Next Article in Journal
Effect of Dosing Interval on Compliance of Osteoporosis Patients on Bisphosphonate Therapy: Observational Study Using Nationwide Insurance Claims Data
Next Article in Special Issue
Development of a Psychosocial Intervention to Promote Treatment Adherence in Patients with Bipolar Disorder at Risk of Cardiovascular Disease
Previous Article in Journal
Epidemiologic, Postmortem Computed Tomography-Morphologic and Biomechanical Analysis of the Effects of Non-Invasive External Pelvic Stabilizers in Genuine Unstable Pelvic Injuries
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Exploring Clinical Correlates of Metacognition in Bipolar Disorders Using Moderation Analyses: The Role of Antipsychotics

by
Paul Roux
1,2,3,*,
Nathan Faivre
4,
Anne-Sophie Cannavo
2,3,
Eric Brunet-Gouet
1,2,3 and
Christine Passerieux
1,2,3
1
DisAP-DevPsy-CESP, UMR 1018, INSERM/Université Paris-Saclay/Université Versailles Saint-Quentin-En-Yvelines, 94807 Villejuif, France
2
Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, Service Hospitalo-Universitaire de Psychiatrie d’Adultes et d’Addictologie, 78150 Le Chesnay, France
3
Fondation Fondamental, 94000 Créteil, France
4
Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10(19), 4349; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10194349
Submission received: 12 August 2021 / Revised: 18 September 2021 / Accepted: 21 September 2021 / Published: 24 September 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Advances in Clinical Management of Bipolar Disorder)

Abstract

:
The determinants of metacognition are still poorly understood in bipolar disorders (BD). We aimed to examine the clinical determinants of metacognition, defined as the agreement between objective and subjective cognition in individuals with BD. The participants consisted of 281 patients with BD who underwent an extensive neuropsychological battery and clinical evaluation. To assess subjective cognition, participants provided a general rating of their estimated cognitive difficulties. Clinical characteristics of BD were also recorded, along with medication. We studied the potential moderation of the association between cognitive complaints and global objective cognitive performance by several clinical variables with ordinal logistic regressions. Depression and impulsivity were associated with greater cognitive complaints. The only variable that moderated the relationship between objective and subjective cognition in the global model was the prescription of antipsychotics. Patients taking antipsychotics had a poorer association between cognitive complaints and objective neuropsychological performance. This result suggests a role for dopamine in the modulation of metacognitive performance, and calls for the systematic control of antipsychotic medication in future studies documenting metacognitive deficits in severe and persistent mental disorders. Depression and impulsivity should be investigated as potential therapeutic targets for individuals with BD and cognitive complaints, before proposing an extensive neuropsychological evaluation.

1. Introduction

Metacognition refers to a spectrum of mental activities of which the object is one’s own thoughts. In individuals suffering from severe and persistent psychiatric disorders, metacognition is a multi-faceted construct encompassing the recognition that one is ill, the awareness of one’s own cognitive style and beliefs, and the awareness of one’s own cognitive performance [1]. Lack of insight into the illness is frequent in BD (Bipolar Disorders) [2], particularly during the manic phase [3]. Several studies have identified dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs in BD [4], which are associated with a higher level of depression, earlier onset of affective illness [5], and worse cognitive impairments [6]. Cognitive insight in BD is characterized by higher self-reflectiveness, i.e., the capacity to reflect on one’s own experiences, which correlates with more severe depression [7].
One way to evaluate the awareness of one’s cognitive performance is to measure the agreement between objective cognition measured using established neuropsychological tests and subjective cognition assessed using self-reported scales. In this framework, good metacognitive performance implies a close relationship between objective and subjective cognition, whereas metacognitive deficits are reflected by their decorrelation. Several studies have explored the association between objective and subjective cognition but reported inconsistent results. Some reported non-significant or weak (r < 0.3) associations [8,9], thus suggesting poor metacognitive performance in BD. In contrast, other studies reported a more pronounced association [10], which appeared to depend on how objective cognition was measured. A previous study indeed reported that the global self-concept of general cognitive ability did not correlate with the general cognitive composite score in BD, whereas self-reported performance just after completing a specific cognitive task correlated with the objective performance [11].
Several attempts have been made to identify the determinants of metacognition in BD. Some proposed computing a meta-sensitivity index as the difference between the individual ranks in objective and subjective cognition, and reported several factors that covaried with this index: mood symptoms, number of hospitalizations, type of BD, socio-occupational difficulties, perceived stress, quality of life, and verbal IQ [12]. Others subtracted the z-scores for objective cognition from the z-scores of subjective cognition. One study using this method found no clinical correlates of metacognition in BD [11], whereas another reported that metacognition was associated with objective cognitive performance [6]. However, the computation of a score reflecting a difference in ranks or z-scores between objective and subjective cognition makes it difficult to test whether a significant association between this score and a clinical correlate could be explained by a simpler and more direct relationship between this correlate and objective or subjective cognition.
An alternative strategy is to use moderation analysis, which avoids the limitation of using a difference between scores. One study reported that depressive symptoms did not moderate the association between subjective and objective cognition in euthymic or mildly depressed individuals with BD, thus suggesting a lack of association between depressive symptoms and metacognition in BD [9]. On the contrary, a later study reported that the association between subjective and objective cognition was also moderated by depressive symptoms only in individuals with depression and by manic symptoms only in individuals with hypomania/mania [13]. However, these two studies had a relatively limited sample size (n < 150), and investigated only mood symptoms as potential correlates of metacognition in BD, ignoring the potential role of important variables such as medication [14], psychosocial functioning [12] and childhood trauma [15].
Here, we aimed to identify potential clinical correlates of metacognition in a large sample of individuals with BD, beyond mood symptoms, using multiple moderation analysis between cognitive complaints and objective cognition.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Characteristics of the Recruiting Network

This monocenter, transversal study included patients recruited into the FACE-BD (FondaMental Advanced Centers of Expertise for Bipolar Disorders) cohort from the BD Expert Center of Versailles. The study was pre-registered (NCT04034147). The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human patients were approved by the local ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Ile de France IX) on 18 January 2010 under French laws for non-interventional studies (observational studies without any risk, constraint, or supplementary or unusual procedure concerning diagnosis, treatment, or monitoring). The board required that all patients be given an informational letter but waived the requirement for written informed consent. However, verbal consent was witnessed and formally recorded. Anonymized data are stored in a national database that was approved by the French body overseeing the safety of computerized databases (Commission Nationale de l’Informatique et des Liberte, DR-2011-069). No incentive was given to patients for their participation.

2.2. Participants

General practitioners or psychiatrists referred the patients who were assessed in the Centers of Expertise. The goal of this referral was to improve global care by delivering personalized care plans, after a systematic set of comprehensive assessment tools including a neuropsychological battery. Patients were interviewed by senior psychiatrists, psychologists, or neuropsychologists specialized in bipolar disorders, who were all members of the specialized multidisciplinary teams of the Expert Centres. Senior psychiatrists diagnosed bipolar disorders with the SCID-IV (Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR) [16]. Outpatients with type 1, type 2, or NOS (Not otherwise specified) BD, who were between 18 and 65 years of age, were eligible for this analysis. Patients who met the following criteria were excluded: history of neurological disorder, dyslexia, dysorthographia, dyscalculia, dysphasia, dyspraxia, substance-related disorders in the previous month (except tobacco use), or electroconvulsive therapy in the past year. No criteria related to the current mood state at inclusion were used to preserve the variability of levels of objective and subjective cognition. However, individuals whose symptom intensity was judged to be incompatible with the one-and-a-half-day evaluation were excluded (for instance, high suicidal risk, agitation, severe distractibility, disability to think or concentrate, or severe indecisiveness). Neuropsychologists screened exclusion criteria related to neurodevelopmental disorders; senior psychiatrists screened exclusion criteria related to neurological and substance-use disorders.

2.3. Assessment Tools

The sociodemographic variables collected at inclusion were sex, age, and education level.

2.3.1. Clinical Assessments

The following clinical variables were recorded using the SCID: age at onset of BD, number and type of previous mood episodes, the subtypes of BD, and history of psychotic symptoms. Predominant polarity was determined following previous recommendations [17].
The CGI-S (Clinical Global Impression-Severity) scale assessed illness severity [18]. We used a yes/no questionnaire for recording patient treatment at the time of evaluation: lithium carbonate, anticonvulsants, antipsychotics, antidepressants, or anxiolytics. Mania was measured using the YMRS (Young Mania Rating Scale) total score [19] and depression using the MADRS (Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale) total score [20]. The state of anxiety was measured using the total score of the state subscale of the STAI-Y-A (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, form Y-A) [21]. Impulsivity was assessed using the total score of the BIS-10 (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale version 10) [22]. Childhood traumatic events were recorded using the total score of the CTQ (Childhood Trauma Questionnaire) [23]. Domain-based psychosocial functioning was measured using the FAST (Functioning Assessment Short Test) [24]. In this study, the total FAST score was used (higher score meaning poorer functioning). Adherence to medication was measured using the total score of the MARS (Medication Adherence Rating Scale) [25]. According to previous reports, all these tools have good psychometric properties (see Supplementary Information S1) for a description of the internal consistency and test-retest reliability for each tool).

2.3.2. Objective and Subjective Cognition

  • Objective cognition: the battery of cognitive tests
The standardized test battery included 11 tests, amongst which five were subtests from the WAIS (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale) version III [26] or version IV [27]. Participants were included between June 2009 and December 2018. The French version of the WAIS-IV became available during this period, and we decided to start to use this version in May 2012. The rationale behind this decision was to use the most recent normative data available, as they gave a more accurate picture of the distance in cognitive performances between the recruited participants and the norm. The battery evaluated six domains:
-
Processing speed: Digit symbol coding (WAIS-III) or coding (WAIS-IV), WAIS symbol search, and TMT (Trail-Making Test) [28] part A
-
Verbal memory: California Verbal Learning Test [29] short and long delay free recall and total recognition
-
Attention: Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II [30] (detectability)
-
Working memory: WAIS digit span (total score) and spatial span (forward and backward scores) from the Wechsler Memory Scale version III [31]
-
Executive functions: color/word condition of the Stroop test [32], semantic and phonemic verbal fluency [33] and TMT part B
-
Verbal and perceptual reasoning: WAIS vocabulary and matrices
Raw scores were transformed to demographically corrected standardized z-scores based on normative data [30,32,34,35]. Higher scores reflected better performance. We computed a mean score for each cognitive domain. Then we computed a global score for objective cognition by averaging the cognitive domain scores. WAIS-III and WAIS-IV standardized scores were analysed together, as this method has been used in several cohort studies [36,37,38,39,40].
  • Subjective cognition: cognitive complaints
Cognitive complaints were assessed with item 10 of the QIDS-SR16 (Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology—Self-Report): “During the past seven days, there has been no change in my usual capacity to concentrate or make decisions” (scored 0); “I occasionally feel indecisive or find that my attention wanders” (scored 1); “Most of the time, I struggle to focus my attention or to make decisions” (scored 2); “I cannot concentrate well enough to read or cannot make even minor decisions” (scored 3). Previous studies have reported evidence of validity and reliability of this single-item assessment. Its test-retest reliability over six months is good (Cronbach alpha of 0.74) [41]. Concurrent validity for this item has been investigated against a similar item rated by a clinician within the clinical-rated form of QIDS, and it was found to be satisfactory [42]. Finally, we checked the concurrent validity of this item within our sample. We ran a correlation analysis between item 10 of QIDS and the cognitive functioning factor of FAST, which assesses the subjective level of cognitive functioning perceived by the clinician. We found a moderate-to-strong correlation (Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.48, t = 9.148, df = 273, p-value < 0.001), thus suggesting a good concurrent validity of the single-item assessment of cognitive complaints used in this study.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

First, missing data were estimated using multivariate imputations by chained equations (50 imputations, mice package of R). The fraction of missing information (fmi) and the proportion of total variance due to missingness (λ) are reported in the results.
Metacognition was quantified as the strength of the association between cognitive complaints and the average cognitive performance on the neuropsychological battery: a negative and significant association between cognitive complaints and objective cognition was interpreted as good metacognition, whereas a lack of an association or a positive association was interpreted as impaired metacognition. This association was operationalized through an ordinal logistic regression with cognitive complaints as the dependent variable and objective cognition as the independent variable. We first ran successive moderation analyses with cognitive complaints as the dependent variable and two independent variables (one clinical moderator belonging to the variables listed above and objective cognition) by declaring their main effects and their interaction in the model. Assuming that metacognition reflects the strength of the association between objective cognition and subjective complaints, a variable was interpreted as a potential moderator of metacognition if its interaction with objective cognition was significant.
We then ran a multiple ordinal logistic regression with several independent variables to check whether the potential clinical moderator identified in the simple logistic regressions remained significant while simultaneously accounting for all the effects. Beyond the main effect of objective cognition, the independent variables were included in this multiple model if:
-
the level of significance of their interaction with objective cognition was p < 0.25. This threshold is usual for selecting variables for multiple regression [43,44]. In this case, both the interaction term and the main effect of the clinical covariate was included in the model
-
the level of significance of their interaction with objective cognition was p ≥ 0.25 and the level of significance of their main effect was p < 0.25. In this case, only the main effect of the clinical covariate was included in the model

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of Participants

We included 281 participants. Socio-demographic, clinical, and functional characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1.
The participants consisted mostly of women with type 2 BD and indeterminate polarity of mood episodes. Most participants were not in an active characterized mood episode during the evaluation: only 13.9% were currently undergoing a major depressive episode. None were manic and only 1.4% were hypomanic. The average severity of illness was between moderate and marked (4.5 ± 0.9).
The MADRS correlated above |r| > 0.5 with the end of the last characterized episode <3 months, the presence of a current major depressive episode, and the state subscale of STAI-YA. These last three variables were thus discarded from the following analyses to avoid multicollinearity issues in the multiple regression. Due to the small sample size, the NOS diagnosis (n = 30) was combined with that of type 2 BD (its nearest neighbor, n = 146) and manic polarity (n = 22) with the indeterminate class (its nearest neighbor, n = 86) of predominant mood polarity.
The neuropsychological results are presented in Table 2.
WAIS-III was proposed to 36 participants, and WAIS-IV to 245 participants. The worst performance in objective cognition was found for attention (−0.4 ± 0.7) and the best for reasoning (0.6 ± 0.7).
For cognitive complaints, the distribution of answers to item 10 of the QIDS was the following (see Supplementary Figure S2): 31.8% for “No change in usual capacity to concentrate and decide“; 34.3% for “Occasionally feels indecisive or notes that attention often wanders”; 20.9% for “Most of the time struggles to focus attention or make decisions”; and 13.0% for “Cannot concentrate well enough to read or cannot make even minor decisions”. The mean cognitive complaints score was 1.2 (sd:1, n = 277), suggesting occasional subjective cognitive difficulties on average in this sample.
Imputation diagnostic is reported in Supplementary Information S3. Data were considered missing at random, as no systematic relationship between domains of missingness and participants’ socio-demographic characteristic were found.

3.2. Moderation Analyses

The bivariable ordinal logistic regression of cognitive complaints on objective cognition was not significant (OR = 0.70 (0.44–1.1), t(257.5) = −1.6, p = 0.113, λ = 0.051, fmi = 0.058). Overall, the study sample showed a non-significant association between objective and subjective cognition.

3.2.1. Trivariable Ordinal Logistic Regressions

The results of the ordinal logistic regressions of cognitive complaints with objective cognition and several successive clinical moderators as independent variables are presented in Table 3 for the interaction between objective cognition and clinical moderators, and Supplementary Tables S4 and S5 for the main effect of clinical moderators and objective cognition respectively.
Significant interactions with objective cognitive performance were found for predominant mood polarity and antipsychotic medication (see Table 3). The distribution of objective cognition according to the level of cognitive complaints and polarity in the observed dataset are reported in Supplementary Figure S6. The association between objective cognition and cognitive complaints was significant for depressive polarity (OR = 0.33 (0.13–0.80), t(66.6) = −2.5, p = 0.015, λ = 0.224, fmi = 0.246), but not indeterminate/manic polarity (OR = 1.02 (0.56–1.87), t(132.6) = 0.1, p = 0.947, λ = 0.179, fmi = 0.192). The distribution of objective cognition according to the level of cognitive complaints and the prescription of antipsychotics in the observed dataset are presented in Figure 1.
The association between objective cognition and cognitive complaints was significant for patients not taking antipsychotics (OR = 0.48 (0.28–0.78), t(215.8) = −2.9, p = 0.004, λ = 0.048, fmi = 0.057), but not for those taking antipsychotics (OR = 2.82 (0.73–10.8), t(36.5) = 1.6, p = 0.127, λ = 0.086, fmi = 0.132).
The clinical moderators that were significantly associated with more cognitive complaints in the absence of a significant interaction with objective cognition were a type 2/NOS BD (OR = 1.75 (1.11–2.76), t(269.6) = 2.4, p = 0.017), a lack of history of psychosis (OR = 1.80 (1.01–3.22), t(177.1) = −2, p = 0.047), greater illness severity measured based on the CGI-S (OR = 1.84 (1.38–2.44), t(267.9) = 4.2, p < 0.001), more severe depressive symptoms measured based on the MADRS (OR = 1.14 (1.1–1.17), t(265.6) = 8.4, p < 0.001), anxiolytic (OR = 2.12 (1.22–3.67), t(270.6) = 2.7, p = 0.008), a lifetime substance use disorder (OR = 1.7 (1.02–2.83), t(259.4) = 2, p = 0.043), greater impulsivity measured based on the BIS (OR = 1.08 (1.06–1.1), t(268.4) = 6.7, p < 0.001), an history of trauma assessed based on the CTQ (OR = 1.04 (1.02–1.05), t(261.2) = 4.7, p < 0.001), poorer functioning measured based on the FAST (OR = 1.08 (1.06–1.1), t(269.5) = 8.1, p < 0.001) and poorer medication adherence measured based on the MARS (OR = 0.87 (0.78–0.96), t(234.2) = −2.7, p = 0.008, see Supplementary Table S4).

3.2.2. Multiple Ordinal Logistic Regression

The multiple ordinal logistic regression included cognitive complaints as the dependent variable and the following independent variables: predominant mood polarity, CGI-S, lithium carbonate, antipsychotic, anxiolytic and FAST (their main effect and their interaction with objective cognition), and the main effect of objective cognition, educational level, type of BD, history of psychosis, MADRS, any lifetime substance use disorder, BIS, CTQ, MARS and type of WAIS (see Table 4).
The only interaction with objective cognition that remained significant in the multiple moderation analysis was found for antipsychotics (OR = 6 (1.13–31.7), t(216.9) = 2.1, p = 0.035). The variables which were significantly associated with increased cognitive complaints were MADRS (OR = 1.1 (1.06–1.14), t(240.5) = 5.2, p < 0.001) and BIS (OR = 1.07 (1.04–1.1), t(243.5) = 4.4, p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

We aimed to identify clinical correlates of metacognition in BD using moderation analysis between cognitive complaints and objective cognition.
First, this study confirmed the weak correlation between objective performance on a battery of neuropsychological tests and subjective perception of cognitive functioning reported in previous studies [8]. Among all investigated clinical variables, only the predominant mood polarity and the prescription of antipsychotics influenced the strength of the association between cognitive complaints and objective cognition. In the absence of covariates, the association between objective cognition and cognitive complaints was more robust for the depressive polarity than for the indeterminate/manic polarity. A negative impact of manic episodes on objective cognition has been reported previously [45], which may progressively lead to an impairment of metacognition. An alternative interpretation is that preserved metacognition facilitates the emergence of depression, whereas impaired metacognition may lead to disinhibition and mania. However, the moderating effect of polarity did not resist the introduction of covariates in the model.
The main factor influencing the strength of the association between cognitive complaints and objective cognition was the prescription of antipsychotics, for which the moderating effect remained significant in the multiple analysis. This result is compatible with the hypothesis that confidence in cognitive performance is modulated by dopamine [46]. More specifically, our results suggest that dopamine antagonists, such as antipsychotics, may decrease metacognitive accuracy. A previous study reported that dopamine administration increased metacognition in healthy participants, in parallel with increased amplitudes of MEG oscillations in the medial prefrontal cortex [47]. Another study recently demonstrated that the administration of haloperidol, a dopamine antagonist, impaired metamemory in healthy individuals in parallel with aberrant fMRI activity in frontostriatal circuits [14]. Of note, this effect may depend on the antipsychotic investigated, as it was not replicated with amisulpride [48]. This is the first study to report an association between antipsychotic medication and metacognition in BD. However, it is not possible to conclude a causal link of antipsychotics on metacognitive impairments, as we did not investigate the dose effect of antipsychotics on metacognition in this study. Longitudinal studies are needed to clarify the effect of antipsychotics on metacognition in BD and should account for the specific psychopharmacological properties of a particular antipsychotic, along with the daily dosage and serum level, duration of exposure, and therapeutic response. Despite these limitations, our result strongly encourages controlling for antipsychotic medication in studies investigating metacognition in severe and persistent psychiatric disorders.
Our results do not support a moderating effect of mood symptoms on the relationship between objective and subjective cognition in BD. This result is consistent with those of previous reports showing a lack of association between mood symptoms and metacognition in euthymic [9,11] and mildly depressed [6] individuals with BD. In contrast, mood symptoms were associated with a weaker relationship between cognitive complaints and objective cognitive performance in acutely ill individuals with BD [13]. This apparent discrepancy between studies can be explained by heterogeneity in the level of mood symptoms, which were low in our sample. Our results did not replicate the findings that hospitalizations and more significant socio-occupational difficulties correlate with metacognition, which were associated with disproportionately more subjective complaints than objective impairment in a previous report involving remitted individuals with BD [12].
The participants’ profile might have influenced some of our results, which may not generalize to all individuals with BD. First, there was a majority of BD 2 (52%) compared with BD 1 (37%). We did not find in the present study a significant association between the type of BD and metacognition. However, a previous study has reported that BD type II was associated with having more subjective complaints than objective impairment [12]. Thus, prevalence of BD type II in this sample might have biased recruiting toward individuals who underestimated their cognitive performance. Second, patients had very low manic symptoms. The majority of participants were euthymic or slightly depressed. Again, this might have biased the recruitment toward excluding individuals who overestimated their cognitive performance and lacked insight.
The multiple regression analysis also identified two robust correlates of cognitive complaints: depressive symptoms and impulsivity. Greater depressive symptoms have been consistently identified as important determinants of cognitive complaints in BD [8,9,13] along with less strong manic symptoms [13]. The association between impulsivity and cognitive complaints may be explained by symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [49], which were not investigated in our sample. Our results are consistent with those of a previous study reporting a lack of greater cognitive complaints by individuals with BD taking lithium or antipsychotic medication [10]. Our results did not replicate the findings that a higher number of episodes, especially the number of mixed episodes, longer duration of the illness and onset of the illness at an earlier age [50], and impairment in psychosocial functioning [51] are associated with more subjective complaints in BD.
The most important limitation was related to the assessment of subjective cognition, which was based only on one question extracted from a scale not specifically designated to measure cognitive complaints. Further studies should be conducted to replicate our results using a scale validated in BD, such as the Cognitive Complaints in Bipolar Disorder Rating Assessment [52]. The measure of objective cognition used in this study was able to identify performance above the norm, whereas the assessment of subjective cognition could not measure better self-reported performance, as cognitive complaints have a low boundary corresponding to normal performance. Moreover, the cognitive complaints question used in this study evaluated subjective cognitive performance relative to the usual capacity of the patient, whereas the measure of objective cognition referred to the distance to a norm of healthy subjects. Others have proposed using a different question to assess subjective cognition in order to resolve these two issues: “compared to healthy individuals of your age, your cognitive skills (concentration, memory, problem-solving, …) are profoundly below average, well below average, below average, average, above average, well above average, or superior” [6,11]. As a consequence, the way we measured subjective cognition in this study may have led to an underestimation of the strength of the association with objective cognition. Another important limitation was the investigation of global cognition only, without exploring the correlates of metacognition within each specific cognitive domain. This may be problematic, as metacognition varies by the domain of impairment, with patients being particularly unaware of attention and processing speed problems [12]. Further studies investigating metacognition in BD may benefit from less biased measures such as the ratio between meta-d’ and d [53]. The participants included in the present study were heterogeneous regarding their mood symptoms, mixing participants with a characterized depressive episode and euthymic participants. Further studies should control mood symptoms by recruiting separated subgroups with euthymic, hypomanic and depressed participants with equal sample size. Finally, grouping antipsychotics in just one class was an important limitation of the current study. Some antipsychotics are known to have a potential direct procognitive effect, such as lurasidone [54], and an indirect procognitive effect through an antidepressant action (ex: quetiapine, lurasidone).

5. Conclusions

This exploratory study investigated several clinical correlates of metacognition in BD using moderation analysis. The main factor influencing metacognition was the prescription of antipsychotics. The strength of the relationship between cognitive complaints and objective cognitive performance was weaker for individuals taking an antipsychotic. This result suggests a crucial role of dopamine in the modulation of metacognitive performance. Our study emphasizes the importance of controlling for antipsychotic medication when assessing metacognition in severe and persistent mental disorders, such as schizophrenia and BD, in future studies. This result also suggests not to rely on the sole self-report evaluation of cognitive functioning in patients with BD who are taking antipsychotics, and complete this evaluation with objective measures of cognitive performance. Depressive symptoms and impulsivity were associated with poorer subjective cognition and may be considered as potential therapeutic targets for individuals with BD and cognitive complaints. Our results may also guide future programs of metacognitive training in BD.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jcm10194349/s1, Supplementary Information S1: Test-retest reliability and internal consistency for the clinical scales; Supplementary Figure S2: Histogram of cognitive complaints, Supplementary Information S3: Diagnostic for imputation of missing data; Supplementary Table S4: Results for the trivariable ordinal logistic regressions with cognitive complaints as the dependent variable and objective cognition, several successive clinical moderators, and the interaction between objective cognition and the clinical moderator as independent variables. This table reports only the main effect of clinical moderators (the main effect of objective cognition and the interaction between objective cognition and the clinical moderator are reported in Supplementary Table S5 & Table 3 respectively); Supplementary Table S5: Results for the trivariable ordinal logistic regressions with cognitive complaints as the dependent variable and objective cognition, several successive clinical moderators, and the interaction between objective cognition and the clinical moderators as the independent variables. This table reports only the main effect of objective cognitive performance (the main effect of the clinical moderators and the interaction between objective cognition and the clinical moderators are reported in Supplementary Table S4 & Table 3 respectively); Supplementary Figure S6: Distribution of objective cognition according to the level of cognitive complaints in the observed dataset: (a) in individuals with predominant depressive polarity; (b) in individuals with predominant indeterminate/manic polarity.

Author Contributions

P.R., N.F., E.B.-G. & C.P. contributed substantially to conception and design of the study. P.R., N.F. & E.B.-G. contributed substantially to the analysis and interpretation of data. P.R., A.-S.C. & C.P. contributed substantially to acquisition of data. All authors drafted the article and revised it critically for important intellectual content. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by a grant from Centre Hospitalier de Versailles (Bourse Registre 2019), by the Fondation FondaMental, Créteil, France, and the Investissements d’Avenir Programs managed by the ANR under references ANR-11-IDEX-0004-02 and ANR-10-COHO-10-01.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human patients were approved by the local ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Ile de France IX) on 18 January 2010 under French laws for non-interventional studies (observational studies without any risk, constraint, or supplementary or unusual procedure concerning diagnosis, treatment, or monitoring).

Informed Consent Statement

The ethical committee required that all patients be given an informational letter but waived the requirement for written informed consent because it considered this study a non-interventional one. However, verbal consent was witnessed and formally recorded.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available as participants did not give their consent for sharing these data.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Fondation FondaMental, in particular the FACE-BD collaborators and data managers. We also thank the psychologists working at the Versailles Center of Expertise in Bipolar Disorders for gathering the data: Anne-Marie Galliot, Isabelle Grevin, and Géraldine Liaskovski. We thank the Centre Hospitalier de Versailles and William Hempel (Alex Edelman and Associates) for editorial assistance.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. David, A.S.; Bedford, N.; Wiffen, B.; Gilleen, J. Failures of Metacognition and Lack of Insight in Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 367, 1379–1390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Varga, M.; Magnusson, A.; Flekkøy, K.; Rønneberg, U.; Opjordsmoen, S. Insight, Symptoms and Neurocognition in Bipolar I Patients. J. Affect. Disord. 2006, 91, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Camelo, E.; Mograbi, D.C.; de Assis da Silva, R.; Santana, C.M.T.; Ferreira do Nascimento, R.L.; de Oliveira E Silva, A.C.; Nardi, A.E.; Cheniaux, E. Clinical and Cognitive Correlates of Insight in Bipolar Disorder. Psychiatr. Q. 2019, 90, 385–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Favaretto, E.; Bedani, F.; Offredi, A.; Schroffenegger, M.; Sassaroli, S.; Ruggiero, G.; Fagiolini, A.; Caselli, G. Metacognitions and Repetitive Negative Thinking in Bipolar Disorder and Healthy Controls: A Comparative Study. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 276, 152–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Østefjells, T.; Melle, I.; Aminoff, S.R.; Hellvin, T.; Hagen, R.; Lagerberg, T.V.; Lystad, J.U.; Røssberg, J.I. An Exploration of Metacognitive Beliefs and Thought Control Strategies in Bipolar Disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 2017, 73, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Van Camp, L.; Sabbe, B.G.C.; Oldenburg, J.F.E. Metacognitive Functioning in Bipolar Disorder versus Controls and Its Correlations with Neurocognitive Functioning in a Cross-Sectional Design. Compr. Psychiatry 2019, 92, 7–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Van Camp, L.; van den Ameele, S.; Sabbe, B.G.C.; Oldenburg, J.F.E. The Longitudinal Course of Cognitive Insight and Mood in Bipolar Disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 269, 9–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Demant, K.M.; Vinberg, M.; Kessing, L.V.; Miskowiak, K.W. Assessment of Subjective and Objective Cognitive Function in Bipolar Disorder: Correlations, Predictors and the Relation to Psychosocial Function. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 229, 565–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. van der Werf-Eldering, M.J.; Burger, H.; Jabben, N.; Holthausen, E.A.E.; Aleman, A.; Nolen, W.A. Is the Lack of Association between Cognitive Complaints and Objective Cognitive Functioning in Patients with Bipolar Disorder Moderated by Depressive Symptoms? J. Affect. Disord. 2011, 130, 306–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Jensen, J.H.; Støttrup, M.M.; Nayberg, E.; Knorr, U.; Ullum, H.; Purdon, S.E.; Kessing, L.V.; Miskowiak, K.W. Optimising Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Bipolar Disorder: Validation and Evaluation of Objective and Subjective Tools. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 187, 10–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Torres, I.J.; Mackala, S.A.; Kozicky, J.-M.; Yatham, L.N. Metacognitive Knowledge and Experience in Recently Diagnosed Patients with Bipolar Disorder. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2016, 38, 730–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Miskowiak, K.; Petersen, J.; Ott, C.; Knorr, U.; Kessing, L.; Gallagher, P.; Robinson, L. Predictors of the Discrepancy between Objective and Subjective Cognition in Bipolar Disorder: A Novel Methodology. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2016, 134, 511–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Lin, X.; Lu, D.; Huang, Z.; Chen, W.; Luo, X.; Zhu, Y. The Associations between Subjective and Objective Cognitive Functioning across Manic or Hypomanic, Depressed, and Euthymic States in Chinese Bipolar Patients. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 249, 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Clos, M.; Bunzeck, N.; Sommer, T. Dopamine Is a Double-Edged Sword: Dopaminergic Modulation Enhances Memory Retrieval Performance but Impairs Metacognition. Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2019, 44, 555–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Aydin, O.; Balikci, K.; Tas, C.; Aydin, P.U.; Danaci, A.E.; Brüne, M.; Lysaker, P.H. The Developmental Origins of Metacognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 245, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. First, M.B.; Spitzer, R.L.; Gibbon, M.; Williams, J.B. User’s Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders SCID-I: Clinician Version; American Psychiatric Pub: Washington, DC, USA, 1997; ISBN 0-88048-931-6. [Google Scholar]
  17. Mazzarini, L.; Pacchiarotti, I.; Colom, F.; Sani, G.; Kotzalidis, G.D.; Rosa, A.R.; Sanna, L.; De Rossi, P.; Girardi, N.; Bonnin, C.M.; et al. Predominant Polarity and Temperament in Bipolar and Unipolar Affective Disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 2009, 119, 28–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Guy, W. Clinical global impression scale. In The ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology; U.S. Dept. of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute of Mental Health, Psychopharmacology Research Branch, Division of Extramural Research Programs: Rockville, MD, USA, 1976; Volume 76, pp. 218–222. [Google Scholar]
  19. Young, R.C.; Biggs, J.T.; Ziegler, V.E.; Meyer, D.A. A Rating Scale for Mania: Reliability, Validity and Sensitivity. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 1978, 133, 429–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Montgomery, S.A.; Asberg, M. A New Depression Scale Designed to Be Sensitive to Change. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 1979, 134, 382–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Spielberger, C.D.; Sydeman, S.J. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory. In The Use of Psychological Testing for Treatment Planning and Outcome Assessment; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1994; pp. 292–321. [Google Scholar]
  22. Patton, J.H.; Stanford, M.S.; Barratt, E.S. Factor Structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995, 51, 768–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bernstein, D.; Fink, L. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A Retrospective Self-Report Manual [Manual]; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  24. Rosa, A.R.; Sanchez-Moreno, J.; Martinez-Aran, A.; Salamero, M.; Torrent, C.; Reinares, M.; Comes, M.; Colom, F.; Van Riel, W.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; et al. Validity and Reliability of the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) in Bipolar Disorder. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health CP EMH 2007, 3, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Thompson, K.; Kulkarni, J.; Sergejew, A.A. Reliability and Validity of a New Medication Adherence Rating Scale (MARS) for the Psychoses. Schizophr. Res. 2000, 42, 241–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Wechsler, D. WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: Administration and Scoring Manual; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  27. Wechsler, D.; Coalson, D.L.; Raiford, S.E. WAIS-IV: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; Pearson: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2008; ISBN 0-15-898084-0. [Google Scholar]
  28. Reitan, R.M. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Indicator of Organic Brain Damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 1958, 8, 271–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Delis, D.C. CVLT-II: California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Version; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2000; ISBN 0-15-803573-9. [Google Scholar]
  30. Conners, C.K.; Staff, M. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II; Multi-Health Syst. Inc.: North Tonawanda, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  31. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  32. Golden, C.J. A Manual for the Clinical and Experimental Use of the Stroop Color and Word Test; Stoelting: Chicago, IL, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  33. Lezak, M.D. Neuropsychological Assessment; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004; ISBN 0-19-511121-4. [Google Scholar]
  34. Godefroy, O. The GREFEX battery: Normative data. In Executive Functions and Neurological and Psychiatric Pathologies: Evaluation in Clinical Practice; Solal Editions: Marseille, France, 2008; p. 231. [Google Scholar]
  35. Poitrenaud, J.; Deweer, B.; Kalafat, M.; Van der Linden, M. French Adaptation of the California Verbal Learning Test; Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée: Paris, France, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  36. Ehrminger, M.; Brunet-Gouet, E.; Cannavo, A.-S.; Aouizerate, B.; Cussac, I.; Azorin, J.-M.; Bellivier, F.; Bougerol, T.; Courtet, P.; Dubertret, C.; et al. Longitudinal Relationships between Cognition and Functioning over 2 Years in Euthymic Patients with Bipolar Disorder: A Cross-Lagged Panel Model Approach with the FACE-BD Cohort. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 2019, 218, 80–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Roux, P.; Brunet-Gouet, E.; Ehrminger, M.; Aouizerate, B.; Aubin, V.; Azorin, J.M.; Bellivier, F.; Bougerol, T.; Courtet, P.; Dubertret, C.; et al. Minimum Clinically Important Differences for the Functioning Assessment Short Test and a Battery of Neuropsychological Tests in Bipolar Disorders: Results from the FACE-BD Cohort. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2020, 29, e144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Roux, P.; Urbach, M.; Fonteneau, S.; Berna, F.; Brunel, L.; Capdevielle, D.; Chereau, I.; Dubreucq, J.; Faget-Agius, C.; Fond, G.; et al. Psychiatric Disability as Mediator of the Neurocognition-Functioning Link in Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders: SEM Analysis Using the Evaluation of Cognitive Processes Involved in Disability in Schizophrenia (ECPDS) Scale. Schizophr. Res. 2018, 201, 196–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Roux, P.; Urbach, M.; Fonteneau, S.; Berna, F.; Brunel, L.; Capdevielle, D.; Chereau, I.; Dubreucq, J.; Faget-Agius, C.; Fond, G.; et al. Screening for Cognitive Deficits with the Evaluation of Cognitive Processes Involved in Disability in Schizophrenia Scale. Clin. Rehabil. 2019, 33, 113–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Brunet-Gouet, E.; Decaix-Tisserand, C.; Urbach, M.; Bazin, N.; Aouizerate, B.; Brunel, L.; Capdevielle, D.; Chereau, I.; Dubertret, C.; Dubreucq, J.; et al. Outcome Prediction with a Social Cognitive Battery: A Multicenter Longitudinal Study. NPJ Schizophr. 2021, 7, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Gili, M.; Lopez-Navarro, E.; Homar, C.; Castro, A.; García-Toro, M.; Llobera, J.; Roca, M. Psychometric Properties of Spanish Version of QIDS-SR16 in Depressive Patients. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 2014, 42, 292–299. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  42. Bernstein, I.H.; Rush, A.J.; Carmody, T.J.; Woo, A.; Trivedi, M.H. Clinical vs. Self-Report Versions of the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology in a Public Sector Sample. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2007, 41, 239–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Mickey, R.M.; Greenland, S. The Impact of Confounder Selection Criteria on Effect Estimation. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1989, 129, 125–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hosmer Jr, D.W.; Lemeshow, S.; Sturdivant, R.X. Applied Logistic Regression; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013; Volume 398, ISBN 0-470-58247-2. [Google Scholar]
  45. Bourne, C.; Aydemir, O.; Balanza-Martinez, V.; Bora, E.; Brissos, S.; Cavanagh, J.T.; Clark, L.; Cubukcuoglu, Z.; Dias, V.V.; Dittmann, S.; et al. Neuropsychological Testing of Cognitive Impairment in Euthymic Bipolar Disorder: An Individual Patient Data Meta-Analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2013, 128, 149–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lou, H.C.; Skewes, J.C.; Thomsen, K.R.; Overgaard, M.; Lau, H.C.; Mouridsen, K.; Roepstorff, A. Dopaminergic Stimulation Enhances Confidence and Accuracy in Seeing Rapidly Presented Words. J. Vis. 2011, 11, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Joensson, M.; Thomsen, K.R.; Andersen, L.M.; Gross, J.; Mouridsen, K.; Sandberg, K.; Østergaard, L.; Lou, H.C. Making Sense: Dopamine Activates Conscious Self-Monitoring through Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015, 36, 1866–1877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Hauser, T.U.; Allen, M.; Purg, N.; Moutoussis, M.; Rees, G.; Dolan, R.J. Noradrenaline Blockade Specifically Enhances Metacognitive Performance. eLife 2017, 6, e24901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Pierre, M.; Cogez, J.; Lebain, P.; Loisel, N.; Lalevée, C.; Bonnet, A.L.; De La Sayette, V.; Viader, F. Detection of Adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder with Cognitive Complaint: Experience of a French Memory Center. Rev. Neurol. 2019, 175, 358–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Martínez-Arán, A.; Vieta, E.; Colom, F.; Torrent, C.; Reinares, M.; Goikolea, J.M.; Benabarre, A.; Comes, M.; Sánchez-Moreno, J. Do Cognitive Complaints in Euthymic Bipolar Patients Reflect Objective Cognitive Impairment? Psychother. Psychosom. 2005, 74, 295–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Ott, C.; Miné, H.; Petersen, J.Z.; Miskowiak, K. Relation between Functional and Cognitive Impairments in Remitted Patients with Bipolar Disorder and Suggestions for Trials Targeting Cognition: An Exploratory Study. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 257, 382–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Rosa, A.R.; Mercade, C.; Sanchez-Moreno, J.; Sole, B.; Bonnin, C.D.M.; Torrent, C.; Grande, I.; Sugranyes, G.; Popovic, D.; Salamero, M. Validity and Reliability of a Rating Scale on Subjective Cognitive Deficits in Bipolar Disorder (COBRA). J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 150, 29–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Maniscalco, B.; Lau, H. A Signal Detection Theoretic Approach for Estimating Metacognitive Sensitivity from Confidence Ratings. Conscious. Cogn. 2012, 21, 422–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Yatham, L.N.; Mackala, S.; Basivireddy, J.; Ahn, S.; Walji, N.; Hu, C.; Lam, R.W.; Torres, I.J. Lurasidone versus Treatment as Usual for Cognitive Impairment in Euthymic Patients with Bipolar I Disorder: A Randomised, Open-Label, Pilot Study. Lancet Psychiatry 2017, 4, 208–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Distribution of objective cognition according to the level of cognitive complaints in the observed dataset: (a) in individuals with antipsychotic; (b) in individuals without antipsychotic. The width of the boxes is proportional to the sample size in each level of cognitive complaints.
Figure 1. Distribution of objective cognition according to the level of cognitive complaints in the observed dataset: (a) in individuals with antipsychotic; (b) in individuals without antipsychotic. The width of the boxes is proportional to the sample size in each level of cognitive complaints.
Jcm 10 04349 g001
Table 1. Participants’ socio-demographical, clinical, and functional characteristics.
Table 1. Participants’ socio-demographical, clinical, and functional characteristics.
VariableMean or %SDn
Age (years)40.211.1281
Sex42.3 (M) 281
Educational level (years)14.32.5281
Diagnosis: Type 137.4 281
Type 252
NOS 110.6
Total number of mood episodes8.17.5170
Predominant polarity: depressive36.5 170
Indeterminate50.6
Manic12.9
Age at onset (years)23.38.2271
History of psychosis19.8 232
Rapid cycling7.6 263
CGI 2 Severity (1–7)4.50.9279
Current major depressive episode13.9 281
Current hypomanic episode1.4 281
Current manic episode0 281
MADRS 3 (0–60)10.48.5281
YMRS 4 (0–60)2.23.4281
STAI-YA 5 (state subscale) (20–80)43.414.5278
End of last characterized episode > 3 months55.6 279
Antidepressant22.1 281
Anticonvulsant25.3 281
Lithium Carbonate13.5 281
Antipsychotic16.4 281
Anxiolytic19.2 281
Any lifetime substance use disorder26 273
BIS 6 (34–136)66.910.6279
CTQ 7 (28–140)43.715.1277
FAST 8 total (0–72)2013280
MARS 9 (0–10)6.82.2254
1 NOS: not otherwise specified; 2 CGI: Clinical Global Impression scale; 3 MADRS: Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale; 4 YMRS: Young Mania Rating Scale; 5 STAI-YA: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory YA form; 6 BIS: Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; 7 CTQ: Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; 8 FAST: Functioning Assessment Short Test; 9 MARS: Medication Adherence Rating Scale.
Table 2. Objective neuropsychological performances expressed in standard deviations from the norm.
Table 2. Objective neuropsychological performances expressed in standard deviations from the norm.
MeanSDn
Verbal Memory0.410.9
CVLT 1 Immediate recall0.581.26279
CVLT 1 Short delay free recall0.291.11279
CVLT 1 Long delay free recall0.371.09278
CVLT 1 Total recognition0.380.59278
Working Memory−0.140.7
Digit Span−0.240.86273
Spatial Span forward−0.080.88270
Spatial Span backward−0.120.85270
Executive Functioning−0.150.77
TMT 2 Part B−0.141.18279
Stroop color/word condition0.041.02267
Phonemic fluency−0.071.05278
Semantic fluency−0.420.96278
Processing speed−0.110.67
Coding−0.20.92271
Symbol search0.010.88270
Stroop word condition−0.080.79268
Stroop color condition−0.450.85267
TMT 2 Part B0.180.96280
Attention−0.380.67
CPT 3 omission−0.841.23266
CPT 3 commission−0.151.06266
CPT 3 variability−0.291.1266
CPT 3 detectability−0.230.98266
Reasoning0.560.71
Vocabulary0.830.87251
Matrices0.340.79270
1 CVLT: California Verbal Learning Test; 2 TMT: Trail Making Test; 3 CPT: Continuous Performance Test.
Table 3. Results for the trivariable ordinal logistic regressions with cognitive complaints as the dependent variable and objective cognition, several successive clinical moderators, and the interaction between objective cognition and the clinical moderators as independent variables. This table reports only the interaction effect between objective cognition and the clinical moderator (the main effect of objective cognition and clinical moderator are reported in Supplementary Tables S4 and S5).
Table 3. Results for the trivariable ordinal logistic regressions with cognitive complaints as the dependent variable and objective cognition, several successive clinical moderators, and the interaction between objective cognition and the clinical moderators as independent variables. This table reports only the interaction effect between objective cognition and the clinical moderator (the main effect of objective cognition and clinical moderator are reported in Supplementary Tables S4 and S5).
Interaction between the Mean Cognitive Performance and a Clinical ModeratorOR (95% CI) 1Statisticpλfmi
Age0.98 (0.94–1.02)t(250.5) = −0.90.3920.0630.071
Sex1 (0.41–2.45)t(258.3) = 00.9920.0440.051
Educational level1.03 (0.86–1.24)t(242.9) = 0.30.7340.0810.088
Diagnosis (Type 2/NOS 2 vs. Type 1)0.83 (0.31–2.19)t(245) = −0.40.7060.0760.083
Total number of mood episodes0.98 (0.89–1.07)t(72.7) = −0.50.5990.5360.548
Predominant Polarity (Indeterminate/Manic vs. Depressive)3.24 (1.02–10.34)t(128.4) = 20.0470.3350.345
Age at onset1 (0.94–1.06)t(251.1) = −0.10.8960.0620.069
History of psychosis1.14 (0.32–4.02)t(159.2) = 0.20.8380.2570.266
Rapid cycling1.33 (0.25–6.93)t(211.8) = 0.30.7380.1460.154
CGI 3 Severity1.61 (0.88–2.94)t(241.6) = 1.50.1240.0840.091
MADRS 41.01 (0.95–1.07)t(252.2) = 0.20.8260.0590.067
YMRS 51.02 (0.89–1.17)t(239.9) = 0.30.7580.0870.095
Antidepressant0.82 (0.28–2.45)t(251.5) = −0.40.7240.0610.068
Anticonvulsant1.05 (0.31–3.63)t(229.8) = 0.10.9330.1090.116
Lithium Carbonate0.45 (0.12–1.69)t(257.5) = −1.20.2330.0460.054
Antipsychotic6.87 (1.64–28.76)t(240) = 2.70.0090.0870.094
Anxiolytic0.49 (0.17–1.47)t(247.2) = −1.30.2050.0710.078
Any lifetime substance use disorder1.15 (0.4–3.33)t(234.1) = 0.30.7930.10.107
BIS 61.02 (0.98–1.07)t(249.4) = 10.3090.0660.073
CTQ 70.99 (0.96–1.03)t(229.7) = −0.50.6170.1090.117
FAST 81.04 (1–1.08)t(246.6) = 1.90.0640.0720.08
MARS 91.09 (0.89–1.33)t(242.8) = 0.80.3970.0810.088
Type of WAIS 100.92 (0.23–3.7)t(192.2) = −0.10.9080.1860.194
1 Odds Ratio [95% Confidence Interval]; 2 NOS: not otherwise specified; 3 CGI: Clinical Global Impression scale; 4 MADRS: Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale; 5 YMRS: Young Mania Rating Scale; 6 BIS: Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; 7 CTQ: Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; 8 FAST: Functioning Assessment Short Test; 9 MARS: Medication Adherence Rating Scale; 10 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.
Table 4. Results for the multiple moderation analysis with ordinal logistic regression, including cognitive complaints as the dependent variable.
Table 4. Results for the multiple moderation analysis with ordinal logistic regression, including cognitive complaints as the dependent variable.
Independant VariableOR (95% CI) 1Statisticpλfmi
Predominant Polarity (Indeterminate/Manic vs. Depressive)1.17 (0.6–2.29)t(116) = 0.50.6460.3530.364
CGI Severity1.37 (0.96–1.96)t(236.4) = 1.70.0830.0550.063
Lithium carbonate1.42 (0.66–3.06)t(248.9) = 0.90.3660.0190.026
Antipsychotic1.4 (0.63–3.1)t(240.5) = 0.80.4080.0440.052
Anxiolytic1.89 (1–3.57)t(248.7) = 20.0510.0190.027
FAST1.02 (1–1.05)t(245.8) = 1.90.0580.0280.036
Objective cognition0.05 (0–1.58)t(213.8) = −1.70.090.1110.119
Educational level0.97 (0.88–1.08)t(247.5) = −0.50.5960.0230.031
Diagnosis (Type 2/NOS 2 vs. Type 1)1.41 (0.67–2.98)t(187.3) = 0.90.3630.170.179
History of psychosis1.22 (0.49–3.02)t(121.2) = 0.40.6640.3370.348
MADRS31.1 (1.06–1.14)t(240.5) = 5.2<0.0010.0440.052
Any lifetime substance use disorder0.81 (0.45–1.45)t(231.2) = −0.70.480.0690.077
BIS41.07 (1.04–1.1)t(243.5) = 4.4<0.0010.0350.043
CTQ 51.02 (1–1.03)t(245.2) = 1.80.0670.030.038
MARS 60.93 (0.82–1.06)t(196.3) = −1.10.2640.150.159
Type of WAIS 7 (IV vs. III)0.75 (0.36–1.59)t(246.1) = −0.70.4580.0270.035
Objective cognition: Polarity (Indeterminate/Manic vs. Depressive)1.42 (0.37–5.47)t(126.5) = 0.50.6050.3210.332
Objective cognition: CGI 81.52 (0.73–3.17)t(228.2) = 1.10.2650.0760.084
Objective cognition: Lithium carbonate0.49 (0.09–2.67)t(221.4) = −0.80.4050.0930.101
Objective cognition: Antipsychotic6 (1.13–31.7)t(216.9) = 2.10.0350.1040.112
Objective cognition: Anxiolytic0.52 (0.13–2.13)t(209.5) = −0.90.360.1210.129
Objective cognition: FAST 91.03 (0.98–1.08)t(224.6) = 1.30.1990.0850.093
1 Odds Ratio [95% Confidence Interval]; 2 NOS: not otherwise specified; 3 MADRS: Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale; 4 BIS: Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; 5 CTQ: Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; 6 MARS: Medication Adherence Rating Scale; 7 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; 8 CGI: Clinical Global Impression scale; 9 FAST: Functioning Assessment Short Test. Significant results (p < 0.005) are in bold.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Roux, P.; Faivre, N.; Cannavo, A.-S.; Brunet-Gouet, E.; Passerieux, C. Exploring Clinical Correlates of Metacognition in Bipolar Disorders Using Moderation Analyses: The Role of Antipsychotics. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 4349. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10194349

AMA Style

Roux P, Faivre N, Cannavo A-S, Brunet-Gouet E, Passerieux C. Exploring Clinical Correlates of Metacognition in Bipolar Disorders Using Moderation Analyses: The Role of Antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2021; 10(19):4349. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10194349

Chicago/Turabian Style

Roux, Paul, Nathan Faivre, Anne-Sophie Cannavo, Eric Brunet-Gouet, and Christine Passerieux. 2021. "Exploring Clinical Correlates of Metacognition in Bipolar Disorders Using Moderation Analyses: The Role of Antipsychotics" Journal of Clinical Medicine 10, no. 19: 4349. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10194349

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop