Next Article in Journal
Challenges in the Management of Cavernoma-Related Epilepsy: Seizure Outcomes, Antiseizure Medication Practices, and Access to Intraoperative Technologies in Kazakhstan
Previous Article in Journal
Navigating the Complexity of Bilingual Aphasia: Current Insights and Future Directions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Language Learning as a Non-Pharmacological Intervention in Older Adults with (Past) Depression

1
Center for Language and Cognition Groningen, University of Groningen, 9712 EK Groningen, The Netherlands
2
Center for Language Studies, Radboud University, 6526 HT Nijmegen, The Netherlands
3
Amsterdam Center for Language and Communication, University of Amsterdam, 1000 BP Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2025, 15(9), 991; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15090991
Submission received: 11 July 2025 / Revised: 9 September 2025 / Accepted: 10 September 2025 / Published: 15 September 2025

Abstract

Background: Language learning has been proposed as a non-pharmacological intervention to promote healthy aging. This intervention has not been studied in older adults with a history of depression, who experience high prevalence of cognitive dysfunction. This small-scale study was the first to investigate the potential efficacy of language learning in older adults with (past) depression. Methods: Data on psychosocial well-being, cognitive functioning, and language outcomes were collected in nineteen participants with (past) depression (M = 69.7 years old, SD = 2.9; 79% women, 21% men) and a control group of fifteen older adults without depression in the past 25 years (M = 70.1 years old, SD = 3.8; 60% women, 40% men). Data were collected before, immediately after, and four months after completing a three-month language course. Results: Participants with (past) depression showed significant decreases in apathy, social loneliness, and cognitive failures, and increases in associative memory and global cognition. The control group improved on associative memory and letter-number sequencing. Both groups improved in linguistic self-confidence and lexical access to English, while the group with (past) depression also improved on listening and speaking proficiency. Conclusions: The intervention had limited benefits for cognition and psychosocial well-being, but (longer) group-based learning interventions may build up social and motivational reserves protecting against morbidity. Research with larger samples and a no-training control sample is needed to further support these findings.

1. Introduction

Major depressive disorder after the age of 60 is a prevalent illness [1]. Aside from affective and somatic symptoms, lower cognitive functioning is commonly reported in older adults with depression [2] and these cognitive symptoms often persist after depression remission [3]. Furthermore, the occurrence of depression in midlife and later in life has been identified both as a risk factor for [4] and a prodromal symptom of dementia [3]. Several types of interventions to potentially increase well-being and cognitive functioning have been investigated both in populations with and without depression. One of the most commonly studied interventions is computerized cognitive training, whereby participants train cognitively demanding tasks [5]. Although study designs vary, this method has been criticized for having too much overlap between the games used in training and the outcome measures, making it difficult to distinguish intervention gains from retest effects, and for not addressing underlying factors that contribute to depression, like social isolation [5]. Clearer transfer effects have been found in other non-pharmacological interventions such as group-based physical exercise [6], showing that improvement in cognitive functions is possible; however, research is scarce, and results regarding both a change in depressive symptoms and cognitive functioning are inconclusive [7]. Therefore, additional interventions should be explored.
This past decade, language learning has received special attention as an intervention that can potentially improve both well-being [8] and cognitive functioning [9]. Lifelong bilingualism has been associated with higher levels of cognitive reserve in older adulthood [10], which is linked to preserved cognitive functioning in older age due to an increased resilience against brain atrophy [11]. As such, lifelong bilingualism has been associated with a later onset of dementia symptoms [12]. These protective effects are thought to stem from the regulation of the constant co-activation of known languages. That is, when speaking one language, all other known languages are active to some extent [13]; the activation of non-target languages must be suppressed to prevent unwanted interference. This constant ‘juggling’ of control involves certain executive functions (e.g., inhibition, selection) that fall under the umbrella of attentional control [14]. These processes already occur in early stages of language learning [15]. This has led researchers to investigate later-life language learning as a tool for healthy aging, not least because the same neural networks that decline in aging have been hypothesized to be strengthened during language learning later in life [9].
While comparatively little focus has been placed on outcomes related to well-being, like reduced loneliness and depression, studies have found later-life language learning to positively impact healthy older adults: language learning has been called an enjoyable experience [8,16], which contributes to one’s sense of autonomy [17] and social well-being [18]. However, most studies did not use validated tools to assess indices of well-being, meaning that it is difficult to assess if later-life language learning could lead to clinically relevant improvements in well-being. With regards to cognitive functioning, some studies find evidence for cognitive adaptations [16,17,19] while other studies do not [20,21,22]. Notably, however, lower baseline levels of cognitive functioning have been associated with larger improvements in older language learners [23], and studies in older adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) have also found significant cognitive improvements following a language course [24].
No work on later-life language learning to date has investigated people with (past) depression. Instead, most studies have investigated cognitively healthy older adults, who score near-ceiling on well-being and neuropsychological tests [25]. The evidence discussed above, however, suggests that language learning can be both a cognitively and socially stimulating activity, which may be especially advantageous for people with lower levels of baseline cognitive functioning, including older adults with a current or past episode of depression [3]. This study is the first to investigate the effect of later-life language learning on cognitive functioning and well-being in older adults with a current depressive episode or a history of depression. We expect baseline levels of cognitive functioning and well-being to be lower in participants with (past) depression in comparison to a control group without mood complaints. Furthermore, we hypothesize that older adults with (past) depression will show larger improvements on indices of cognitive functioning and well-being than the control group.

2. Method

2.1. Study Design

In this intervention study, the efficacy of a three-month English language learning intervention was assessed in two independent samples (participants with (past) depression and a control group without mood complaints in the past 25 years) recruited in the Netherlands. Recruitment and interventions took place from February 2021 to July 2022. Data on cognitive functioning and well-being were collected before, immediately after, and four months after completion of the intervention. Data collection and the interventions were conducted through videoconferencing due to COVID-19. This study was part of a larger project that also comprised a randomized-controlled trial (RCT) comparing effects of a language course with those of a music course and lecture series in older adults not experiencing cognitive decline or depression [26]. The language sample of this RCT was used as the control group for participants with (past) depression in this study.

2.2. Participants and Data Collection Procedures

Two samples of community-dwelling older adults (aged ≥ 65; range = 65–76) in the Netherlands were included, both of whom were enrolled in the language intervention. This study did not include a no-intervention control group. This choice was an ethical consideration out of concern for the welfare of the participants (see article 1.1 in the core principles of the Canadian panel of research ethics). We expected participants to enroll in the study because of a personal interest in (language) learning. Being randomized into a no-training control group, then, may have been disappointing and disempowering to participants, which in turn may have negatively affected their well-being. This consideration was endorsed by the Ethical Research Committee (CETO) of the University of Groningen. Since the researchers had no formal clinical background to address these potential negative feelings, we opted for the current design (see Section 4.1. for more about this limitation). The experimental group (n = 19) had (past) depression (major depression ≤ 25 years ago based on DSM-5 criteria) or current depressive symptoms (DSM-5 criteria for major depression and/or a Geriatric Depression Scale score ≥ 5). The control group (n = 15) had been free of depression or other psychiatric disorders in the past 25 years. Other inclusion criteria were speaking Dutch ≥ 80% in their daily lives, having normal intelligence [27], being able to perform instrumental activities of daily living [28], and having access to a laptop or desktop computer with a webcam and microphone.
Exclusion criteria were: English proficiency ≥ B2 on the CEFR scale, Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score ≤ 22 [29], playing a musical instrument ≥ once a week in the past 20 years, severe reading difficulties, neurological problems, uncorrected visual or hearing impairment, past or present alcohol and/or drug dependency, and using benzodiazepines ≥ 3 times per week. Due to difficulties recruiting suitable participants, the criterion for benzodiazepine use was loosened during recruitment (where it used to be no benzodiazepines), as was the criterion for presence of depression (this was initially only ≥5 GDS or current major depressive episode).
Participants were mostly recruited through the local library network Biblionet and participant repository Hersenonderzoek.nl. After participants provided verbal informed consent, a brief telephone screening determined preliminary inclusion. In case of a positive evaluation, written informed consent was obtained. Subsequently, full eligibility was assessed in a screening appointment held via videoconferencing.
Data were collected through videoconferencing with the participant and an experimenter in two test sessions lasting between 1 and 1.5 h each. Sessions typically took place on the same day, with a 2.5 h break in between sessions. English proficiency was assessed before and after the intervention in a separate testing session taking approximately 45 min.
The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Arts at the University of Groningen (ref. nr.: 6989509), and was executed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.3. Outcome Variables and Covariates

A battery of questionnaires and tasks, (adapted from [30], also used in [26]) was used to measure cognitive functioning, psychosocial well-being, and English proficiency. Table 1 gives an overview of all the outcome measures.

2.4. English Course

Participants followed a three-month English course consisting of one introductory class (explaining the course’s goals and materials) and six bi-weekly classes led by an experienced instructor. Classes, taught through videoconferencing, lasted approximately 1.5 h each. Classes focused mostly on communicating in English. Additionally, participants were asked to engage in self-study for five days a week, 45 min a day, using the materials and online environment of the Leidse Onderwijsinstellingen (LOI), a Dutch commercial provider of self-study courses. A participant adhering to the study protocol would spend approximately 4.5 h per week undertaking activities related to the intervention (e.g., going to class, self-study). Adherence to the self-study protocol was self-reported in digital study diaries that were sent out every week.

2.5. Analysis

All analyses were conducted in R version 4.2.0 [44]. All dependent variables (DVs) were normalized (z-scaled) and centered to ease interpretation. Scores for some DVs were reversed, so an increased score always represented an improvement. A single multivariate multiple regression model was built for cognitive and psychosocial DVs, to reduce Type I errors [45]. A second model assessed if differentiating between participants with current (in the past six months) and past (previous 25 years to six months) depressive symptoms improved the model fit. Since this was not the case, we continued the analysis without distinguishing between those with current and past depression. Language outcomes were analyzed in a separate model, as they were only assessed at pre-test and post-test. Models included covariates for age, sex, cognitive reserve index score [46] and premorbid intelligence [27], and a random intercept per participant. No power analysis was conducted beforehand.
Planned comparisons and effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated using ‘emmeans’ version 1.8.4.-1 [47]. Results were visualized using ‘interactions’ version 1.1.5 [48]. Model assumptions were checked using ‘performance’ version 0.10.2. [49]. Some of the categorical variables and some of the interaction terms between categorical variables had high variance inflation factor values (VIFs), suggesting multicollinearity (see Supplementary S1). We ran the model again without interaction terms, which yielded no problematic VIFs. This suggests that the high VIF scores stem from the inclusion of interaction terms, which is a condition under which multicollinearity can be safely ignored [50].

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

A total of 37 participants were included in the language intervention (for details on recruitment and exclusion, please see Supplementary S2). All participants in the control group completed the entire intervention. Two participants with (past) depression dropped out during the course due to stress, one did not complete the post-test measurement due to illness, and one participant did not respond to invitations for the follow-up assessment. The final sample consisted of 34 participants: 15 in the control group and 19 in the (past) depression group. Demographic baseline characteristics stratified by group are listed in Table 2. As expected, the control group had higher global cognitive functioning and higher levels of cognitive reserve than those with (past depression) at screening. On average, based on the time spent on the intervention each week participants in both groups adhered to the protocol.

3.2. Intervention Outcomes

Table 3 shows the raw data stratified by time and group. Table 4 shows significant results from our planned comparisons. Cognitive and psychosocial intervention outcomes are visualized in Figure 1 and English outcomes in Figure 2. Only significant results will be discussed below. Model building procedures and complete results (including non-significant findings) are available in Supplementary S1 and S3.

3.2.1. Association of Language Learning with Psychosocial Measures

Between-group differences
Please note that AES, GDS-15, LARSS, and loneliness were reversed for the analyses. At pre-test, post-test, and follow-up, the control group scored significantly higher than those with (past) depression on LARSS, emotional loneliness, and BRS. Additionally, the control group had higher GDS-15 scores at baseline, higher AES scores at pre-test and follow-up, and higher social loneliness scores at pre-test and post-test.
Within-group changes over time
Those with (past) depression improved significantly from pre-test to post-test on AES and social loneliness. This improvement was maintained at follow-up.
Between-group differences in change over time
There was a significant difference in the change from pre-test to post-test between groups on the AES, such that apathy levels decreased in participants with (past) depression while the control group’s apathy levels increased. This pattern reversed from post-test to follow-up, and the difference in change over time between the two groups was again significant.

3.2.2. Association of Language Learning with Cognitive Functioning

Between-group differences
At pre-test, the control group scored significantly higher than those with (past) depression on CFQ, MoCA, and digit span forward. At post-test, the control group scored higher on CFQ, digit span forward, digit span backward, and letter-number sequencing. At follow-up, the control group scored higher on the MoCA and verbal fluency task.
Within-group change over time
Participants with (past) depression improved significantly from pre-test to post-test on the MoCA. This group also showed a significant improvement between pre-test and follow-up for CFQ and VAT-E paired association. Participants in the control group improved significantly from pre-test to post-test on letter-number sequencing, which significantly decreased again from post-test to follow-up. This group also improved on the VAT-E paired association task from pre-test to follow-up.
Between-group differences in change over time
The control group showed a significantly larger improvement from pre-test to pos-test than participants with (past) depression on letter-number sequencing. The subsequent decrease from post-test to follow-up was significantly larger in the control group than in participants with (past depression).

3.3. Associations of Language Learning with English Proficiency

At pre-test, the control group scored significantly higher than those with (past) depression on all IELTS measures. At post-test, the control group only scored significantly higher than those with (past) depression on the English verbal fluency task.
Within-group changes over time
Those with (past) depression improved significantly from pre-test to post-test on all IELTS measures, English verbal fluency, self-rated English productive skills, and self-rated English productive skills. The control group improved significantly on English verbal fluency, self-rated productive English skills, and self-rated receptive English skills.
Between-group differences in change over time
There were no significant differences in change in any of the English proficiency measures across time-points between groups.

4. Discussion

This small-scale study investigated if later-life language learning stimulates psychosocial well-being and cognitive functioning in older adults with (past) depression and a control group without depression in the last 25 years. For psychosocial well-being, we found a reduction in apathy levels from pre-test to post-test only in participants with (past) depression, suggesting that the course rekindled their overall motivation and goal-directed behavior. Apathy scores increased again at follow-up, suggesting that continued course engagement is needed to retain improvements. Furthermore, while participants with (past) depression reported higher levels of loneliness than the control group across time-points, we found a sustained reduction in social loneliness in participants with (past) depression, meaning they felt more satisfied with their broader social networks [51]. Contrary to our findings, Ware et al. [22] found no change in loneliness after a language course. Potentially this discrepancy arises because the earlier study did not differentiate between social and emotional loneliness. We argue it is necessary to do so, as one cannot necessarily expect intimate relationships, which the emotional loneliness subscale taps into, after a short course. The reduced social loneliness we report is promising, as low levels of loneliness are often associated with reduced overall morbidity and mortality [52], while high levels of loneliness have been associated with increased depression severity at two-year follow-up [53]. Likewise, reduced apathy could also be positive in the longer term, as high apathy levels in late-life depression are associated with increased mortality [54] and higher levels of depression at six-year follow-up [55]. We speculate that language learning (or other group-based learning interventions) could build up a social and motivational reserve over time [56], subsequently reducing morbidity in the long-term. To assess this, future studies should include longer follow-up periods. Furthermore, future work should investigate if the reduced apathy and social loneliness associated with group-based language learning could provide additive benefits when coupled with methods that, contrary to language learning, are effective for targeting depressive symptoms, resilience, and emotion regulation, such as cognitive behavioral therapy [57].
In line with some previous studies conducted with participants without depression [19,20,21], most cognitive outcome measures did not change from pre- to post-test in the control group. Participants with (past) depression, on the other hand, improved significantly on the MoCA, a screening test for cognitive impairment, from pre-test to post-test, but not on tasks that appear in the MoCA in a shortened or modified form (VF, TMT, digit-span). This is partially in line with previous work, which postulated that specifically performance on comprehensive measurements of cognitive functioning should improve, since language learning activates extensive brain networks [19]. However, the control group did not significantly improve on this measure, potentially because they scored closer to ceiling level at baseline. Significant reductions in self-assessed cognitive failures were only reported in participants with (past) depression. Past research has reported that subjective cognitive complaints are more strongly linked to depressive symptoms than to actual cognitive functioning [58], suggesting that participants with (past) depression were more sensitive to improvements on the cognitive failures questionnaire. A surprising result was the significant improvement of letter-number sequencing in the control group, while no such improvement was found in the (past) depression group. While we do not have a cogent explanation, we speculate that the higher baseline levels of English proficiency in the control group meant that they more easily re-trained working-memory manipulation processes needed in speaking additional languages. That is, when speaking a second language, one needs to keep the intended message (i.e., the words themselves) in working memory, while simultaneously applying a word order that is different from one’s first language. Perhaps the higher levels of English proficiency found in the control group at baseline meant they more easily retrained these processes of manipulating items held in working memory (which letter-number sequencing indexes). Lastly, both groups showed an identical improvement on associative memory from pre-test to follow-up. Potentially, the skill of mapping new vocabulary in an additional language to existing semantic concepts, as undertaken in language learning, led to delayed transfer effects on the VAT-E. However, this effect only became significant at follow-up and only one version of this test was used, which taken together suggests a test–retest effect. Notably, our study differs from some later-life language learning interventions, as our participants had at least some basis in the taught language. Future research should include both completely unknown languages and languages for which participants have some basis, to see if this leads to differential effects in those with (past) depression.
The linguistic outcome measures paint a clearer picture. A short but intense language course led to significant improvements in speaking and listening proficiency (IELTS), lexical access, and linguistic self-confidence in participants with (past) depression. This is in line with previous research with older adults without depression, which also reported significant improvements in proficiency after or within three months of individual and classroom-based training [59,60]. The control group in our study also showed improvements in lexical access and linguistic self-confidence, but not on IELTS, presumably because their English proficiency was higher at baseline. These improvements in linguistic skills and self-confidence are positive, as seeing oneself improve may positively affect one’s overall motivation and self-efficacy [17,22]. Future research could investigate the link between linguistic self-confidence and overall self-efficacy in more detail.

4.1. Strengths and Weaknesses

A strength of the study was that it focused on psychosocial, cognitive, as well as linguistic factors, providing a more holistic picture of the effects of later-life language learning. Another strength was including a follow-up period, shedding light on whether continued course engagement may be necessary for retainment. Furthermore, we screened and controlled for potentially confounding variables. Lastly, our study expanded on previous work by including validated tools used in clinical settings and by focusing on participants with (past) depression, a group which has not been represented yet in the later-life language learning field, even though they could benefit from this intervention.
A major limitation of the study is that it is underpowered due to the small sample size. While the call for participants was distributed to a large participant database, including a large sample of people with (past) depression, very few older adults experiencing a current major depressive episode enrolled. This is a common issue with depression trials, potentially caused by participants viewing the study as incurring a high burden or presenting a risk of symptom exacerbation [61]. The small sample overall could be explained by relatively stringent inclusion criteria regarding participants’ language background (cf. [26]). This small sample means that the obtained results must be interpreted with caution. A further limitation to the robustness of our findings was not including a no-training control group (but see the method section for the ethical consideration underlying this design choice). The lack of a group that did not receive an intervention means that it is [25] not possible to fully attribute changes in cognition and psychosocial well-being to the intervention itself. Instead, other environmental factors or test–retest effects may have played a role in the results obtained for the two respective samples recruited for this study. Since our design cannot rule out these factors, caution must be applied when interpreting the results. Future research should include samples that do not take part in an intervention to make the results more robust. Lastly, the study protocol was conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, meaning that both the intervention and the data collection were carried out via the internet. As a result, participation was limited to those with pre-existing proficiency in using computers. Furthermore, data collection via videoconferencing meant that participants were not always in an environment without distractors. However, digital data-collection also had its strengths, as it allowed older adults with reduced mobility to participate.

5. Conclusions

This small-scale study explored the effects of a three-month language learning intervention on psychosocial well-being, cognitive functioning, and English proficiency in older adults with (past) depression and a control group. Overall, language learning seems to have limited immediate benefits for psychosocial well-being and cognition. However, indicators of psychosocial well-being included in this study suggest that a social and motivational reserve may be built up in the process of following a group-based intervention. Furthermore, we report increases in linguistic self-confidence in both groups, which may also be associated with higher levels of self-efficacy and motivation in the long term.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci15090991/s1, File S1: Model building and results; Figure S2: Flowchart for inclusion in the study; File S3: Complete results including non-significant findings.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.B., F.v.d.B., R.K., H.L. and M.K.; methodology, J.B.; F.v.d.B., R.K., H.L. and M.K; software, J.B. and F.v.d.B.; validation, J.B. and F.v.d.B.; formal analysis, J.B.; investigation, J.B. and F.v.d.B.; resources, M.K.; data curation, J.B. and F.v.d.B.; writing—original draft preparation, J.B.; writing—review and editing, J.B., F.v.d.B., R.K., H.L. and M.K.; visualization, J.B.; supervision, R.K. and M.K.; project administration, J.B. and F.v.d.B.; funding acquisition, M.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was funded by the Dutch Research Council (NWO Talent Programme grant awarded to M. Keijzer: 016.Vidi.185.190).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Arts at the University of Groningen (ref. nr.: 69895095), and was executed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The original data presented in the study are openly available in Open Science Framework at [https://osf.io/sa94q, accessed on 11 September 2025].

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank everyone who was involved in developing and conduct-ing this project, including the participants. Funding from the Dutch Research Council (NWO Talent Programme grant awarded to M. Keijzer: 016.Vidi.185.190) is gratefully acknowledged. Participant recruitment was partly accomplished through Hersenonderzoek.nl, a Dutch online registry that facilitates participant recruitment for neuroscience studies (www.hersenonderzoek.nl, accessed on 11 September 2025). Hersenonderzoek.nl is funded by ZonMw-Memorabel (project no 73305095003), a project in the context of the Dutch Deltaplan Dementie, Gieskes-Strijbis Foundation, the Alzheimer’s Society in the Netherlands and Brain Foundation Netherlands.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no potential conflict of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

References

  1. Zenebe, Y.; Akele, B.; Selassie, M.W.; Necho, M. Prevalence and determinants of depression among old age: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry 2021, 20, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Wu, Z.; Zhong, X.; Peng, Q.; Chen, B.; Zhang, M.; Zhou, H.; Mai, N.; Huang, X.; Ning, Y. Longitudinal Association Between Cognition and Depression in Patients with Late-Life Depression: A Cross-Lagged Design Study. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 577058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bhalla, R.K.; Butters, M.A.; Mulsant, B.H.; Begley, A.E.; Zmuda, M.D.; Schoderbek, B.; Pollock, B.G.; Reynolds, C.F.; Becker, J.T. Persistence of Neuropsychologic Deficits in the Remitted State of Late-Life Depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2006, 14, 419–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Sáiz-Vázquez, O.; Gracia-García, P.; Ubillos-Landa, S.; Puente-Martínez, A.; Casado-Yusta, S.; Olaya, B.; Santabárbara, J. Depression as a Risk Factor for Alzheimer’s Disease: A Systematic Review of Longitudinal Meta-Analyses. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 1809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Motter, J.N.; Devanand, D.P.; Doraiswamy, P.M.; Sneed, J.R. Computerized Cognitive Training for Major Depressive Disorder: What’s Next? Front. Psychiatry 2015, 6, 137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Neviani, F.; Belvederi Murri, M.; Mussi, C.; Triolo, F.; Toni, G.; Simoncini, E.; Tripi, F.; Menchetti, M.; Ferrari, S.; Ceresini, G.; et al. Physical exercise for late life depression: Effects on cognition and disability. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2017, 29, 1105–1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Steffens, D.C. Exercise for late-life depression? It depends. Lancet 2013, 382, 4–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Matsumoto, D. Exploring Third-Age Foreign Language Learning from the Well-being Perspective: Work in Progress. SiSAL 2019, 10, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Antoniou, M.; Gunasekera, G.M.; Wong, P.C.M. Foreign language training as cognitive therapy for age-related cognitive decline: A hypothesis for future research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2689–2698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Schweizer, T.A.; Ware, J.; Fischer, C.E.; Craik, F.I.M.; Bialystok, E. Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: Evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 2012, 48, 991–996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 2015–2028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Woumans, E.; Santens, P.; Sieben, A.; Versijpt, J.; Stevens, M.; Duyck, W. Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 568–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Van Hell, J.G.; Dijkstra, T. Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 2002, 9, 780–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.M. How does bilingualism modify cognitive function? Attention to the mechanism. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 2022, 29, 1246–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. McLaughlin, J.; Osterhout, L.; Kim, A. Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 703–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Meltzer, J.A.; Kates Rose, M.; Le, A.Y.; Spencer, K.A.; Goldstein, L.; Gubanova, A.; Lai, A.C.; Yossofzai, M.; Armstrong, S.E.M.; Bialystok, E. Improvement in executive function for older adults through smartphone apps: A randomized clinical trial comparing language learning and brain training. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2023, 30, 150–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Pfenninger, S.E.; Polz, S. Foreign language learning in the third age: A pilot feasibility study on cognitive, socio-affective and linguistic drivers and benefits in relation to previous bilingualism of the learner. J. Eur. Second. Lang. Assoc. 2018, 2, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Klimova, B. Gao, Q., Zhou, J., Eds.; Cognitive, Mental and Social Benefits of Online Non-native Language Programs for Healthy Older People. In Human Aspects of IT for the Aged Population. Supporting Everyday Life Activities; Springer International Publishing: New York City, NY, USA, 2021; Volume 12787, pp. 251–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Wong, P.C.M.; Ou, J.; Pang, C.W.Y.; Zhang, L.; Tse, C.S.; Lam, L.C.W.; Antoniou, M. Language Training Leads to Global Cognitive Improvement in Older Adults: A Preliminary Study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 2411–2424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Berggren, R.; Nilsson, J.; Brehmer, Y.; Schmiedek, F.; Lövdén, M. Foreign language learning in older age does not improve memory or intelligence: Evidence from a randomized controlled study. Psychol. Aging 2020, 35, 212–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Grossmann, J.A.; Aschenbrenner, S.; Teichmann, B.; Meyer, P. Foreign language learning can improve response inhibition in individuals with lower baseline cognition: Results from a randomized controlled superiority trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2023, 15, 1123185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ware, C.; Damnee, S.; Djabelkhir, L.; Cristancho, V.; Wu, Y.-H.; Benovici, J.; Pino, M.; Rigaud, A.-S. Maintaining Cognitive Functioning in Healthy Seniors with a Technology-Based Foreign Language Program: A Pilot Feasibility Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Kliesch, M.; Pfenninger, S.E.; Wieling, M.; Stark, E.; Meyer, M. Cognitive benefits of learning additional languages in old adulthood? Insights from an intensive longitudinal intervention study. Appl. Linguist. 2022, 43, 653–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Wong, P.C.M.; Ou, J.; Pang, C.W.Y.; Zhang, L.; Tse, C.S.; Lam, L.C.W.; Antoniou, M. Foreign language learning as potential treatment for mild cognitive impairment. Hong Kong Med. J. 2019, 25, 41–43. [Google Scholar]
  25. Keijzer, M.; Brouwer, J.; van der Berg, F.; van den Ploeg, M. Experimental methods to study late-life language learning. In The Routledge Handbook of Experimental Linguistics, 1st ed.; Zufferey, S., Gygax, P., Eds.; Routledge: Milton Park, UK, 2023; pp. 473–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Brouwer, J.; van den Berg, F.; Knooihuizen, R.; Loerts, H.; Keijzer, M. The effects of language learning on cognitive functioning and psychosocial well-being in cognitively healthy older adults: A semi-blind randomized controlled trial. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2025, 32, 270–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Schmand, B.A.; Bakker, D.; Saan, R.J.; Louman, J. De Nederlandse Leestest voor Volwassenen: Een maat voor het premorbide intelligentieniveau. [The Dutch Adult Reading Test: A measure of premorbid intelligence.]. TGG 1991, 22, 15–19. [Google Scholar]
  28. Sikkes, S.A.M.; de Lange-de Klerk, E.S.M.; Pijnenburg, Y.A.L.; Gillissen, F.; Romkes, R.; Knol, D.L.; Uitdehaag, B.M.J.; Scheltens, P. A new informant-based questionnaire for instrumental activities of daily living in dementia. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2012, 8, 536–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Carson, N.; Leach, L.; Murphy, K.J. A re-examination of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) cutoff scores: Re-examination of MoCA cutoff scores. Int. J. Geriat. Psychiatry 2018, 33, 379–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Nijmeijer, S.E.; Van Tol, M.-J.; Aleman, A.; Keijzer, M. Foreign Language Learning as Cognitive Training to Prevent Old Age Disorders? Protocol of a Randomized Controlled Trial of Language Training vs. Musical Training and Social Interaction in Elderly with Subjective Cognitive Decline. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2021, 13, 550180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Reitan, R.M. Trail Making Test. Manual for Administration, Scoring, and Interpretation; Indiana University Press: Bloomington, IN, USA, 1956. [Google Scholar]
  32. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV); Pearson: London, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  33. Meyer, S.R.A.; De Jonge, J.F.M. Visuele Associatietest—Extended: Handleiding; Hogrefe Uitgevers BV: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  34. Nasreddine, Z.S.; Phillips, N.A.; Bédirian, V.; Charbonneau, S.; Whitehead, V.; Collin, I.; Cummings, J.L.; Chertkow, H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005, 53, 695–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Broadbent, D.E.; Cooper, P.F.; FitzGerald, P.; Parkes, K.R. The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1982, 21, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sheikh, J.I.; Yesavage, J.A. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin. Gerontol. J. Aging Ment. Health 1986, 5, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. American Psychiatric Association. SCID-5-S Gestructureerd Klinisch Interview voor DSM-5 Syndroomstoornissen. Nederlandse Vertaling van Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5® Disorders—Clinician Version (SCID-5-CV), First Edition, en User’s Guide to Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5® Disorders–Clinician Version (SCID-5-CV), First Edition en Delen van de Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5® DisordersResearch Version (SCID-5-RV); Boom: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gierveld, J.D.J.; Tilburg, T.V. A 6-Item Scale for Overall, Emotional, and Social Loneliness: Confirmatory Tests on Survey Data. Res. Aging 2006, 28, 582–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Smith, B.W.; Dalen, J.; Wiggins, K.; Tooley, E.; Christopher, P.; Bernard, J. The brief resilience scale: Assessing the ability to bounce back. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2008, 15, 194–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Consten, C.P. Measuring Resilience with the Brief Resilience Scale: Factor Structure, Reliability and Validity of the Dutch Version of the BRS (BRSnl). Bachelor’s Thesis, University of Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands, 2016. Available online: https://purl.utwente.nl/essays/70095 (accessed on 11 September 2025).
  41. Marin, R.S.; Biedrzycki, R.C.; Firinciogullari, S. Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psych. Res. 1991, 38, 143–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Raes, F.; Bijttebier, P. Leuven Adaptation of the Rumination on Sadness Scale (LARSS). Gedragstherapie 2012, 45, 85–87. [Google Scholar]
  43. Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, D.M. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th ed.; (PPVTTM-4); Pearson Education: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  44. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, version 4.2.0; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2023; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 11 September 2025).
  45. Rencher, A.C.; Christensen, W.F. Methods of Multivariate Analysis: Rencher/Methods; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Nucci, M.; Mapelli, D.; Mondini, S. Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire (CRIq): A new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2012, 24, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Lenth, R.V. Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means, Version 1.8.4.-1. 2023. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans (accessed on 11 September 2025).
  48. Long, J.A. Interactions: Comprehensive, User-Friendly Toolkit for Probing Interactions, Version 1.1.5. 2019. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=interactions (accessed on 11 September 2025).
  49. Lüdecke, D.; Ben-Shachar, M.; Patil, I.; Waggoner, P.; Makowski, D. performance: An R Package for Assessment, Comparison and Testing of Statistical Models. J. Open Source Softw. 2021, 6, 3139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Allison, P.D. When Can You Safely Ignore Multicollinearity. Available online: https://statisticalhorizons.com/multicollinearity/ (accessed on 8 September 2025).
  51. De Jong Gierveld, J.; Van Tilburg, T. The De Jong Gierveld short scales for emotional and social loneliness: Tested on data from 7 countries in the UN generations and gender surveys. Eur. J. Ageing 2010, 7, 121–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ong, A.D.; Uchino, B.N.; Wethington, E. Loneliness and Health in Older Adults: A Mini-Review and Synthesis. Gerontology 2016, 62, 443–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Holvast, F.; Burger, H.; de Waal, M.M.W.; van Marwijk, H.W.J.; Comijs, H.C.; Verhaak, P.F.M. Loneliness is associated with poor prognosis in late-life depression: Longitudinal analysis of the Netherlands study of depression in older persons. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 185, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Cai, W.; Mueller, C.; Shetty, H.; Perera, G.; Stewart, R. Predictors of mortality in people with late-life depression: A retrospective cohort study. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 266, 695–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Jeuring, H.W.; Stek, M.L.; Huisman, M.; Oude Voshaar, R.C.; Naarding, P.; Collard, R.M.; van der Mast, R.C.; Kok, R.M.; Beekman, A.T.F.; Comijs, H.C. A Six-Year Prospective Study of the Prognosis and Predictors in Patients with Late-Life Depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psych. 2018, 26, 985–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Forstmeier, S.; Verena Thoma, M.; Maercker, A. Motivational reserve: The role of motivational processes in cognitive impairment and alzheimer’s disease. In Multiple Pathways of Cognitive Aging: Motivational and Contextual Influences, 1st ed.; Sedek, G., Hess, T., Touron, D., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2022; pp. 128–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bilbrey, A.C.; Laidlaw, K.; Cassidy-Eagle, E.; Thompson, L.W.; Gallagher-Thompson, D. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Late-Life Depression: Evidence, Issues, and Recommendations. Cogn. Behav. Pract. 2022, 29, 128–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Zlatar, Z.Z.; Moore, R.C.; Palmer, B.W.; Thompson, W.K.; Jeste, D.V. Cognitive Complaints Correlate with Depression Rather Than Concurrent Objective Cognitive Impairment in the Successful Aging Evaluation Baseline Sample. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2014, 27, 181–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kliesch, M.; Pfenninger, S.E. Cognitive and Socioaffective Predictors of L2 Microdevelopment in Late Adulthood: A Longitudinal Intervention Study. Mod. Lang. J. 2021, 105, 237–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Van der Ploeg, M.; Keijzer, M.; Lowie, W. Language pedagogies and late-life language learning proficiency. Int. Rev. Appl. Ling. Lang. Teach. 2023, 63, 303–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hughes-Morley, A.; Young, B.; Waheed, W.; Small, N.; Bower, P. Factors affecting recruitment into depression trials: Systematic review, meta-synthesis and conceptual framework. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 172, 274–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Intervention outcomes for all cognitive and psychosocial variables stratified by group.
Figure 1. Intervention outcomes for all cognitive and psychosocial variables stratified by group.
Brainsci 15 00991 g001
Figure 2. Intervention outcomes for all English proficiency variables stratified by group.
Figure 2. Intervention outcomes for all English proficiency variables stratified by group.
Brainsci 15 00991 g002
Table 1. Overview of outcome measures, as also reported in [26].
Table 1. Overview of outcome measures, as also reported in [26].
(Sub) DomainMeasureReferencePrimary Outcome
Cognition
Cognitive flexibilityTrail Making Test B (TMT-B)[31]Seconds to complete
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV) digit-symbol substitution[32]Number of symbols in 120 s (0–133)
WAIS-IV letter-number sequencing
(Please note:. Also indexes working memory)
[32]Total correctly recalled sequences (0–21)
Episodic memoryVisual Association Task-Extended (VAT-E) paired association subscale[33]Number of correctly recalled images (1–24)
VAT-E free recall subscale[33]Number of correctly recalled images (1–48)
Global cognitionMontreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)[34]Overall score (0–30)
Subjective cognitive functioningCognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ)[35]Degree of experienced cognitive failures (0–100)
Attention/processing speedTrail Making Task A (TMT-A)[31]Seconds to complete
Working memoryWAIS-IV digit span forward[32]Total correctly recalled sequences (0–12)
WAIS-IV digit span backward[32]Total correctly recalled sequences (0–12)
Executive functioning/verbal fluencyPhonetic verbal fluency (DAT, KOM, or PGR) Total correctly named words
Psychosocial well-being
Depression symptomsGeriatric Depression Scale 15-item version (GDS-15)[36]Depression severity (0–15)

Psychiatric disordersStructured Clinical Interview for the DSM-V (SCID-5)[37]Presence of current or past psychiatric disorders 
LonelinessDe Jong-Gierveld 6-item loneliness scale[38]Social and emotional loneliness (0–3)
ResilienceBrief Resilience ScaleOriginal: [39]
Translation: [40]
Ability to bounce back after setbacks and stressful life events (1–5)
ApathyApathy Evaluation Scale (AES)[41]Total score (18–72)
RuminationLeuven Adaptation of the Rumination on Sadness[42]Total score (21–105)
English proficiency
Lexical accessEnglish Phonetic Verbal Fluency (PRW, CFL, FAS)  Total correctly named words
Vocabulary sizePeabody Picture Vocabulary Naming Test (PPVT-4)[43]Raw score (ceiling item minus number of incorrect answers) (0–228)
Speaking proficiencyInternational English Language Testing System (IELTS) speaking test Overall score on spontaneous (0–9) and prepared speech (0–9)
Listening proficiencyInternational English Language Testing System (IELTS) listening test Overall summed score on listening tests (0–20)
Table 2. Baseline characteristics of participants who completed the intervention stratified by group.
Table 2. Baseline characteristics of participants who completed the intervention stratified by group.
Control Group
Not Depressed in the Past 25 Years or Longer
(n = 15)
Participants with Current Depression or Depression in the Past 25 Years
(n = 19)
p
 M (SD)/n %M (SD)/n % 
Age (years)70.1 (3.8)69.7 (2.9)n.s.
Gender
Women
Men

9 (60%)
5 (40%)

15 (79%)
4 (21%)
n.s.
Current working situationRetired: 11 (73.3%)
Volunteering: 1 (6.7%)
Part-time: 1 (6.7%)
Informal care: 1 (6.7%)
Self-employed: 1 (6.7%)
Retired 17: (89.5%)
Volunteering: 1 (5.3%)
Part-time: 1 (5.3%)
n.s.
MoCA (at screening)26.9 (1.2)25.1 (2.4)<0.05 *
Cognitive reserve144.7 (19.7)131.2 (13.5)<0.05 *
Premorbid intelligence92.5 (8.5)94.0 (8.2)n.s.
Number of participants with GDS ≥ 5 OR current major depressive disorder according to DSM-V criteria at screening09<0.01 **
Average number of hours per week spent on intervention (4.5 h requested by researchers) 5.7 (4.3)5.2 (2) 
Please note: Baseline characteristics were compared using Fisher’s exact test for ategorical data, and Mann–Whitney U tests for continuous data. n.s.: not significant; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Raw data stratified by time and group.
Table 3. Raw data stratified by time and group.
Control Group
(n = 15)
Participants with Current or Past Depression (n = 19)
Cognitive
Measures
PretestPosttestFollowupPretestPosttestFollowup
CFQ31.40 (11.68)32.20 (8.13)30.33 (10.41)46.11 (13.43)39.89 (8.37)37.56 (12.21)
MoCA26.93 (1.87)27.07 (2.25)27.71 (1.14)24.95 (2.46)26.63 (1.67)25.83 (1.69)
VAT-E (paired association)14.33 (4.82)17.33 (6.26)19.07 (5.81)14.58 (6.21)17.63 (6.95)19.72 (4.96)
VAT-E (free recall)24.93 (7.52)28.53 (9.36)29.67 (10.33)20.89 (8.03)25.42 (7.81)25.61 (8.68)
DSST60.67 (7.52)63.38 (9.12)64.67 (9.42)54.06 (14.98)57.68 (17.58)58.33 (13.96)
Digit span forward7.20 (2.18)7.60 (2.69)6.57 (2.03)5.74 (1.41)5.89 (1.73)5.83 (2.04)
Digit span backward7.60 (1.92)8.27 (2.31)7.43 (1.79)6.37 (1.38)6.68 (1.63)6.61 (2.20)
Letter-number sequencing10.93 (2.87)13.53 (3.56)10.36 (1.86)9.47 (1.84)10.00 (2.00)9.72 (1.71)
TMT-A38.73 (8.22)36.93 (6.49)37.40 (7.83)44.79 (12.49)41.84 (12.02)41.39 (12.22)
TMT-B78.53 (19.46)79.93 (26.33)74.53 (19.71)87.26 (24.49)90.95 (35.68)78.50 (16.22)
Dutch verbal fluency46.53 (9.66)47.00 (9.65)52.40 (12.00)44.58 (10.30)44.05 (11.79)45.56 (9.92)
Psychosocial well-beingPretestPosttestFollowupPretestPosttestFollowup
AES26.2 (3.86)28.2 (5.39)25.0 (4.90)31.0 (6.77)27.5 (5.59)30.0 (7.25)
GDS-150.40 (1.30)1.13 (2.56)1.07 (2.81)4.21 (3.55)3.00 (3.46)3.06 (3.24)
ERQ (cognitive reappraisal)28.27 (7.27)26.53 (8.87)24.33 (8.57)24.95 (5.03)25.84 (5.89)23.28 (7.16)
ERQ (expressive suppression)13.00 (4.11)13.47 (4.16)13.67 (6.85)12.47 (5.33)12.00 (5.12)12.61 (4.17)
LARSS32.67 (14.52)28.27 (15.41)33.33 (16.47)43.58 (14.49)42.47 (14.01)44.06 (16.46)
Emotional loneliness0.20 (0.77)0.27 (0.80)0.27 (0.80)1.11 (1.15)1.05 (1.08)1.17 (1.04)
Social loneliness0.27 (0.59)0.27 (0.80)0.47 (1.06)1.89 (1.33)1.16 (0.90)1.17 (1.34)
BRS3.91 (0.43)3.69 (0.69)3.79 (0.62)3.08 (0.55)3.07 (0.51)3.11 (0.56)
Language outcomesPretestPosttestFollowupPretestPosttestFollowup
English verbal fluency26.62 (5.77)36.27 (11.18)N/A23.47 (8.02)28.69 (7.17)N/A
PPVT153.25 (17.72)160.60 (15.01)N/A144.32 (25.93)153.44 (25.30)N/A
IELTS speaking (without preparation)5.07 (1.21)5.53 (1.46)N/A4.16 (1.34)5.25 (1.00)N/A
IELTS speaking (with preparation)5.64 (0.93)6.07 (1.53)N/A4.94 (1.30)5.75 (1.06)N/A
IELTS Listening proficiency15.13 (3.66)16.50 (3.08)N/A13.06 (3.90)16.00 (3.32)N/A
Can-do (productive)3.09 (0.46)3.70 (0.30)N/A3.25 (0.83)3.80 (0.79)N/A
Can-do (receptive)3.55 (0.58)4.01 (0.41)N/A3.62 (0.89)4.03 (0.77)N/A
N/A: English proficiency was assessed before and after the intervention in a separate testing session taking approximately 45 min.
Table 4. Planned comparison results for the significant group × time × variable interactions.
Table 4. Planned comparison results for the significant group × time × variable interactions.
Difference Between Intervention Groups per Time-Point for Psychosocial and Cognitive Outcomes
ContrastGroupVariableestSEtpd
control—(past) depressionpretestAES0.810.332.47<0.050.89
control—(past) depressionpretestBRS1.270.333.89<0.0011.41
control—(past) depressionpretestCFQ1.220.333.74<0.0011.35
control—(past) depressionpretestDigit span (forward)0.710.332.16<0.050.78
control—(past) depressionpretestGDS1.190.333.65<0.0011.32
control—(past) depressionpretestLARSS0.690.332.1<0.050.76
control—(past) depressionpretestloneliness (emotional)0.880.332.68<0.010.97
control—(past) depressionpretestloneliness (social)1.370.334.19<0.0011.52
control—(past) depressionpretestMoCA0.960.332.95<0.011.07
control—(past) depressionposttestBRS0.950.332.91<0.011.05
control—(past) depressionposttestCFQ0.640.331.97<0.050.71
control—(past) depressionposttestDigit span (backward)0.820.332.5<0.050.9
control—(past) depressionposttestDigit span (forward)0.820.332.52<0.050.91
control—(past) depressionposttestLARSS0.890.332.72<0.010.99
control—(past) depressionposttestletter-number seq1.340.334.09<0.0011.48
control—(past) depressionposttestloneliness (emotional)0.760.332.33<0.050.84
control—(past) depressionposttestloneliness (social)0.750.332.3<0.050.83
control—(past) depressionfollowupAES0.830.342.42<0.050.92
control—(past) depressionfollowupBRS1.060.333.2<0.011.17
control—(past) depressionfollowupLARSS0.690.332.09<0.050.77
control—(past) depressionfollowuploneliness (emotional)0.890.332.69<0.010.98
control—(past) depressionfollowupMoCA0.880.342.63<0.010.98
control—(past) depressionfollowupVF0.660.332.01<0.050.74
Differences over time within groups for psychosocial and cognitive outcomes
ContrastGroupVariableestSEtpd
pretest—posttest(past) depressionMoCA−0.830.29−2.83<0.01−0.92
pretest—posttest(past) depressionAES−0.590.3−2<0.05−0.66
pretest—posttest(past) depressionLoneliness (social)−0.640.29−2.17<0.05−0.7
pretest—followup(past) depressionCFQ−0.720.3−2.42<0.05−0.8
pretest—followup(past) depressionVAT-E (paired assoc)−0.850.3−2.86<0.01−0.94
pretest—followup(past) depressionLoneliness (social)−0.620.3−2.09<0.05−0.69
pretest—posttestcontrolLetter-number seq−10.33−3.02<0.01−1.1
pretest—followupcontrolVAT-E (paired assoc)−0.80.33−2.43<0.05−0.89
posttest—followupcontrolLetter-number seq1.230.343.67<0.0011.36
Difference over time between intervention groups for psychosocial and cognitive outcomes
Time (pairwise)Group (pairwise)VariableestSEtpd
pretest—posttestcontrol—(past) depressionAES0.910.442.05<0.051.01
posttest—followupcontrol—(past) depressionAES−0.930.46−2.04<0.05−1.03
posttest—followupcontrol—(past) depressionLetter-number seq1.120.452.5<0.051.24
Difference between groups per time-point for English proficiency outcomes
ContrastTimeVariableestSEtpd
control—(past) depressionpretestIELTS (listening)0.70.332.16<0.050.93
control—(past) depressionpretestIELTS
(speaking w/o prep)
0.720.322.22<0.050.95
control—(past) depressionpretestIELTS
(speaking with prep)
0.650.331.98<0.050.86
control—(past) depressionposttestVF0.90.332.75<0.011.2
Differences over time within group for English proficiency outcomes
ContrastGroupVariableestSEtpd
pretest—posttest(past) depressionIELTS (listening)−0.770.29−2.63<0.01−1.03
pretest—posttest(past) depressionIELTS
(speaking w/o prep)
−0.80.26−3.12<0.01−1.06
pretest—posttest(past) depressionIELTS
(speaking with prep)
−0.680.26−2.61<0.01−0.9
pretest—posttest(past) depressionVF−0.540.26−2.12<0.05−0.72
pretest—posttest(past) depressionCan-do (productive)−0.790.25−3.16<0.01−1.04
pretest—posttest(past) depressionCan-do (receptive)−0.610.24−2.5<0.05−0.81
pretest—posttestcontrolVF−1.030.29−3.59<0.001−1.36
pretest—posttestcontrolCan-do (productive)−0.90.28−3.28<0.01−1.2
pretest—posttestcontrolCan-do (receptive)−0.660.28−2.4<0.05−0.88
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Brouwer, J.; van den Berg, F.; Knooihuizen, R.; Loerts, H.; Keijzer, M. Language Learning as a Non-Pharmacological Intervention in Older Adults with (Past) Depression. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 991. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15090991

AMA Style

Brouwer J, van den Berg F, Knooihuizen R, Loerts H, Keijzer M. Language Learning as a Non-Pharmacological Intervention in Older Adults with (Past) Depression. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(9):991. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15090991

Chicago/Turabian Style

Brouwer, Jelle, Floor van den Berg, Remco Knooihuizen, Hanneke Loerts, and Merel Keijzer. 2025. "Language Learning as a Non-Pharmacological Intervention in Older Adults with (Past) Depression" Brain Sciences 15, no. 9: 991. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15090991

APA Style

Brouwer, J., van den Berg, F., Knooihuizen, R., Loerts, H., & Keijzer, M. (2025). Language Learning as a Non-Pharmacological Intervention in Older Adults with (Past) Depression. Brain Sciences, 15(9), 991. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15090991

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop