Next Article in Journal
Maturational Changes in Prefrontal and Amygdala Circuits in Adolescence: Implications for Understanding Fear Inhibition during a Vulnerable Period of Development
Next Article in Special Issue
Target Size Manipulations Affect Error-Processing Duration and Success Perceptions but not Behavioural Indices of Learning
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Paired Associative Stimulation on Corticomotor Excitability in Chronic Smokers
Previous Article in Special Issue
Corticospinal Excitability to the Biceps Brachii is Not Different When Arm Cycling at a Self-Selected or Fixed Cadence
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Interactive Effect of Tonic Pain and Motor Learning on Corticospinal Excitability

University of Ontario Institute of Technology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Oshawa, ON L1G 0C5, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2019, 9(3), 63; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030063
Submission received: 19 February 2019 / Revised: 28 February 2019 / Accepted: 13 March 2019 / Published: 16 March 2019

Abstract

:
Prior work showed differential alterations in early somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) and improved motor learning while in acute tonic pain. The aim of the current study was to determine the interactive effect of acute tonic pain and early motor learning on corticospinal excitability as measured by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Two groups of twelve participants (n = 24) were randomly assigned to a control (inert lotion) or capsaicin (capsaicin cream) group. TMS input–output (IO) curves were performed at baseline, post-application, and following motor learning acquisition. Following the application of the creams, participants in both groups completed a motor tracing task (pre-test and an acquisition test) followed by a retention test (completed without capsaicin) within 24–48 h. Following an acquisition phase, there was a significant increase in the slope of the TMS IO curves for the control group (p < 0.05), and no significant change for the capsaicin group (p = 0.57). Both groups improved in accuracy following an acquisition phase (p < 0.001). The capsaicin group outperformed the control group at pre-test (p < 0.005), following an acquisition phase (p < 0.005), and following a retention test (p < 0.005). When data was normalized to the pre-test values, the learning effects were similar for both groups post-acquisition and at retention (p < 0.005), with no interactive effect of group. The acute tonic pain in this study was shown to negate the increase in IO slope observed for the control group despite the fact that motor performance improved similarly to the control group following acquisition and retention. This study highlights the need to better understand the implications of neural changes accompanying early motor learning, particularly while in pain.

1. Introduction

There is evidence that pain negatively impacts the neuroplasticity associated with motor output [1,2] and negates the increases in somatosensory evoked potential (SEP) peaks that would otherwise occur following early motor learning [3,4]. Motor learning is the acquisition of new muscle patterns in order to improve motor performance [5]. Motor learning induces neuroplasticity in the motor cortex (M1): An expansion of motor areas [6,7,8,9] and changes in the kinematics of movements and facilitation of the motor evoked potentials (MEPs) [10,11]. The interactions between pain and motor learning are complicated and few experiments have examined the interactive effect of acute tonic pain and early motor learning on corticospinal excitability (CSE) in healthy humans. In addition, while there is some evidence that pain during motor learning interferes with acquisition [12,13,14], other work has shown no impact of pain on acquisition [15,16,17] and improved acquisition in the presence of acute pain [3,4]. Previous work using a tonic cutaneous pain model demonstrated improved early motor learning with remote acute pain (applied to the same limb) as compared to a control group [3,4] and with local pain as compared to remote pain [4]. Therefore, as the neuroplasticity associated with early motor learning is mediated by alterations in attention [18,19,20,21], these differing outcomes may be attributed to the location (local versus remote) as local pain may help to focus attention on the effector performing the task leading to enhanced performance. In addition, as manipulating arousal through pharmacological means has been shown to improve performance [22], the increase in arousal associated with acute tonic may have a positive impact on motor performance. Another factor may be the type of nociceptive input (muscle versus cutaneous). Experimental muscle pain (induced by the injection of hypertonic saline) tends to increase with movement in certain directions, leading to altered motor control strategies in an effort to avoid the pain [1]. In contrast, cutaneous pain does not increase with movement and therefore does not interfere with motor learning. An acute tonic pain model (capsaicin cream) that does not lead to increased pain with movement was chosen for this study in order to elucidate some of the differences between the type of pain (cutaneous versus muscular) and early motor learning outcomes. The topical application of capsaicin cream is a widely used experimental pain model [23,24,25] that elicits activation in C-nociceptors inducing central sensitization and a region of hyperalgesia [26,27].
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has been utilized to study alterations in CSE that occurs with acute and chronic pain as well as following motor learning. TMS studies of patients in chronic pain generally demonstrate decreased M1 excitability [28,29] although increased M1 excitability has been found in patients suffering from phantom limb pain [29]. In terms of acute pain, research demonstrates that experimental muscle pain modulates neuromuscular control through differential activation of muscle groups [30,31,32,33]. Although there are divergent effects of acute pain on M1 excitability as it increases excitability under certain circumstances [34], the preponderance of the literature demonstrates that there is generally a reduction in excitability [35,36,37]. Acute pain leads to alterations in excitability for differing muscles, and these changes in excitability seem to be dependent on which muscles need to be activated or inhibited to produce a protective motor strategy [1]. While acute pain generally reduces excitability, the alterations in M1 excitability following improved motor performance vary considerably across different studies. This may depend on the number of trials during motor learning as there is increased excitability of the M1 in the early stage of motor learning [38,39]. The early learning stage occurs in which there is a within-session improvement induced by a few trials on a time scale of minutes [40]. Following this early (fast) learning, there is slow increase in performance gains, and this is referred to as slow learning [40]. This phase in motor learning is a result of the consolidation of experience dependent changes in the cortex, triggered by learning. There is also a body of literature that has examined the interactive effect of pain and motor learning on somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) which can be used to assess the areas of the brain that are involved in sensorimotor integration [3,4,41,42]. Following local, remote, and contralateral cutaneous pain, there are decreases in an early SEP peak (P25) and differential changes in cortical SEP peaks for a control group following motor learning acquisition [42]. These findings were not observed for the capsaicin group, suggesting that acute pain may have negated alterations in SEP peaks that would have otherwise ensued following pain-free motor learning acquisition [42].
The literature demonstrates that there are conflicting findings in terms of the interactive effect of acute pain and motor learning on CSE. Previous studies have suggested that pain may interfere with learning-induced neuroplasticity [14]; however, other research findings demonstrated that pain improves motor learning [3] or has no effect when efforts were taken to ensure that the pattern of movement was not altered [16,43]. Research investigating the effect of local pain on motor learning found that following motor learning there were increases in MEPs for the control groups, with a lack of change in MEPs for the local acute pain groups [13,16]. It was hypothesized that local pain negatively impacts the CSE increases that occur with motor learning [13,16]. Ingham and Tucker [43] extended this research by examining how the location of the painful stimulus impacts CSE and found that there were no changes in MEPs for either group (control or hypertonic pain, both local or remote) following a finger adduction task. However, following motor learning, they did show that there was a reduction in TMS-evoked finger movement in the abduction direction for both the control and local pain condition, with no alterations following remote pain. This suggests that the location of pain may impact the CSE associated with motor learning. Furthermore, Mavromatis [44] examined the interactive effect of locally applied capsaicin cream and a motor task on corticospinal excitability. This study utilized short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) and found that the control group had an increase in excitability that was not observed for the capsaicin group but did not find a negative impact of local pain on motor learning outcomes. TMS input–output (IO) curves provide an opportunity to study CSE across a range of stimulus intensities [45]. When examining the IO curve, the slope of the linear component represents CSE [46] and therefore may provide a more robust way to explore the interactive effect of motor learning and pain on CSE than changes in MEP amplitude at a single stimulation intensity.
Our current study examines the effect of remote pain (capsaicin applied to the elbow) on the hand performing the task, and is therefore a distinct extension of previous research that has investigated the interactive effect of local pain and motor learning on CSE [14,17,44]. This paradigm allows us to examine the interactive effect of motor learning and acute pain on CSE. These findings on healthy individuals can then be contrasted with those suffering from chronic pain or other neurodegenerative disorders in order to understand the neurophysiological mechanisms at work. In addition, the application of capsaicin to the elbow is a model for lateral epicondylitis and thus the effect of elbow pain on motor performance is of relevance to ergonomics. This study provides an appealing model to investigate the interaction of pain and motor learning and it is also complimentary to our previous work, which found that remote cutaneous pain negated the somatosensory evoked potential (SEP) peaks that occurred in a control (non-capsaicin) group following motor learning [3,4].
A challenge with motor learning paradigms is that every repetition of the task, including repeated baseline testing, leads to a potential learning effect. Given that our primary interest was the effect of pain on motor learning, as distinct from the effect of pain itself on performance, and that our past work [41] has shown that pain improves performance, we opted to test baseline performance in the presence of pain. This enabled us to measure the effect of pain on motor learning as distinct from the impact of pain itself. In line with our previous work [4,41], we expected that the group which performed their novel motor acquisition task in the presence of acute tonic pain would show improved accuracy as compared to a control group.
Our primary hypothesis was that a motor acquisition task performed in a pain-free condition (controls) as compared to acute tonic pain (capsaicin group) would show an increase in the mean slope of the TMS IO curves. The results of this study will contribute to our understanding of how acute pain impacts CSE in response to novel motor acquisition, as well as provide insight on the relationship between excitability changes and the impact on motor skill retention when acquired during acute tonic pain.

2. Methods

2.1. Methods Overview

Two groups of twelve participants (6 males, 18 females; aged (M 20.2 1.31 SD)) were volunteers recruited from the student population at the University of Ontario Institute of Technology. Participants completed a confidential health history form to rule out any conditions which might impact normal somatosensation and/or motor function. This included recent cervicothoracic injury, chronic pain, neurologic conditions, and medication use. Participants self-reported their handedness in order to determine where the topical cream would be applied. This study was approved by the University of Ontario Institute of Technology Research Ethics Board and each participant gave both verbal and written informed consent. This study was performed according to the principles set out by the Declaration of Helsinki for the use of humans in experimental research.
The effect of acute tonic pain on CSE was assessed by performing TMS IO curves at baseline, at 20 min post-application, and then following the acquisition phase (45 min from baseline) (See Figure 1 for a schematic of the protocol). Participants in the capsaicin group received a topical application of capsaicin (0.075% Zostrix, New York, NY, USA) while the control group received a topical control skin lotion (Life Brand, Shopper’s Drug Mart, ON, Canada). The topical creams were applied to a 50 cm2 area on the lateral aspect of the dominant elbow.

2.2. Outcome Measures

The outcome measures for this study included pain (numeric pain rating scale (NPRS)), mean motor error on the motor task (%), and the slope of the IO curves.

2.2.1. Pain

Pain was measured using an NPRS in which participants classified their pain intensity from 0–10 [47]. Participants in both groups rated their pain at baseline, post-application (5 min), post-application (20 min), following the acquisition phase (35 min), and following the last round of TMS measurements (45 min).

2.2.2. Motor Learning Task

A custom Leap Motion software tool (Leap Motion, Inc., San Francisco, CA, USA) was used to design the tracing task. Participants were asked to use their dominant thumb to trace sequences of sinusoidal waves with varying amplitude and frequency on an external touchpad (Logitech, Inc., Fremont, CA, USA). The trace moved vertically down a computer screen passing a horizontal line where the participants were attempting to follow the trace as accurately as possible using their dominant thumb. As described previously, participants were given visual feedback for each dot in the trace, with green indicating they were on the optimal trace and yellow indicating that they were further away [42]. There was a pre-test, an acquisition phase which took approximately 15 min, a post-acquisition test, and a retention test 24–48 h later. The pre-test, post-acquisition test, and retention tests all took 4 min to complete and consisted of four previously selected sinusoidal patterns of varying frequency and amplitude, as determined previously (See Figure 2) [48]. The traces consisted of a series of dots and each trial consisted of 500 dots. The acquisition phase consisted of a total of twelve traces as each version of the sinusoidal pattern was performed three times. The participants used the abductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscle as they were required to sweep their dominant thumb from right to left. As previously described [41], the software calculated the distance that the participant’s cursor dot was from the optimal trace by recording the mean distance the participant’s cursor was from each dot. Mean motor error expressed the percentage that the participant’s trace was from the original optimal trace, with larger numbers indicating more motor error and therefore reduced performance of the motor task.

2.2.3. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Focal TMS was performed over the hand region of the dominant M1 using a figure-eight coil (outer diameter 10 cm) linked to two Magstim 200 stimulators connected with a BiStim unit (Magstim Co., Whitland, Dyfed, UK). The coil was positioned rotated approximately 45 degrees away from the mid-sagittal line with the handle pointed backwards. This orientation induced the current perpendicular to the central sulcus, and therefore the TMS coil stimulated corticospinal neurons trans-synaptically [49,50]. The optimal coil position for inducing MEPs in the APB muscle was established as the site where stimulation at a suprathreshold level generated the largest MEPs (after averaging ten stimuli). The site was marked on a cap with a marker in order to confirm that the coil was correctly placed throughout the study. The location of the cap was checked throughout the experiment to ensure that it hadn’t moved. Resting motor threshold (rMT) was found utilizing the guidelines set forth by Rossini et al. [51], and thus we utilized the lowest stimulator intensity that in five out of ten sessions evoked a MEP of at least 0.05 mV, while the participant was at rest.

2.2.4. Electromyography Recording

Surface electromyography (EMG) recordings were obtained from the APB muscle with surface Ag–AgCl electrodes. The electrodes were positioned over the APB, while the reference electrode was located over the lateral epicondyle of the same limb (there was no overlap between the application site of the inert lotion/capsaicin and the reference site). We requested that participants maintain a relaxed position throughout the study and monitored their level of muscle contraction using continuous visual EMG feedback. The recorded EMG signal was amplified, band-pass filtered (1000×), and digitized.

2.2.5. IO Curves

The intensities used to develop the TMS IO curves were established for each participant using their rMT attained at the start of the experiment (prior to application of the capsaicin lotion/inert lotion) (See Table 1). As pain intensity changed throughout the experiment, it was important to collect recruitment curve data as efficiently as possible. Therefore, in keeping with previous research [46,48], we utilized magnetic stimuli between 90 and 140% of rMT (in 10% increments), as this range encompassed the linear portion of the curve for the majority of participants. Twelve stimuli were given at each stimulus intensity with the interstimulus interval set at 5 s. Therefore, a single IO curve session consisted of 72 stimuli.

2.3. Data Analysis

For the NPRS measurements, a repeated measures ANOVA with factors TIME (baseline, post-application (5 min), post-application (20 min), post-acquisition test (35 min), post-acquisition test (45 min)) and GROUP (control versus capsaicin) was performed.
Mauchly’s test of sphericity and the Shapiro–Wilk test for normality were run on the accuracy data (mean motor error). To compare accuracy between groups, a repeated measures ANOVA with factors TIME (pre-test versus post-acquisition test versus retention test) and GROUP (control versus capsaicin) was performed on the accuracy data with post-hoc ANOVA testing with a Bonferroni correction if there were significant main effects. Given that two past studies [41,42] showed that capsaicin leads to better motor performance, it was possible that there would be baseline performance differences between the two groups. Therefore, the accuracy data was also normalized to each individual’s pre-test accuracy data, and a repeated measures ANOVA with factors TIME (pre-test versus post-acquisition test versus retention) and GROUP (control versus capsaicin) was performed on the normalized data, in order to enable comparison of the relative improvements in performance between groups.
MEP amplitude measurements were taken peak-to-peak and averaged for each intensity. This file was exported to Microsoft Excel 2010 (version 16.5), and we took the average of the 12 stimuli for every intensity and graphed these results. The slope of the linear component of the IO curve was calculated and exported to IBM SPSS Statistics. R2 values were calculated to illustrate how well the slope fit the linear portion of the IO curve. The plateau phase was excluded for participants that had levelling off at the lower intensity (90% rMT), and thus only the slope of the curve from the 100% to 140% intensity was included in the final analysis for all of the participants. MEP amplitudes were normalized to baseline values to account for variability between participants at baseline and to allow for between participant comparisons. Mauchly’s test of sphericity and the Shapiro–Wilk test for normality were run on the slope data. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 19.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) was utilized for statistical analysis. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. To explore the interactive effect of acute tonic pain and the motor task on the IO slopes, a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with factors TIME (baseline, post-application, post-acquisition test) and GROUP (control versus capsaicin) was performed with post hoc ANOVA tests with a Bonferroni correction if a significant main effect was found.

3. Results

There was a total of 24 participants, with 12 participants in the capsaicin group (9 females, 3 males; aged 19–22 (M 19.9 SD 0.9)) and 12 participants in the control group (9 females, 3 males; aged 19–23 (M 20.7 SD 1.4)). Participants were stratified by sex to ensure equal numbers of males and females in each group.

3.1. Pain Ratings

There were significant differences in subjective pain ratings as compared to baseline for the capsaicin group 5 min post-application (F(1,11) = 54.55, p < 0.001), 20 min post-application (F(1,11) = 286.00, p < 0.001), post-acquisition test (35 min mark) (F(1, 11) = 11.64, p < 0.01), and post-acquisition test (45 min mark) (F(1,11) = 7.05, p < 0.05). The average NPRS ratings are shown in Figure 3. The participants in the control group did not report any pain.

3.2. Behavioural Data

For all of the accuracy data, Mauchly’s test of sphericity was not significant. The Shapiro–Wilk normality test indicated that the accuracy data was normally distributed for both groups, and thus an ANOVA was performed. The behavioural data demonstrated that motor learning occurred as both the control (F(1,11) = 59.93, p < 0.001) and capsaicin (F(1,11) = 23.16, p < 0.001) groups showed decreases in error post-acquisition and following the retention test (from pre-test). The interaction effect of TIME by GROUP was significant (F(2,23) = 3.23, p < 0.05), with post-hoc ANOVA testing with a Bonferroni correction demonstrating that pre-test (F(1,11) = 18.88, p < 0.005), post-acquisition test (F(1,11) = 15.32, p < 0.005), and following the retention test (F(1,11) = 17.04, p < 0.005), the capsaicin group was more accurate than the control group (See Figure 4). Because the pre-test performance was different between groups, the performance data was normalized to each participant’s pre-test data before running the ANOVA. This confirmed that motor learning occurred as there was a significant effect of TIME (F(2,23) = 93.27, p < 0.005) with significant differences post-acquisition (F(1,11) = 61.21, p < 0.005) and at retention (F(1,11) = 192.81, p < 0.005) (see Figure 4). The effect of TIME by GROUP was not significant (p = 0.55), indicating that there was no difference in degree of improvement between groups when the baseline differences were taken into account. Relative to the pre-test values, the control group had a 48.7% decrease in mean motor error post motor acquisition and a subsequent 21.9% decrease at retention. The capsaicin group had a 35.2% decrease in mean motor error following motor acquisition and a subsequent 10.7% decrease at retention. The average time from the post-acquisition test to the retention test was 29.3 h (SD 5.6) for the control group and 34.5 h (SD 9.9) for the capsaicin group.

3.3. Transcranial IO Curves

The slope data was normally distributed, and Mauchly’s test of sphericity was not significant. The average R2 value for the capsaicin group was 0.87 and for the control group was 0.84 (see Table 2). There was no effect of TIME on the IO slopes (p = 0.16) following cream application (capsaicin or placebo) or post motor acquisition. However, there was a significant TIME by GROUP interaction (F(2,23) = 3.69, p < 0.05), with post-hoc tests with a Bonferroni correction indicating that following motor acquisition there was a significant increase in slope for the control group (F(1,11) = 3.49, p < 0.05), and no significant change for the capsaicin group (p = 0.59). Figure 5 shows the average MEP data for a representative control participant at all of the stimulation intensities. The average IO slopes are illustrated in Figure 6. Figure 7 depicts example individual IO curves of typical control (A) and capsaicin (B) participants and Figure 8 depicts the mean IO curves for control (A) and capsaicin (B) participants.

4. Discussion

Our findings support our hypothesis that a motor task performed in a pain-free condition (controls) as compared to acute tonic pain (capsaicin group) would show an increase in the slope of the TMS IO curves.
Improvements in accuracy were found for both groups (during post-acquisition and retention tests), demonstrating that motor learning had occurred. The normalized post-acquisition/retention data demonstrated that both groups showed similar improvement in accuracy following acquisition and at retention. Pain is likely to have impacted motor performance throughout the study as the capsaicin group outperformed the control group pre-test, post-acquisition test, and following the retention test, corroborating our previous research [3,4]. Previous research demonstrated improved early motor learning with acute tonic pain, and it was hypothesized that this was due to increased attention to the limb performing the task as improvements in performance were seen with remote pain (applied to the same limb) as compared to a control condition [3,4] and with local pain as compared to remote pain [4]. Other work has demonstrated that acute experimental pain did not negatively impact acquisition and retention [15]. The acute tonic pain stimulus (capsaicin) used in the current study and our previous work [3,4] is similar to that used in the Bilodeau study [15]. This tonic cutaneous pain was unrelated to the motor task, and thus it is hypothesized that pain may only have a negative impact on motor performance when it directly impacts the ability to perform a motor task.
Our results demonstrated an increase in the slope of the mean IO curve for the control group following motor learning which is in line with the theory that there is an increase in excitability in the M1 that complements motor learning. However, there was no change in the mean slope of the IO for the capsaicin group. This suggests that, although an increase in the excitability of the M1 often accompanies improved motor performance, it is not essential for improvements to occur as both groups demonstrated motor learning acquisition and retention. These results can also be interpreted from a cognitive perspective. Increased excitability of the M1 accompanies cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease, and this may be compensatory following the loss of neurons that occurs with this disease [52]. Our current findings substantiate our previous work [42] in which we found differential SEP peak amplitudes (N18) for participants who performed motor acquisition with capsaicin applied to the elbow similar to this study (remote pain, as compared to contralateral and local pain groups), but with no differences in performance accuracy between groups. These results are in line with Cirillo [53], who also found that there was no association between MEP changes and improved motor performance. Other work has found that increased MEPs are associated with motor learning, and this correlates with improved performance [8,54]. Increased IO slope excitability reflects the initial early (fast) motor learning stage [54,55]. There is an increase in MEPs following motor performance of the Purdue pegboard [56] or finger [8] tasks. It has also been revealed that this increase in MEPs following motor learning may be transient [9]. There was an increase in MEPs following a pinch task with a return to baseline once learning saturation occurred [39] and an increase in the cortical maps of the muscles involved in a serial reaction time test with a return to baseline values once the task was explicitly learned [9]. The fast motor learning stage occurs when there is a within-session improvement and is induced by a few trials on a time scale of minutes [40]. The increase in excitability of the M1 is associated with early stage (fast) motor learning and is not present once a skill has been learned. This may provide an explanation as to why there was not a significant increase in the slope post-acquisition test for the capsaicin group as this group was more accurate initially.
The TMS testing could be performed at baseline, post capsaicin, and post motor acquisition. However, all performance testing for the capsaicin group, including the baseline pre-acquisition test, was performed in the presence of pain. As described in the introduction, if we had performed the baseline testing without capsaicin and then performed the post-acquisition test in the presence of capsaicin, we would not have known if the performance improvements reflected motor learning or the effects of the capsaicin. Hence, as described in the introduction, we intentionally chose to perform the pre-acquisition testing in the presence of pain in order to uncouple motor learning effects on performance from pain effects on performance. This means that we cannot definitively say that the improved pre-acquisition performance was due to capsaicin. However, given that two past studies [41,42] showed that capsaicin led to better motor performance, it is likely that the improved baseline was due to the presence of capsaicin.
The IO curve is affected by the excitability and distribution of excitability of interneurons and motoneurons at cortical and spinal locations [57]. Therefore, it could be argued that the alterations in excitability following motor learning are due to peripheral or spinal mechanisms as opposed to alterations at the level of the M1. However, a prior motor learning study utilized TMS and TES (transcranial electrical stimulation) and found that there was an increase in excitability following TMS but not following TES. TES activates the corticospinal tract directly while TMS activates this tract transynaptically, and therefore it was suggested that the alterations in excitability following motor learning are occurring primarily at the level of the cortex [58]. However, there is also adaptive plasticity at the spinal level that occurs with motor learning [59] which may occur through presynaptic inhibition at the spinal level learning [60], and therefore we cannot say definitively whether the alterations in excitability are occurring at the cortical, subcortical, or spinal level.
Currently, the mechanism responsible for changes in CSE are not fully understood. It is known that the increases in MEPs associated with motor performance are impacted by GABAergic intracortical inhibition [54], and it is hypothesized that disinhibition in the M1 plays an important role in the early stages of motor learning [61,62,63]. In addition, synaptic neuroplasticity in the M1 is proposed to be facilitated by the activation of the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor [64]. In addition, a decrease in GABA inhibition and an increase in glutamate provides a mechanism for the increase in M1 excitability following motor learning for the control group. A reduction in GABA-mediated mechanisms of inhibition has been observed in different clinical scenarios, corroborating that a “disinhinibited” M1 plays an important role in the early stages of motor learning [65,66,67]. In addition, the synaptic neuroplasticity in the M1 is most likely facilitated by the activation of the NMDA receptor [68,69,70]. In addition, animal studies have suggested that long term potentiation (LTP) and long term depression (LTD) of synapses contributes to the neuroplasticity associated with motor learning [62,71] and provides a mechanism for increased excitability following motor learning.
A recent study by Mavromatis, Neige [44] examined the interactive effect of capsaicin cream and a pinch motor task on corticospinal excitability as measured by TMS (using SICI). Similar to our findings, this study found that the control group had an increase in excitability that was not observed for the capsaicin group and did not find a negative impact of pain on motor performance outcomes. Our current finding corroborates previous work that found differential effects of pain and early motor learning on neuroplasticity as measured by SEPs when compared to a control group, without a negative impact on motor performance [3,4,41].

5. Conclusions

The current study suggests that there may be contradictory effects of acute pain on motor learning neuroplasticity. Research indicates that cortical representations in the primary somatosensory area (S1) and M1 are altered in response to pain, and that pain perception and the associated alterations in neuroplasticity can be reversed by motor learning [72]. Clearly there is a link between motor and sensory systems and the effects of pain on motor learning may be due to cortico-thalamic, cortico-cerebellar, or cortico-cortico loops.

6. Limitations

Given that capsaicin causes vasodilation that is mediated locally [73], it is possible that some of the effects of MEPs that were observed in response to acute experimental pain may have been due to other local effects (i.e., skin temperature) as opposed to a direct effect of pain on MEP peak amplitude. Another limitation of this study is that we could only collect one set of pre-acquisition data due to learning effects, and therefore we did not collect pre-motor learning accuracy data before the capsaicin was applied, so it is possible that the two groups differed in performance at baseline, unrelated to the capsaicin application. However, as mentioned, two previous studies using the same task [41,42] found that the presence of remote capsaicin-induced pain improved initial motor performance, making it likely that the baseline differences were indeed due to capsaicin. There were some limitations to our study design as we used resting as opposed to active TMS IO curves to assess adaptation of a motor task. In addition, we did not measure the compound motor action potential (CMAP). As the ratio between the maximal (transcranially evoked) MEP amplitude and CMAP is believed to reflect the central mechanisms contributing to the MEP amplitude, we would have minimized inter-individual differences, and we could make stronger conclusions about the effect of pain and early motor learning on M1 excitability.
This study found an increase in slope for a control group following an acquisition phase that was not observed for the capsaicin group. A future direction for a study would involve the measurement of cortisol levels in response to acute cutaneous pain and early motor learning as endogenous stress hormones are a component of a memory modulating system that results in memory strength proportional to memory importance [74]. In addition, performing TMS IO curves midway through the acquisition phase could demonstrate whether there is an increase in slope for a capsaicin group at this time point that has already returned to baseline by the time measured in the current study. In addition, another future study could include the application of the cream to remote locations (contralateral arm or the leg) and the evaluation of CSE from both hemispheres, in order to obtain bilateral data to compare and to speculate on mechanisms of transcallosal interaction (and possibly inhibition).

Author Contributions

All authors are listed and all have contributed substantially to the manuscript. E.D., P.Y., and B.M. contributed to the conception and design of research; E.D. performed experiments; E.D. analyzed data and prepared figures; E.D., P.Y., and B.M. interpreted results of experiments; E.D. and B.M. drafted the manuscript; E.D., P.Y., and B.M. edited and revised the manuscript; E.D., P.Y., and B.M. approved the final version of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the following organizations for support and funding: Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), Canada Foundation for Innovation, Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation, and the University of Ontario Institute of Technology. There were no conflicts of interest. All authors are listed and all have contributed substantially to the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The founding sponsors had no role in the design of the study, in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, and in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Hodges, P.W.; Tucker, K. Moving differently in pain: A new theory to explain the adaptation to pain. Pain 2011, 152, S90–S98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bank, P.; Peper, C.; Marinus, J.; Beek, P.; Hilten, J. Motor consequences of experimentally induced limb pain: A systematic review. Eur. J. Pain 2013, 17, 145–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Dancey, E.; Murphy, B.; Srbely, J.; Yielder, P. The effect of experimental pain on motor training performance and sensorimotor integration. Exp. Brain Res. 2014, 232, 2879–2889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Dancey, E.; Murphy, B.; Andrew, D.; Yielder, P. The effect of local versus remote experimental pain on motor learning and sensorimotor integration using a complex typing task. Pain 2016, 157, 1682–1695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Sanes, J.N.; Donoghue, J.P. Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2000, 23, 393–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Karni, A.; Meyer, G.; Jezzard, P.; Adams, M.M.; Turner, R.; Ungerleider, L.G. Functional MRI evidence For adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature 1995, 377, l55–l58. [Google Scholar]
  7. Nudo, R.; Milliken, G.; Jenkins, W.M.; Merzenich, M. Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J. Neurosci. 1996, 16, 785–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Pascual-Leone, A.; Nguyet, D.; Cohen, L.G.; Brasil-Neto, J.P.; Cammarota, A.; Hallett, M. Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation during the acquisition of new fine motor skills. J. Neurophysiol. 1995, 74, 1037–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Pascual-Leone, A.; Grafman, J.; Hallett, M. Modulation of cortical motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science 1994, 263, 1287–1289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Classen, J.; Liepert, J.; Wise, S.; Hallett, M.; Cohen, L. Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. J. Neurophysiol. 1998, 79, 1117–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bütefisch, C.M.; Davis, B.C.; Wise, S.P.; Sawaki, L.; Kopylev, L.; Classen, J.; Cohen, L.G. Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in the human motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 3661–3665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Flor, H. Cortical reorganisation and chronic pain: Implications for rehabilitation. J. Rehabil. Med.-Suppl. 2003, 35, 66–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Schweinhardt, P.; Lee, M.; Tracey, I. Imaging pain in patients: Is it meaningful? Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2006, 19, 392–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Boudreau, S.; Romaniello, A.; Wang, K.; Svensson, P.; Sessle, B.J.; Arendt-Nielsen, L. The effects of intra-oral pain on motor cortex neuroplasticity associated with short-term novel tongue-protrusion training in humans. Pain 2007, 132, 169–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Bilodeau, M.-C.; Roosink, M.; Mercier, C. Effect of local versus remote tonic heat pain during training on acquisition and retention of a finger-tapping sequence task. Exp. Brain Res. 2015, 234, 475–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Bouffard, J.; Bouyer, L.J.; Roy, J.-S.; Mercier, C. Tonic pain experienced during locomotor training impairs retention despite normal performance during acquisition. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 9190–9195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rittig-Rasmussen, B.; Kasch, H.; Fuglsang-Frederiksen, A.; Svensson, P.; Jensen, T.S. The role of neuroplasticity in experimental neck pain: A study of potential mechanisms impeding clinical outcomes of training. Man. Ther. 2014, 19, 288–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. McGaughy, J.; Dalley, J.; Morrison, C.; Everitt, B.; Robbins, T. Selective behavioral and neurochemical effects of cholinergic lesions produced by intrabasalis infusions of 192 IgG-saporin on attentional performance in a five-choice serial reaction time task. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 1905–1913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Conner, J.M.; Culberson, A.; Packowski, C.; Chiba, A.A.; Tuszynski, M.H. Lesions of the basal forebrain cholinergic system impair task acquisition and abolish cortical plasticity associated with motor skill learning. Neuron 2003, 38, 819–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Rosenkranz, K.; Rothwell, J.C. The effect of sensory input and attention on the sensorimotor organization of the hand area of the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 2004, 561, 307–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Stefan, K.; Wycislo, M.; Classen, J. Modulation of associative human motor cortical plasticity by attention. J. Neurophysiol. 2004, 92, 66–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Coull, J.T. Neural correlates of attention and arousal: Insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Prog. Neurobiol. 1998, 55, 343–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Treede, R.-D.; Magerl, W. Multiple mechanisms of secondary hyperalgesia. Prog. Brain Res. 2000, 129, 331–341. [Google Scholar]
  24. Koltzenburg, M.; Torebjörk, H.E.; Wahren, L.K. Nociceptor modulated central sensitization causes mechanical hyperalgesia in acute chemogenic and chronic neuropathic pain. Brain 1994, 117, 579–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Maihöfner, C.; Handwerker, H.O. Differential coding of hyperalgesia in the human brain: A functional MRI study. Neuroimage 2005, 28, 996–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Torebjörk, H.; Lundberg, L.; LaMotte, R. Central changes in processing of mechanoreceptive input in capsaicin-induced secondary hyperalgesia in humans. J. Physiol. 1992, 448, 765–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Schmidt, R.; Schmelz, M.; Forster, C.; Ringkamp, M.; Torebjork, E.; Handwerker, H. Novel classes of responsive and unresponsive C nociceptors in human skin. J. Neurosci. 1995, 15, 333–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Dettmers, C.; Adler, T.; Rzanny, R.; Van Schayck, R.; Gaser, C.; Weiss, T.; Miltner, W.; Brückner, L.; Weiller, C. Increased excitability in the primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area in patients with phantom limb pain after upper limb amputation. Neurosci. Lett. 2001, 307, 109–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Krause, P.; Förderreuther, S.; Straube, A. TMS motor cortical brain mapping in patients with complex regional pain syndrome type I. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006, 117, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Falla, D.; Farina, D. Neuromuscular adaptation in experimental and clinical neck pain. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 2008, 18, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Madeleine, P.; Leclerc, F.; Arendt-Nielsen, L.; Ravier, P.; Farina, D. Experimental muscle pain changes the spatial distribution of upper trapezius muscle activity during sustained contraction. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006, 117, 2436–2445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Madeleine, P.; Lundager, B.; Voigt, M.; Arendt-Nielsen, L. Shoulder muscle co-ordination during chronic and acute experimental neck-shoulder pain. An occupational pain study. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1999, 79, 127–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Sae-Lee, D.; Whittle, T.; Forte, A.R.; Peck, C.C.; Byth, K.; Sessle, B.J.; Murray, G.M. Effects of experimental pain on jaw muscle activity during goal-directed jaw movements in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 2008, 189, 451–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Romaniello, A.; Cruccu, G.; McMillan, A.S.; Arendt-Nielsen, L.; Svensson, P. Effect of experimental pain from trigeminal muscle and skin on motor cortex excitability in humans. Brain Res. 2000, 882, 120–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Valeriani, M.; Restuccia, D.; Di Lazzaro, V.; Oliviero, A.; Profice, P.; Le Pera, D.; Saturno, E.; Tonali, P. Inhibition of the human primary motor area by painful heat stimulation of the skin. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1999, 110, 1475–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Valeriani, M.; Restuccia, D.; Di Lazzaro, V.; Oliviero, A.; Le Pera, D.; Profice, P.; Saturno, E.; Tonali, P. Inhibition of biceps brachii muscle motor area by painful heat stimulation of the skin. Exp. Brain Res. 2001, 139, 168–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Le Pera, D.; Graven-Nielsen, T.; Valeriani, M.; Oliviero, A.; Di Lazzaro, V.; Tonali, P.A.; Arendt-Nielsen, L. Inhibition of motor system excitability at cortical and spinal level by tonic muscle pain. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001, 112, 1633–1641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hirano, M.; Kubota, S.; Tanabe, S.; Koizume, Y.; Funase, K. Interactions among learning stage, retention, and primary motor cortex excitability in motor skill learning. Brain Stimul. 2015, 8, 1195–1204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Muellbacher, W.; Ziemann, U.; Boroojerdi, B.; Cohen, L.; Hallett, M. Role of the human motor cortex in rapid motor learning. Exp. Brain Res. 2001, 136, 431–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Karni, A. The acquisition of perceptual and motor skills: A memory system in the adult human cortex. Cogn. Brain Res. 1996, 5, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Dancey, E.; Murphy, B.; Andrew, D.; Yielder, P. The interactive effect of acute pain and motor learning acquisition on sensorimotor integration and motor learning outcomes. J. Neurophysiol. 2016, 116, 2210–2220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Dancey, E.; Yielder, P.; Murphy, B. Does Location of Tonic Pain Differentially Impact Motor Learning and Sensorimotor Integration? Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Ingham, D.; Tucker, K.J.; Tsao, H.; Hodges, P.W. The effect of pain on training-induced plasticity of the corticomotor system. Eur. J. Pain 2011, 15, 1028–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Mavromatis, N.; Neige, C.; Gagné, M.; Reilly, K.T.; Mercier, C. Effect of Experimental Hand Pain on Training-Induced Changes in Motor Performance and Corticospinal Excitability. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Cirillo, J.; Lavender, A.P.; Ridding, M.C.; Semmler, J.G. Motor cortex plasticity induced by paired associative stimulation is enhanced in physically active individuals. J. Physiol. 2009, 587, 5831–5842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Siebner, H.; Rothwell, J. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: New insights into representational cortical plasticity. Exp. Brain Res. 2003, 148, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Dolphin, N.W.; Crue, B.L., Jr. Pain: Clinical Manual For Nursing Practice. Clin. J. Pain 1989, 5, 363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Holland, L.; Murphy, B.; Passmore, S.; Yielder, P. Time course of corticospinal excitability changes following a novel motor training task. Neurosci. Lett. 2015, 591, 81–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Kaneko, K.; Kawai, S.; Fuchigami, Y.; Morita, H.; Ofuji, A. The effect of current direction induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation on the corticospinal excitability in human brain. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol./Electromyogr. Mot. Control 1996, 101, 478–482. [Google Scholar]
  50. Werhahn, K.; Fong, J.; Meyer, B.-U.; Priori, A.; Rothwell, J.; Day, B.; Thompson, P. The effect of magnetic coil orientation on the latency of surface EMG and single motor unit responses in the first dorsal interosseous muscle. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol./Evoked Potentials Sect. 1994, 93, 138–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Rossini, P.M.; Burke, D.; Chen, R.; Cohen, L.; Daskalakis, Z.; Di Iorio, R.; Di Lazzaro, V.; Ferreri, F.; Fitzgerald, P.; George, M. Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an IFCN Committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015, 126, 1071–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Pennisi, G.; Bella, R.; Lanza, G. Motor cortex plasticity in subcortical ischemic vascular dementia: What can TMS say? Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015, 126, 851–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Cirillo, J.; Rogasch, N.C.; Semmler, J.G. Hemispheric differences in use-dependent corticomotor plasticity in young and old adults. Exp. Brain Res. 2010, 205, 57–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Ziemann, U.; Muellbacher, W.; Hallett, M.; Cohen, L.G. Modulation of practice-dependent plasticity in human motor cortex. Brain 2001, 124, 1171–1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Muellbacher, W.; Ziemann, U.; Wissel, J.; Dang, N.; Kofler, M.; Facchini, S.; Boroojerdi, B.; Poewe, W.; Hallett, M. Early consolidation in human primary motor cortex. Nature 2002, 415, 640–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Rossi, F.; Triggs, W.J.; Eisenschenk, S. Topographic differences in task-dependent facilitation of magnetic motor evoked potentials. Neurology 1999, 52, 537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Rosenkranz, K.; Kacar, A.; Rothwell, J.C. Differential modulation of motor cortical plasticity and excitability in early and late phases of human motor learning. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 12058–12066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Rothwell, J. Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. Methods 1997, 74, 113–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Wolpaw, J.R.; Tennissen, A.M. Activity-dependent spinal cord plasticity in health and disease. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 24, 807–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Thompson, A.K.; Chen, X.Y.; Wolpaw, J.R. Acquisition of a simple motor skill: Task-dependent adaptation plus long-term change in the human soleus H-reflex. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 5784–5792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hess, G.; Donoghue, J. Long-term potentiation of horizontal connections provides a mechanism to reorganize cortical motor maps. J. Neurophysiol. 1994, 71, 2543–2547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Hess, G.; Aizenman, C.D.; Donoghue, J.P. Conditions for the induction of long-term potentiation in layer II/III horizontal connections of the rat motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1996, 75, 1765–1778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Jones, E. GABAergic neurons and their role in cortical plasticity in primates. Cereb. Cortex 1993, 3, 361–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Castro-Alamancos, M.A.; Connors, B.W. Short-term synaptic enhancement and long-term potentiation in neocortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1996, 93, 1335–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Bella, R.; Lanza, G.; Cantone, M.; Giuffrida, S.; Puglisi, V.; Vinciguerra, L.; Pennisi, M.; Ricceri, R.; D’Agate, C.C.; Malaguarnera, G. Effect of a gluten-free diet on cortical excitability in adults with celiac disease. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0129218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Lanza, G.; Bella, R.; Cantone, M.; Pennisi, G.; Ferri, R.; Pennisi, M. Cognitive Impairment and Celiac Disease: Is Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation a Trait d’Union between Gut and Brain? Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2018, 19, 2243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Pennisi, G.; Lanza, G.; Giuffrida, S.; Vinciguerra, L.; Puglisi, V.; Cantone, M.; Pennisi, M.; D’Agate, C.C.; Naso, P.; Aprile, G. Excitability of the motor cortex in de novo patients with celiac disease. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e102790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Bella, R.; Ferri, R.; Lanza, G.; Cantone, M.; Pennisi, M.; Puglisi, V.; Vinciguerra, L.; Spampinato, C.; Mazza, T.; Malaguarnera, G. TMS follow-up study in patients with vascular cognitive impairment-no dementia. Neurosci. Lett. 2013, 534, 155–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Cantone, M.; Bramanti, A.; Lanza, G.; Pennisi, M.; Bramanti, P.; Pennisi, G.; Bella, R. Cortical Plasticity in Depression: A Neurochemical Perspective From Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. ASN Neuro 2017, 9, 1759091417711512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Lanza, G.; Bramanti, P.; Cantone, M.; Pennisi, M.; Pennisi, G.; Bella, R. Vascular cognitive impairment through the looking glass of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Behav. Neurol. 2017, 2017, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Rioult-Pedotti, M.-S.; Friedman, D.; Donoghue, J.P. Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science 2000, 290, 533–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Pleger, B.; Tegenthoff, M.; Ragert, P.; Förster, A.F.; Dinse, H.R.; Schwenkreis, P.; Nicolas, V.; Maier, C. Sensorimotor returning in complex regional pain syndrome parallels pain reduction. Ann. Neurol. 2005, 57, 425–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Hayman, M.; Kam, P.C. Capsaicin: A review of its pharmacology and clinical applications. Curr. Anaesth. Crit. Care 2008, 19, 338–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Cahill, L.; McGaugh, J.L. Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci. 1998, 21, 294–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Schematic of the protocol illustrating that transcranial magnetic stimulation input–output (TMS IO) curves were performed at baseline, 20 min post-application, and then following the acquisition phase (45 min from baseline). Somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs), numeric pain rating scale (NPRS).
Figure 1. Schematic of the protocol illustrating that transcranial magnetic stimulation input–output (TMS IO) curves were performed at baseline, 20 min post-application, and then following the acquisition phase (45 min from baseline). Somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs), numeric pain rating scale (NPRS).
Brainsci 09 00063 g001
Figure 2. A photograph of the motor learning task that was performed by each participant. The traces consist of a series of dots which the participant tracked when the trace passed a horizontal line. The orange dot shows the location of the participant’s cursor along the horizontal line. Motor error was calculated as the average distance that the cursor was from the optimal trace.
Figure 2. A photograph of the motor learning task that was performed by each participant. The traces consist of a series of dots which the participant tracked when the trace passed a horizontal line. The orange dot shows the location of the participant’s cursor along the horizontal line. Motor error was calculated as the average distance that the cursor was from the optimal trace.
Brainsci 09 00063 g002
Figure 3. Line-graph depicting the average NPRS ratings for the capsaicin and control groups. Significant differences are indicated by asterisks (all p < 0.05). Error bars represent the standard deviation.
Figure 3. Line-graph depicting the average NPRS ratings for the capsaicin and control groups. Significant differences are indicated by asterisks (all p < 0.05). Error bars represent the standard deviation.
Brainsci 09 00063 g003
Figure 4. (A) Line-graph depicting the average percent motor error (in %) for the capsaicin and control groups. Significant differences between the groups are indicated by asterisks (all p < 0.005). Error bars represent the standard deviation. (B) Bar graph depicting the normalized percent error for the capsaicin and control groups. Significant differences (from pre-test) are indicated by asterisks (all p < 0.005) for both groups post-acquisition and at retention with no significant differences seen between the two groups.
Figure 4. (A) Line-graph depicting the average percent motor error (in %) for the capsaicin and control groups. Significant differences between the groups are indicated by asterisks (all p < 0.005). Error bars represent the standard deviation. (B) Bar graph depicting the normalized percent error for the capsaicin and control groups. Significant differences (from pre-test) are indicated by asterisks (all p < 0.005) for both groups post-acquisition and at retention with no significant differences seen between the two groups.
Brainsci 09 00063 g004
Figure 5. Average MEP data at baseline and post-motor learning from a representative control participant at all stimulation intensities (90% rMT–140% rMT).
Figure 5. Average MEP data at baseline and post-motor learning from a representative control participant at all stimulation intensities (90% rMT–140% rMT).
Brainsci 09 00063 g005
Figure 6. Bar-graph depicting the normalized average slope post-application and post-motor learning acquisition for the control and capsaicin groups. There was a significant increase in slope for the control group (p < 0.05) following motor learning acquisition as indicated by an asterisk. Error bars represent the standard deviation.
Figure 6. Bar-graph depicting the normalized average slope post-application and post-motor learning acquisition for the control and capsaicin groups. There was a significant increase in slope for the control group (p < 0.05) following motor learning acquisition as indicated by an asterisk. Error bars represent the standard deviation.
Brainsci 09 00063 g006
Figure 7. IO curves of control (A) and capsaicin (B) participants. Note increase in slope for the control (A) participants, representative of the significant change in the group data.
Figure 7. IO curves of control (A) and capsaicin (B) participants. Note increase in slope for the control (A) participants, representative of the significant change in the group data.
Brainsci 09 00063 g007
Figure 8. Mean IO curves of control (A) and capsaicin (B) groups. Note the increase in mean slope for the control (A) group as indicated by an asterisk. Error bars represent the standard deviation.
Figure 8. Mean IO curves of control (A) and capsaicin (B) groups. Note the increase in mean slope for the control (A) group as indicated by an asterisk. Error bars represent the standard deviation.
Brainsci 09 00063 g008
Table 1. Resting motor threshold (rMT) values that were determined by finding the lowest stimulator intensity that elicited an MEP of at least 0.05 mV in at least five out of ten trials.
Table 1. Resting motor threshold (rMT) values that were determined by finding the lowest stimulator intensity that elicited an MEP of at least 0.05 mV in at least five out of ten trials.
ParticipantControlCapsaicin
13662
24954
35860
46249
54452
66453
74540
85342
94356
105964
115747
124253
Averages51.8252.67
SD9.027.40
Table 2. Goodness of fit data (R2) for the capsaicin and control groups at baseline, post-application, and post-motor learning acquisition.
Table 2. Goodness of fit data (R2) for the capsaicin and control groups at baseline, post-application, and post-motor learning acquisition.
ControlCapsaicin
ParticipantBaselinePost-ApplicationPost-Motor LearningBaselinePost-ApplicationPost-Motor Learning
10.900.800.950.860.770.75
20.880.880.910.740.960.95
30.770.760.870.780.850.89
40.780.700.900.920.750.92
50.930.890.870.870.780.84
60.900.800.950.960.930.89
70.670.780.740.880.810.88
80.900.860.900.930.950.76
90.840.830.720.920.870.79
100.850.970.970.870.900.96
110.820.940.910.880.850.89
120.820.880,800.960.930.88
Averages0.840.840.880.880.860.87
SD0.0730.0770.0810.0670.0730.069

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dancey, E.; Yielder, P.; Murphy, B. The Interactive Effect of Tonic Pain and Motor Learning on Corticospinal Excitability. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030063

AMA Style

Dancey E, Yielder P, Murphy B. The Interactive Effect of Tonic Pain and Motor Learning on Corticospinal Excitability. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(3):63. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030063

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dancey, Erin, Paul Yielder, and Bernadette Murphy. 2019. "The Interactive Effect of Tonic Pain and Motor Learning on Corticospinal Excitability" Brain Sciences 9, no. 3: 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030063

APA Style

Dancey, E., Yielder, P., & Murphy, B. (2019). The Interactive Effect of Tonic Pain and Motor Learning on Corticospinal Excitability. Brain Sciences, 9(3), 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9030063

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop