F. Attention bias for social threat in youth with tic disorders: Links with tic severity and social anxiety. Child Neuropsychol. 2019, 25, 394–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brown, W.C. Influence of Musical Engagement on Symptoms of Tourette’s Disorder. Ph.D. Thesis, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Bodeck, S.; Lappe, C.; Evers, S. Tic-reducing effects of music in patients with Tourette’s syndrome: Self-reported and objective analysis. J. Neurol. Sci. 2015, 352, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nixon, E.; Glazebrook, C.; Hollis, C.; Jackson, G.M. Reduced Tic Symptomatology in Tourette Syndrome After an Acute Bout of Exercise: An Observational Study. Behav. Modif. 2014, 38, 235–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pringsheim, T.; Nosratmirshekarlou, E.; Doja, A.; Martino, D. Physical activity, sleep and neuropsychiatric symptom severity in children with tourette syndrome. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2021, 30, 711–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Muller-Vahl, K.R.; Buddensiek, N.; Geomelas, M.; Emrich, H.M. The influence of different food and drink on tics in Tourette syndrome. Acta Paediatr. 2008, 97, 442–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, C.C.; Wong, L.C.; Hsu, C.J.; Yang, C.W.; Tsai, Y.C.; Cheng, F.S.; Hu, H.Y.; Lee, W.T. Randomized Controlled Trial of Probiotic PS128 in Children with Tourette Syndrome. Nutrients 2021, 13, 3698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bloch, M.H.; Panza, K.E.; Yaffa, A.; Alvarenga, P.G.; Jakubovski, E.; Mulqueen, J.M.; Landeros-Weisenberger, A.; Leckman, J.F. N-Acetylcysteine in the Treatment of Pediatric Tourette Syndrome: Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Add-On Trial. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2016, 26, 327–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gabbay, V.; Babb, J.S.; Klein, R.G.; Panzer, A.M.; Katz, Y.; Alonso, C.M.; Petkova, E.; Wang, J.; Coffey, B.J. A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of omega-3 fatty acids in Tourette’s disorder. Pediatrics 2012, 129, e1493–e1500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rizzo, R.; Prato, A.; Scerbo, M.; Saia, F.; Barone, R.; Curatolo, P. Use of Nutritional Supplements Based on L-Theanine and Vitamin B6 in Children with Tourette Syndrome, with Anxiety Disorders: A Pilot Study. Nutrients 2022, 14, 852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garcia-Lopez, R.; Romero-Gonzalez, J.; Perea-Milla, E.; Ruiz-Garcia, C.; Rivas-Ruiz, F.; de Las Mulas Bejar, M. An open study evaluating the efficacy and security of magnesium and vitamin B(6) as a treatment of Tourette syndrome in children. Med. Clin. (Barc.) 2008, 131, 689–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Caligiore, D.; Mannella, F.; Arbib, M.A.; Baldassarre, G. Dysfunctions of the basal ganglia-cerebellar-thalamo-cortical system produce motor tics in Tourette syndrome. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2017, 13, e1005395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- McNaught, K.S.; Mink, J.W. Advances in understanding and treatment of Tourette syndrome. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2011, 7, 667–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mink, J.W. Neurobiology of basal ganglia and Tourette syndrome: Basal ganglia circuits and thalamocortical outputs. Adv. Neurol. 2006, 99, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Maia, T.V.; Conceicao, V.A. Dopaminergic Disturbances in Tourette Syndrome: An Integrative Account. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 84, 332–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Conceicao, V.A.; Dias, A.; Farinha, A.C.; Maia, T.V. Premonitory urges and tics in Tourette syndrome: Computational mechanisms and neural correlates. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2017, 46, 187–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Robertson, M.M.; Eapen, V.; Singer, H.S.; Martino, D.; Scharf, J.M.; Paschou, P.; Roessner, V.; Woods, D.W.; Hariz, M.; Mathews, C.A.; et al. Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 2017, 3, 16097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Walkup, J.; Mink, J.; McNaught, K. (Eds.) A Family’s Guide to Tourette Syndrome; iUniverse: Bloomington, IN, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Nagai, Y.; Cavanna, A.; Critchley, H.D. Influence of sympathetic autonomic arousal on tics: Implications for a therapeutic behavioral intervention for Tourette syndrome. J. Psychosom. Res. 2009, 67, 599–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhuo, C.; Li, L. The application and efficacy of combined neurofeedback therapy and imagery training in adolescents with Tourette syndrome. J. Child Neurol. 2014, 29, 965–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1.
Summary of factors that impact tics.
Table 1.
Summary of factors that impact tics.
Improves Tics | Mixed Effects | Worsens Tics | Unclear Effects |
---|
Rewards for tic suppression Musical performance Exercise | Stress Distraction Observation by others | Fatigue Anxiety Thinking about tics Attention to tics Social conflict | Social media Some foods Dietary supplements |
| Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).