Spoonerism Beyond Language: A Multi-Componential Perspective on Phonological Awareness
Abstract
1. Introduction
Modularity Level | Examples | Impairments | Key Features |
---|---|---|---|
First-level modules | Visual and auditory frequency perception, motor reflexes | Agnosia, auditory agnosia | Genetically innate; fully encapsulated; domain-specific; mandatory operation [29]. |
Second-level modules | Object recognition, first-level language, walking | Aphasia, basic agnosias | Genetically predisposed but require environmental input; partially encapsulated; domain-specific; semi-mandatory operation. |
Third-level modules | Reading, complex motor skills (e.g., dancing) | Dyslexia, alexia, apraxia, agraphia | Assembled through experience and hyper-learning; during development, they depend on executive and attentional control (CEN); exhibit low encapsulation; interact across domains; show functional specialization, semi-mandatory operation. |
Additional Characteristics of the Model
A minimal degree of mandatoriness—even if partial or context-dependent—is a necessary condition for classifying a complex system as modular. |
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Measures
Working Memory Capacity Tasks
3. Results
3.1. Step 1
3.2. Step 2
4. Discussion
5. Limitations and Future Perspectives
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Wagner, R.K.; Torgesen, J.K. The Nature of Phonological Processing and Its Causal Role in the Acquisition of Reading Skills. Psychol. Bull. 1987, 101, 192–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anthony, J.L.; Francis, D.J. Development of Phonological Awareness. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 14, 255–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ziegler, J.C.; Goswami, U. Reading Acquisition, Developmental Dyslexia, and Skilled Reading Across Languages: A Psycholinguistic Grain Size Theory. Psychol. Bull. 2005, 131, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anthony, J.L.; Lonigan, C.J. The Nature of Phonological Awareness: Converging Evidence from Four Studies of Preschool and Early Grade School Children. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 96, 43–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mann, V. Phonological Awareness and Early Reading Ability: One Perspective. In Phonological Awareness in Reading; Sawyer, D.J., Fox, B.J., Eds.; Springer Series in Language and Communication; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1991; Volume 28, pp. 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ball, E.W. Phonological awareness—What’s important and to whom? Read. Writ. 1993, 5, 141–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knoop-van Campen, C.A.N.; Segers, E.; Verhoeven, L. How phonological awareness mediates the relation between working memory and word reading efficiency in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia 2018, 24, 156–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gray, S.; Green, S.; Alt, M.; Hogan, T.; Kuo, T.; Brinkley, S.; Cowan, N. The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence. J. Mem. Lang. 2017, 92, 183–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A.; Hitch, G.; Allen, R. A Multicomponent Model of Working Memory. In Working Memory: State of the Science; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 10–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hitch, G.J.; Allen, R.J.; Baddeley, A.D. The multicomponent model of working memory fifty years on. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2025, 78, 222–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradley, L.; Bryant, P.E. Categorizing sounds and learning to read-A causal connection. Nature 1983, 301, 419–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snowling, M.J. Dyslexia. 2009. Available online: https://www.wiley.com/en-us/Dyslexia%2C+2nd+Edition-p-9780631205746 (accessed on 31 July 2025).
- Vellutino, F.R.; Fletcher, J.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Scanlon, D.M. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2004, 45, 2–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Lyster, S.A.H.; Hulme, C. Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 322–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scarborough, H.S. Very Early Language Deficits in Dyslexic Children. Child. Dev. 1990, 61, 1728–1743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Catts, H.W.; Fey, M.E.; Zhang, X.; Tomblin, J.B. Estimating the Risk of Future Reading Difficulties in Kindergarten Children. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2001, 32, 38–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramus, F. Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2003, 13, 212–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennington, B.F. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 2006, 101, 385–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valdois, S.; Bosse, M.L.; Tainturier, M.J. The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia 2004, 10, 339–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bosse, M.L.; Tainturier, M.J.; Valdois, S. Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition 2007, 104, 198–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Di Filippo, G.; Zoccolotti, P.; Ziegler, J.C. Rapid naming deficits in dyslexia: A stumbling block for the perceptual anchor theory of dyslexia. Dev. Sci. 2008, 11, F40–F47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lallier, M.; Tainturier, M.J.; Dering, B.; Donnadieu, S.; Valdois, S.; Thierry, G. Behavioral and ERP evidence for amodal sluggish attentional shifting in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 4125–4135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grainger, J.; Lété, B.; Bertand, D.; Dufau, S.; Ziegler, J.C. Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition 2012, 123, 280–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ziegler, J.C.; Castel, C.; Pech-Georgel, C.; George, F.; Alario, F.X.; Perry, C. Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition 2008, 107, 151–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennington, B.F.; Santerre-Lemmon, L.; Rosenberg, J.; MacDonald, B.; Boada, R.; Friend, A.; Leopold, D.; Daniel, R.; Samuelsson, S.; Byrne, B.; et al. Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2012, 121, 212–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F. Developmental dyslexia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2015, 11, 283–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castles, A.; Coltheart, M. Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition 2004, 91, 77–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakhill, J.; Kyle, F. The Relation between Phonological Awareness and Working Memory. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2000, 75, 152–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fodor, J.A. The Modularity of Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, M.L. Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 2010, 33, 245–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrett, H.C.; Kurzban, R. Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 628–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ritchie, J.B.; Carruthers, P. Massive modularity is consistent with most forms of neural reuse. Behav. Brain Sci. 2010, 33, 289–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rizzolatti, G.; Craighero, L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 27, 169–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, J.B.; Hirsch, S.B.; Vogel-Farley, V.K.; Redcay, E.; Nelson, C.A. Eye-tracking, autonomic, and electrophysiological correlates of emotional face processing in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 188–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, L.; Wassermann, D.; Abrams, D.A.; Kochalka, J.; Gallardo-Diez, G.; Menon, V. The visual word form area (VWFA) is part of both language and attention circuitry. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 5601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mahon, B.Z.; Cantlon, J.F. The specialization of function: Cognitive and neural perspectives. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2011, 28, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rabaglia, C.D.; Marcus, G.F.; Lane, S.P. What can individual differences tell us about the specialization of function? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2011, 28, 288–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moscovitch, M.; Umilta, C. Modularity and Neuropsychology: Modules and Central Processes in Attention and Memory. In Modular Deficits in Alzheimer-Type Dementia; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1990; pp. 1–59. [Google Scholar]
- Benso, F.; Chiorri, C.; Ardu, E.; Venuti, P.; Pasqualotto, A. Beyond modular and non-modular states: Theoretical considerations, exemplifications, and practical implications. Front. Psychol. 2025, 16, 1456587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dosenbach, N.U.F.; Fair, D.A.; Miezin, F.M.; Cohen, A.L.; Wenger, K.K.; Dosenbach, R.A.; Fox, M.D.; Snyder, A.Z.; Vincent, J.L.; Raichle, M.E.; et al. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 11073–11078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hikosaka, O.; Isoda, M. Switching from automatic to controlled behavior: Cortico-basal ganglia mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 154–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Menon, V. Salience Network. Brain Mapp. Encycl. Academic Press. 2015, 2, 597–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCarthy, K.M.; Skoruppa, K. Language-specific phonological skills and the relationship with reading accuracy in Sylheti-English sequential bilinguals. Child. Dev. 2023, 94, e85–e102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vender, M.; Melloni, C. Phonological Awareness across Child Populations: How Bilingualism and Dyslexia Interact. Languages 2021, 6, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benso, F.; Clavarezza, V.; Caria, A.; Chiorri, C. Validazione di un modello multicomponenziale della lettura. Dislessia-G. Ital. Ric. Clin. E Appl. 2013, 10, 39–65. [Google Scholar]
- Varvara, P.; Varuzza, C.; Sorrentino, A.C.P.; Vicari, S.; Menghini, D. Executive functions in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 67775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 254–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child. Neuropsychol. 2002, 8, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benso, F.; Santoro, G.M.; Ardu, E. MEA, Measures of Executive Attention. 2019. Available online: https://www.hogrefe.it/i-nostri-progetti/lavori-corso/mea-measures-of-executive-attention/ (accessed on 31 July 2025).
- Engle, R.W.; Kane, M.J. Executive Attention, Working Memory Capacity, and A Two-Factor Theory of Cognitive Control. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. Adv. Res. Theory 2004, 44, 145. [Google Scholar]
- Palladino, P.; Cornoldi, C.; De Beni, R.; Pazzaglia, F. Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Mem. Cogn. 2001, 29, 344–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marotta, L.; Ronchetti Claudia Trasciani, M.; Vicari, S. Test CMF-Valutazione Delle Competenze Metafonologiche. Available online: https://www.erickson.it/it/test-cmf-valutazione-delle-competenze-metafonologiche (accessed on 31 July 2025).
- Rosen, V.M.; Engle, R.W. The Role of Working Memory Capacity in Retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1997, 126, 211–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Troyer, A.K.; Moscovitch, M.; Winocur, G. Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology 1997, 11, 138–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lis, A.; Di Nuovo, S. Test della grande figura complessa di Rey. Traduzione italiana. Organ. Spec. Firenze Italy 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Hammill, D.; Pearson, N.; Voress, J. Test TPV: Test Di Percezione Visiva e Integrazione Visuo-Motoria. Available online: https://www.ibs.it/tpv-test-di-percezione-visiva-libro-vari/e/9788879461146?srsltid=AfmBOor7-HjKhdHVIR1Qoz0xx0DV5asvCgCp75y7BKw-lnmNu2mmd_B7 (accessed on 31 July 2025).
- Draheim, C.; Tsukahara, J.S.; Martin, J.D.; Mashburn, C.A.; Engle, R.W. A toolbox approach to improving the measurement of attention control. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2021, 150, 242–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J.; Cohen, P.; West, S.G.; Aiken, L.S. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 1–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epskamp, S.; Fried, E.I. A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol. Methods 2018, 23, 617–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epskamp, S.; Waldorp, L.J.; Mõttus, R.; Borsboom, D. The Gaussian Graphical Model in Cross-Sectional and Time-Series Data. Multivariate Behav. Res. 2018, 53, 453–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liu, H.; Lafferty, J.; Wasserman, L.; Wainwright, M.J. The Nonparanormal: Semiparametric Estimation of High Dimensional Undirected Graphs. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 2009, 10, 2295–2328. [Google Scholar]
- Beneventi, H.; TØNnessen, F.E.; Ersland, L.; Hugdahl, K. Executive working memory processes in dyslexia: Behavioral and fMRI evidence. Scand. J. Psychol. 2010, 51, 192–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Franceschini, S.; Gori, S.; Ruffino, M.; Pedrolli, K.; Facoetti, A. A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, 814–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Catts, H.W.; Petscher, Y. A Cumulative Risk and Resilience Model of Dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 2022, 55, 171–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conway, A.R.A.; Kane, M.J.; Bunting, M.F.; Hambrick, D.Z.; Wilhelm, O.; Engle, R.W. Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2005, 12, 769–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, R.W.; Tuholski, S.W.; Laughlin, J.E.; Conway, A.R.A. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1999, 128, 309–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kane, M.J.; Conway, A.R.A.; Bleckley, M.K.; Engle, R.W. A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2001, 130, 169–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. Working memory. Science 1992, 255, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hale, J.B.; Hoeppner, J.A.B.; Fiorello, C.A. Analyzing Digit Span Components for Assessment of Attention Processes. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2002, 20, 128–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craig, M.; Dewar, M. Rest-related consolidation protects the fine detail of new memories. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 6857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- D’Esposito, M.; Postle, B.R.; Rypma, B. Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: Evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp. Brain Res. 2000, 133, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oberauer, K.; Süß, H.M.; Schulze, R.; Wilhelm, O.; Wittmann, W.W. Working memory capacity-Facets of a cognitive ability construct. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2000, 29, 1017–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, N.; Jones, D.M. Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. Br. J. Psychol. 1990, 81, 111–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmiedek, F.; Hildebrandt, A.; Lövdén, M.; Wilhelm, O.; Lindenberger, U. Complex Span Versus Updating Tasks of Working Memory: The Gap Is Not That Deep. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2009, 35, 1089–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magimairaj, B.M.; Montgomery, J.W. Examining the Relative Contribution of Memory Updating, Attention Focus Switching, and Sustained Attention to Children’s Verbal Working Memory Span. Child. Dev. Res. 2013, 2013, 763808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carruthers, P. The Architecture of the Mind; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pasqualotto, A.; Venuti, P. Predictors of Reading and Spelling Difficulties in Italian Children: Specific Language and General Cognitive Skills. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2025, 40, 17–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tilanus, E.A.T.; Segers, E.; Verhoeven, L. Diagnostic profiles of children with developmental dyslexia in a transparent orthography. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 4194–4202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ding, N.; Peng, P.; Tang, J.; Ding, Y.; Zhao, J. An investigation of phonological predictors in Chinese developmental dyslexia using a machine learning approach. In Reading and Writing; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2025; pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stein, J.F.; Zoccolotti, P. Success Is Not the Entire Story for a Scientific Theory: The Case of the Phonological Deficit Theory of Dyslexia. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramus, F.; Szenkovits, G. What phonological deficit? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2008, 61, 129–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyytinen, H.; Guttorm, T.K.; Huttunen, T.; Hämäläinen, J.; Leppänen, P.H.T.; Vesterinen, M. Psychophysiology of developmental dyslexia: A review of findings including studies of children at risk for dyslexia. J. NeuroLinguist. 2005, 18, 167–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Group | Statistics | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Response Variable | Low-WMC | High-WMC | ||||||||||
n | M | SD | Range | n | M | SD | Range | t(df) | p | adj-p | d | |
SPO | 20 | 14.45 | 7.92 | 1-29 | 29 | 26.28 | 3.57 | 17–30 | −6.257 (24.36) | <0.001 | <0.001 | 1.92 |
VFT | 20 | 16.95 | 6.09 | 9–29 | 29 | 25.31 | 7.25 | 9–42 | −4.266 (47) | <0.001 | <0.001 | 1.28 |
FFT | 19 | 29.42 | 12.73 | 11–47 | 29 | 40.76 | 12.76 | 12–64 | −3.012 (46) | 0.004 | 0.010 | 0.73 |
REY | 20 | 26.93 | 5.39 | 14.50–35 | 29 | 30.66 | 3.94 | 20.50–36 | −2.801 (47) | 0.007 | 0.013 | 0.79 |
TPV | 20 | 33.60 | 3.52 | 26–38 | 29 | 35.76 | 3.64 | 25–40 | −2.068 (47) | 0.044 | 0.062 | 0.60 |
FET | 20 | 63.64 | 9.74 | 49–88 | 29 | 68.02 | 10.38 | 48–94 | −1.489 (47) | 0.143 | 0.167 | 0.43 |
RNN | 20 | 14.90 | 4.17 | 9–23 | 29 | 14.17 | 3.30 | 10–23 | 0.682 (47) | 0.499 | 0.499 | 0.20 |
Group | Statistics | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Response Variable | Low-WMC | High-WMC | ||||||||||
n | M | SD | Range | n | M | SD | Range | t(df) | p | adj-p | d | |
SPO | 41 | 17.61 | 6.98 | 1–29 | 23 | 27,87 | 2.47 | 20–30 | −8.510 (54.93) | <0.001 | <001 | 1.96 |
VFT | 41 | 19.07 | 5.76 | 9–34 | 23 | 23.04 | 8.05 | 9–42 | −2.287 (62) | 0.026 | 0.060 | 0.57 |
FFT | 40 | 32.45 | 12.64 | 11–51 | 23 | 42.30 | 9.55 | 23–59 | −3.493 (56.37) | 0.001 | 0.003 | 0.88 |
REY | 41 | 28.11 | 5.12 | 14.50–36 | 23 | 28.78 | 4.57 | 17.5–36 | −0.524 (62) | 0.602 | 0.602 | 0.14 |
TPV | 41 | 35.02 | 2.98 | 27–40 | 23 | 35.83 | 3.66 | 26–40 | −0.950 (62) | 0.346 | 0.484 | 0.24 |
FET | 41 | 65.22 | 10.10 | 49–94 | 23 | 63.44 | 12.99 | 42–94 | 0.609 (62) | 0.545 | 0.602 | 0.15 |
RNN | 41 | 15.24 | 4.13 | 9–28 | 23 | 14.22 | 3.07 | 10–20 | 1.039 (62) | 0.303 | 0.484 | 0.28 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Benso, F.; Mazzoni, N.; Chiorri, C.; Ardu, E.; Venuti, P.; Pasqualotto, A. Spoonerism Beyond Language: A Multi-Componential Perspective on Phonological Awareness. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 878. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080878
Benso F, Mazzoni N, Chiorri C, Ardu E, Venuti P, Pasqualotto A. Spoonerism Beyond Language: A Multi-Componential Perspective on Phonological Awareness. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(8):878. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080878
Chicago/Turabian StyleBenso, Francesco, Noemi Mazzoni, Carlo Chiorri, Eleonora Ardu, Paola Venuti, and Angela Pasqualotto. 2025. "Spoonerism Beyond Language: A Multi-Componential Perspective on Phonological Awareness" Brain Sciences 15, no. 8: 878. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080878
APA StyleBenso, F., Mazzoni, N., Chiorri, C., Ardu, E., Venuti, P., & Pasqualotto, A. (2025). Spoonerism Beyond Language: A Multi-Componential Perspective on Phonological Awareness. Brain Sciences, 15(8), 878. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080878