Next Article in Journal
The Influence of Therapy Enriched with the Erigo®Pro Table and Motor Imagery on the Body Balance of Patients After Stroke—A Randomized Observational Study
Previous Article in Journal
Editorial for Brain Sciences Special Issue “Advances in Restorative Neurotherapeutic Technologies”
Previous Article in Special Issue
Psychiatric Disorders and Cognitive Fluctuations in Parkinson’s Disease: Changing Approaches in the First Decades of the 21st Century
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive–Motor Coupling in Multiple Sclerosis: Do Chronological Age and Physical Activity Matter?

by
Brenda Jeng
,
Peixuan Zheng
and
Robert W. Motl
*
Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition, University of Illinois Chicago, Chicago, IL 60612, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2025, 15(3), 274; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15030274
Submission received: 7 January 2025 / Revised: 26 February 2025 / Accepted: 27 February 2025 / Published: 5 March 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue From Bench to Bedside: Motor–Cognitive Interactions—2nd Edition)

Abstract

:
Background: People with multiple sclerosis (MS) often demonstrate both cognitive and physical dysfunctions, particularly with greater age and lower physical activity levels, and there is evidence of a relationship between these outcomes (i.e., cognitive–motor coupling) in MS. To date, little is known about cognitive–motor coupling when controlling for chronological age and levels of physical activity. Objectives: We examined cognitive–motor coupling in people with MS while accounting for chronological age and physical activity. Methods: The sample included 290 people with MS between the ages of 22 and 77 years. Participants underwent the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) for cognitive processing speed and the California Verbal Learning and Memory Test–Second Edition (CVLT-II) for verbal learning and memory. Participants completed the 6-Minute Walk and the Timed 25-Foot Walk tests for walking endurance and speed, respectively. Participants wore an accelerometer for a 7-day period to measure moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA). Results: The bivariate correlation analyses indicated that cognitive function had moderate-to-strong associations with motor function (range of rs between 0.433 and 0.459). The linear regression analyses indicated cognitive–motor coupling between SDMT and motor function (with a range of β between 0.139 and 0.145) when controlling for demographic and clinical characteristics. The regression analyses further indicated that the CVLT-II was associated with motor function (with a range of β between 0.125 and 0.135) when controlling for demographic and clinical characteristics. When age and MVPA were entered into the regression analyses, SDMT was still associated with the motor function of individuals (β = 0.119), and CVLT-II was still associated with the motor function of individuals (with a range of β between 0.115 and 0.124). Conclusions: Cognitive–motor coupling is present in people with MS independent of chronological age and levels of physical activity. This warrants further investigation of the underlying mechanism and potential approaches for the management of co-occurring MS-related dysfunction.

1. Introduction

Cognitive and walking dysfunctions are prevalent, disabling, and often co-occurring for inter-related consequences of multiple sclerosis (MS) [1]. The relationship between cognitive and motor functions in MS has been termed cognitive–motor coupling [2]. The presence of cognitive–motor coupling was first reported in a study of people with MS and controls wherein cognitive processing speed, based on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), was independently associated with walking speed, based on the Timed 25-Foot Walk (T25FW) test, and the association was stronger in MS than controls [2]. Other researchers have reported cognitive–motor coupling based on associations between SDMT and T25FW as well as the 6-Minute Walk test (6MW; walking endurance) performance in people with MS [3]. This strong relationship may suggest that both cognitive and motor functions involve the particular overlap of brain regions subserving these functions in MS [4,5,6] that might further be associated with age and/or physical activity. There is value in understanding cognitive–motor coupling [7,8] as possibly an age-related issue and identifying physical activity as a potential modifiable behavior with value in mitigating this phenomenon.
To date, the studies that have examined cognitive–motor coupling in MS have primarily involved young and middle-aged samples, yet there is a demographic shift such that MS is present across the entire lifespan, particularly in older adults [9]. Some research has focused on cognitive–motor coupling in older adults with MS and across the lifespan of individuals. For example, one study recruited a sample of older adults with MS and controls matched by age and sex and reported moderate-to-strong relationships between SDMT and T25FW as well as 6MW in MS (i.e., cognitive–motor coupling) but no relationships in controls [10]. Another study examined physical and cognitive function across the lifespan in MS and controls and identified moderate-to-strong relationships in MS but weak relationships in controls [10]. This study further detected strong relationships between cognitive and motor performance in young and middle-aged samples with MS [11].
To date, there has been a dearth of research examining physical activity as an underlying factor for explaining cognitive–motor coupling in MS, despite a conceptual paper that argues for such an inquiry based on proposed synergistic effects of physical activity on cognitive and motor outcomes [12]. This is intriguing, as the first study on cognitive–motor coupling in MS identified physical activity and exercise behavior as possible explanations for the association between variables based on the putative effects on underlying brain parameters [2]. To that end, cardiorespiratory fitness, a physiological surrogate of participating in regular aerobic physical activity, accounted for the association between cognitive and motor outcomes [13,14], whereas deep grey matter brain structures (e.g., thalamus) did not in a small sample of young and middle-aged people with MS [13]. Importantly, cardiorespiratory fitness is a characteristic that reflects the contributions of physical activity behavior in individuals, but it further reflects genetic and other lifestyle variables and is not directly amenable to modifications through interventions, unlike physical activity behavior [15,16].
The current study examined cognitive–motor coupling in people with MS while accounting for chronological age and physical activity. Chronological age might represent a confounding variable, as both walking and cognitive function decline with increasing age in MS [11]. Physical activity might account for cognitive–motor coupling based on conceptual arguments [2,12] and recent evidence linking cardiorespiratory fitness with the association between cognitive and motor outcomes in MS [13,14].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This cross-sectional study involved a secondary analysis of data from 5 studies [10,17,18,19,20]. Participants with MS in those studies were included based on the following criteria: (1) MS diagnosis; (2) relapse-free in the last 30 days; (3) an age of at least 18 years; (4) ability to walk with or without an assistive device; and (5) willingness to complete the assessments.

2.2. Outcome Measures

In the aforementioned studies, demographic information was collected using a laboratory-standardized questionnaire, including age, sex, years of education, and race. Race was dichotomized into Caucasian and non-Caucasian categories. Participants with MS further provided clinical information, including disease duration. Ambulatory disability status was assessed using the one-item Patient-Determined Disease Steps (PDDS) scale [21]. PDDS scale scores have been strongly correlated with scores from the Expanded Disability Status Scale [21], and therefore, the PDDS has been identified as a reasonable surrogate for capturing disability status in MS [22].
The SDMT and CVLT-II assessed cognitive processing speed and verbal learning and memory, respectively. For the SDMT, participants were presented with a series of symbols and instructed to verbally indicate the digit that was paired with each symbol according to the key located at the top of the page [23]. Participants were instructed to work as quickly as possible, and we recorded the total correct responses in 90 s. For the CVLT-II, the examiner read a list of 16 words aloud, and participants were instructed to recall as many of the words as possible, in any order, and this process was repeated for another 4 trials [24]. We recorded the total correct responses from the five trials for a sum.
The 6MW and T25FW assessed walking endurance and speed, respectively. For the 6MW, participants were instructed to walk as far and as fast as possible within the limits of their stability in a 6-min period [25]. We recorded the total distance covered in meters. For the T25FW, participants were instructed to walk a marked 25-foot course as quickly and safely as possible [26]. We recorded the time to complete each of the two trials and averaged the time of the two trials to calculate speed.
Free-living physical activity was measured using an ActiGraph GT3X+ accelerometer (ActiGraph LLC, Pensacola, FL, USA). We placed the accelerometer in a pouch on a belt, and participants wore the belt around their waist with the accelerometer placed above the non-dominant hip. Participants further wore the device for 7 days during waking hours except during water-based activities (i.e., showering, swimming) and recorded periods of wear time in a daily log; this was inspected to verify the wear time during data processing. The data were downloaded and processed into one-minute epochs with low-frequency extension in ActiLife, version 6 [27]. Data were included based on a daily total wear time of at least 600 min (i.e., 10 h); participants with at least one valid day of data were included in the analyses. The outcome of interest was time spent undertaking moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA; minutes/day).

2.3. Procedures

The studies were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the procedures of the 5 studies were approved by appropriate Institutional Review Boards. All participants provided written consent prior to data collection. Participants provided demographic and clinical information and then completed assessments of cognitive and motor function. We further provided participants with an accelerometer on a belt, a daily log, and a pre-stamped, pre-addressed envelope for return service via the United States Postal Service at the end of the visit.

2.4. Data Analysis

The data were analyzed using SPSS, version 29 (SPSS, IBM Statistics, Armonk, NY, USA). We provided descriptive statistics for demographic, clinical, and physical activity variables, as well as cognitive and motor assessments. We then examined associations among cognitive (SDMT, CVLT-II) and motor (6MW, T25FW) outcomes, along with the demographic, clinical, and physical activity variables, using Spearman’s rank-order bivariate correlations [28]. We finally performed a series of stepwise, linear regression analyses. We regressed motor function (6MW or T25FW) based on demographic characteristics (sex, years of education, and race) in Step 1, clinical characteristics (disease duration and PDDS) in Step 2, cognitive function (SDMT or CVLT-II) in Step 3 (i.e., cognitive–motor coupling), and age and physical activity in Step 4 (i.e., independence of cognitive–motor coupling).

3. Results

We provide descriptive statistics of participant characteristics in Table 1. The mean age of participants was 53.0 (13.3) years, and the sample consisted mainly of females (76%) and Caucasians (65%). Participants had a mean disease duration of 14.6 (9.6) years and a median PDDS score of two (IQR of three). On average, the sample engaged in 19.9 (20.2) min/day of MVPA.
The descriptive statistics for outcomes of cognitive and motor function are presented in Table 2. The sample spanned greater ranges in age, education level, race, and disease duration but had a similar ratio of females to males, MS type, and disability level, compared with the initial study on cognitive–motor coupling in MS [2].
The bivariate correlations among outcomes of cognitive function, motor function, age, physical activity, and demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 3. The results indicate that SDMT scores had moderate-to-strong associations with 6MW (rs = 0.459) and T25FW (rs = 0.433), whereas CVLT-II scores had moderate associations with 6MW (rs = 0.370) and T25FW (rs = 0.322). Of note, age and MVPA were both associated with cognitive and motor outcomes, supporting the linear regression analyses examining cognitive–motor coupling while accounting for chronological age and physical activity.
The linear regression analyses are provided in Table 4, Table 5, Table 6 and Table 7. The linear regression analyses indicate cognitive–motor coupling between SDMT and 6MW (β = 0.139, p = 0.003) and SDMT and T25FW (β = 0.145, p = 0.010) when controlling for sex, years of education, race, disease duration, and disability status (Table 4 and Table 5). Moreover, CVLT-II scores were associated with both 6MW (β = 0.135, p = 0.004) and T25FW (β = 0.125, p = 0.019) when controlling for demographic and clinical characteristics (Table 6 and Table 7). When age and MVPA were entered into the regression analyses, the associations remained significant between SDMT with 6MW and T25FW (β = 0.119 and 0.119, p = 0.012 and 0.036, respectively), as well as between CVLT-II with 6MW and T25FW (β = 0.124 and 0.115, p = 0.006 and 0.029, respectively).

4. Discussion

This cross-sectional study examined cognitive–motor coupling while accounting for chronological age and physical activity in people with MS. Our bivariate correlation analyses indicated significant associations among cognitive and motor function outcomes. These relationships were further confirmed by the linear regression analyses, even after controlling for sex, years of education, race, disease duration, and disability status. Notably, the relationships among cognitive and motor function outcomes remained significant when accounting for age and MVPA in the linear regression models. These results suggest that (1) chronological age does not represent a confounding variable, despite its association with the decline in both physical and cognitive function in MS [11], and (2) physical activity does not account for cognitive–motor coupling, despite conceptual arguments [2,12] and recent evidence of cardiorespiratory fitness accounting for the association between cognitive and motor outcomes [13,14].
There is consistent evidence of cognitive–motor coupling based on bivariate correlations between performance scores for the measures of cognitive and motor function in people with MS. For example, multiple studies have reported moderate associations between cognitive processing speed and motor function outcomes in people with MS [2,3,10], and some studies have reported small-to-moderate or no association between verbal learning and memory and motor function outcomes [2,10]. Our bivariate correlation analyses further revealed significant relationships among cognitive and motor function outcomes in people with MS. This finding is notable, as it establishes cognitive–motor coupling in a manner consistent with previous research before examining the influences of age and physical activity. This replication of cognitive–motor coupling in MS is further important given recent and broad concerns regarding the reproducibility of results in science [29,30].
The presence of cognitive–motoring coupling has been established when controlling for a range of demographic and clinical variables in MS [2,10]. For example, the original paper on cognitive–motoring coupling in MS accounted for demographic (sex, years of education) and clinical (disease duration, disability status) characteristics [2]. We accounted for a similar set of variables as well as race when examining cognitive–motor coupling in MS, as race is an important covariate based on its association with cognitive and motor outcomes in MS [31]. We observed consistent associations among cognitive and motor outcomes even after controlling for demographic (sex, years of education, race) and clinical (disease duration, disability status) characteristics in the current study. This replication demonstrated the independence of cognitive–motor coupling from demographic and clinical variables in MS, and this is also critical given concerns regarding the reproducibility of the results in science [29].
Our results further indicate that cognitive–motor coupling remained even after accounting for age in people with MS. This finding is important, as increasing age has consistently been associated with worse cognitive and motor functions in MS [11,17], making age a potential confounder of cognitive–motor coupling. Some research studies have reported a significant relationship between cognitive and motor outcomes among older adults with MS [10] as well as young and middle-aged adults with MS [11]. Interestingly, we provide evidence that age is associated with cognitive and motor outcomes in this study, but this did not account for cognitive–motor coupling in the regression analyses. This suggests that the presence of cognitive–motor coupling independent of age and the worsening of cognitive and motor functions with increases in age in MS [11] does not explain the observed association between cognitive and motor outcomes.
The current study focused on physical activity and its influence on cognitive–motor coupling in people with MS. This is important, as physical activity has consistently been associated with better cognitive and motor functions in MS [17,32], and there are further conceptual arguments [2,12] that support such an examination [13,14]. Our results indicate that physical activity is associated with both cognitive and motor outcomes based on the bivariate analyses, but it does not account for cognitive–motor coupling in people with MS when included in the regression analyses. This seemingly runs counter with a recent report which states that cardiorespiratory fitness, a physiological surrogate of regular aerobic physical activity participation, accounts for the association between cognitive and motor outcomes [13,14], whereas deep grey matter brain structures (e.g., thalamus) do not, in a small sample of young and middle-aged people with MS [13]. Perhaps the results of that study reflected the small sample and/or the focus on a physiological characteristic of a person as a surrogate of physical activity, and our results with larger sample sizes and the direct measurement of physical activity might argue against the involvement of physical activity in cognitive–motor coupling in MS.
We acknowledge several limitations of our research. One limitation was the cross-sectional design of the study, as it does not permit inferences on causality. This limitation could be overcome in future research by examining longitudinal changes in cognitive–motor coupling as a function of disease progression. Another limitation was the lack of data on MS-related symptoms such as depression, as this has been associated with cognitive and motor performance in MS [33] and might account for cognitive–motor coupling. We did not examine the influence of comorbid conditions (i.e., cardiovascular disease, osteoarthritis) or medications, and these might independently or concurrently influence cognitive and motor function.
There are multiple avenues for future research on cognitive–motor coupling in MS. One potential avenue for future research may include examining the associations between cognitive [34] and physical [35] reserve, either separately or combined, with cognitive–motor coupling in MS. There is evidence that cognitive and physical reserve are associated with cognitive and motor outcomes and that people with MS who have both lower cognitive and physical reserves demonstrate significantly worse cognitive and motor outcomes [36]. This pattern suggests that having a higher cognitive or physical reserve, or both, may have protective effects on cognitive–motor coupling in people with MS. Another avenue for future research includes identifying magnetic resonance imaging metrics such as global brain atrophy or lesion burden and location as correlates of cognitive–motor coupling [4,37,38]. One other direction may include the development of screening tools that are valid for detecting cognitive–motor deficits, particularly in the early stages of the disease, where deficits may precede disease manifestations and become sensitive to changes over time [8,39,40]. Other research directions may explore modifiable approaches beyond physical activity for managing the relationship between cognitive and motor functions in this population, as deficits in cognitive–motor coupling can interfere with daily tasks such as talking while walking or driving [41,42]. One final research avenue might involve examining the relationship between cognitive–motor coupling and fall risk [43], as this might inform future fall prevention programs for researchers and clinicians [41].

5. Conclusions

Cognitive–motor coupling is consistently present in samples of people with MS, and we report here, for the first time, that this is independent of chronological age and physical activity as well as other demographic and clinical variables. This warrants further investigation of the underlying mechanism and potential approaches to managing co-occurring MS-related dysfunctions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.J., P.Z. and R.W.M.; Data curation, B.J. and R.W.M.; Formal analysis, B.J., P.Z. and R.W.M.; Investigation, B.J., P.Z. and R.W.M.; Methodology, B.J., P.Z. and R.W.M.; Project administration, B.J. and R.W.M.; Supervision, R.W.M.; Validation, B.J., P.Z. and R.W.M.; Writing—original draft, B.J. and R.W.M.; Writing—review and editing, B.J., P.Z. and R.W.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (F32HD113333). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This is a secondary analysis of previously published datasets, and the approval of data use is noted in the references for the studies from which we included data. The five studies were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the procedures of the studies were approved by appropriate Institutional Review Boards.

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the five studies.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available. The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Trapp, B.D.; Nave, K.A. Multiple sclerosis: An immune or neurodegenerative disorder? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 31, 247–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Benedict, R.H.B.; Holtzer, R.; Motl, R.W.; Foley, F.W.; Kaur, S.; Hojnacki, D.; Weinstock-Guttman, B. Upper and lower extremity motor function and cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 643–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Motl, R.W.; Cadavid, D.; Sandroff, B.M.; Pilutti, L.A.; Pula, J.H.; Benedict, R.H.B. Cognitive processing speed has minimal influence on the construct validity of Multiple Sclerosis Walking Scale-12 scores. J. Neurol. Sci. 2013, 335, 169–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Leone, C.; Feys, P.; Moumdjian, L.; D’Amico, E.; Zappia, M.; Patti, F. Cognitive-motor dual-task interference: A systematic review of neural correlates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 75, 348–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Leone, C.; Moumdjian, L.; Patti, F.; Vanzeir, E.; Baert, I.; Veldkamp, R.; Van Wijmeersch, B.; Feys, P. Comparing 16 dfferent dual-tasking paradigms in individuals with multiple sclerosis and healthy controls: Working memory tasks indicate cognitive-motor interference. Front. Neurol. 2020, 11, 918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Veldkamp, R.; Baert, I.; Kalron, A.; Tacchino, A.; D’hooge, M.; Vanzeir, E.; Van Geel, F.; Raats, J.; Goetschalckx, M.; Brichetto, G.; et al. Structured cognitive-motor dual task training compared to single mobility training in persons with multiple sclerosis, a multicenter RCT. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8, 2177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Wajda, D.A.; Sosnoff, J.J. Cognitive-motor interference in multiple sclerosis: A systematic review of evidence, correlates, and consequences. BioMed Res. Int. 2015, 2015, 720856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kalron, A.; Dvir, Z.; Achiron, A. Walking while talking—difficulties incurred during the initial stages of multiple sclerosis disease process. Gait Posture 2010, 32, 332–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Wallin, M.T.; Culpepper, W.J.; Campbell, J.D.; Nelson, L.M.; Langer-Gould, A.; Marrie, R.A.; Cutter, G.R.; Kaye, W.E.; Wagner, L.; Tremlett, H.; et al. The prevalence of MS in the United States. Neurology 2019, 92, e1029–e1040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bollaert, R.E.; Sandroff, B.M.; Stine-Morrow, E.A.L.; Sutton, B.P.; Motl, R.W. The intersection of physical function, cognitive performance, aging, and multiple sclerosis: A cross-sectional comparative study. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2019, 32, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Jeng, B.; Šilić, P.; Bollaert, R.E.; Sandroff, B.M.; Motl, R.W. Physical function across the lifespan in adults with multiple sclerosis: An application of the Short Physical Performance Battery. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 2023, 73, 104624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Motl, R.W.; Sandroff, B.M.; DeLuca, J. Exercise training and cognitive rehabilitation: A symbiotic approach for rehabilitating walking and cognitive functions in multiple sclerosis? Neurorehabilit. Neural Repair 2016, 30, 499–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Motl, R.W.; Sandroff, B.M.; Benedict, R.H.; Hubbard, E.A.; Pilutti, L.A.; Sutton, B.P. Do subcortical gray matter volumes and aerobic capacity account for cognitive-motor coupling in multiple sclerosis? Mult. Scler. 2021, 27, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Chaparro, G.N.; Sosnoff, J.J.; Hernandez, M.E. Effects of aerobic fitness on cognitive motor interference during self-paced treadmill walking in older adults. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2020, 32, 2539–2547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bouchard, C.; An, P.; Rice, T.; Skinner, J.S.; Wilmore, J.H.; Gagnon, J.; Pérusse, L.; Leon, A.S.; Rao, D.C. Familial aggregation of VO2 max response to exercise training: Results from the HERITAGE Family Study. J. Appl. Physiol. 1999, 87, 1003–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bouchard, C.; Blair, S.N.; Katzmarzyk, P.T. Less sitting, more physical activity, or higher fitness? Mayo Clin. Proc. 2015, 90, 1533–1540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Baird, J.F.; Cederberg, K.L.J.; Sikes, E.M.; Jeng, B.; Sasaki, J.E.; Sandroff, B.M.; Motl, R.W. Changes in cognitive performance with age in adults with multiple sclerosis. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2019, 32, 201–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Baird, J.F.; Motl, R.W. Cognitive function and whole-brain MRI metrics Are not associated with mobility in older adults with multiple sclerosis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Cederberg, K.L.J.; Mathison, B.; Schuetz, M.L.; Motl, R.W. Restless legs syndrome severity and cognitive function in adults with multiple sclerosis: An exploratory pilot study. Int. J. MS Care 2022, 24, 154–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sandroff, B.M.; Wylie, G.R.; Baird, J.F.; Jones, C.D.; Diggs, M.D.; Genova, H.; Bamman, M.M.; Cutter, G.R.; DeLuca, J.; Motl, R.W. Effects of walking exercise training on learning and memory and hippocampal neuroimaging outcomes in MS: A targeted, pilot randomized controlled trial. Contemp. Clin. Trials 2021, 110, 106563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Learmonth, Y.C.; Motl, R.W.; Sandroff, B.M.; Pula, J.H.; Cadavid, D. Validation of patient determined disease steps (PDDS) scale scores in persons with multiple sclerosis. BMC Neurol. 2013, 13, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Marrie, R.A.; McFadyen, C.; Yaeger, L.; Salter, A. A systematic review of the validity and reliability of the Patient-Determined Disease Steps scale. Int. J. MS Care 2023, 25, 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Smith, A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test; The clinical neuropsychologist; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
  24. Delis, D.C.; Kramer, J.H.; Kaplan, E.; Ober, B.A. California Verbal Learning Test—Second Edition; APA PsycNet: Washington, DC, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  25. Goldman, M.D.; Marrie, R.A.; Cohen, J.A. Evaluation of the six-minute walk in multiple sclerosis subjects and healthy controls. Mult. Scler. 2008, 14, 383–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Motl, R.W.; Cohen, J.A.; Benedict, R.; Phillips, G.; LaRocca, N.; Hudson, L.D.; Rudick, R.; Multiple Sclerosis Outcome Consortium. Validity of the timed 25-foot walk as an ambulatory performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. J. 2017, 23, 704–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Sandroff, B.M.; Riskin, B.J.; Agiovlasitis, S.; Motl, R.W. Accelerometer cut-points derived during over-ground walking in persons with mild, moderate, and severe multiple sclerosis. J. Neurol. Sci. 2014, 340, 50–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Science, 2nd ed.; Routledge: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  29. Baker, M. 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature 2016, 533, 452–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Reality check on reproducibility. Nature 2016, 533, 437. [CrossRef]
  31. Huynh, T.L.T.; Williams, M.J.; Motl, R.W. Walking and physical performance in black and white adults with multiple sclerosis controlling for social determinants of health. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 2024, 83, 105439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sandroff, B.M.; Motl, R.W. Device-measured physical activity and cognitive processing speed impairment in a large sample of persons with multiple sclerosis. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2020, 26, 798–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ensari, I.; Balto, J.M.; Hubbard, E.A.; Pilutti, L.A.; Motl, R.W. Do depressive symptoms influence cognitive-motor coupling in multiple sclerosis? Rehabil. Psychol. 2018, 63, 111–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Stein, C.; O’Keeffe, F.; Strahan, O.; McGuigan, C.; Bramham, J. Systematic review of cognitive reserve in multiple sclerosis: Accounting for physical disability, fatigue, depression, and anxiety. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 2023, 79, 105017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Giustiniani, A.; Quartarone, A. Defining the concept of reserve in the motor domain: A systematic review. Front. Neurosci. 2024, 18, 1403065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Holtzer, R.; Choi, J.; Motl, R.W.; Foley, F.W.; Picone, M.A.; Lipton, M.L.; Izzetoglu, M.; Hernandez, M.; Wagshul, M.E. Individual reserve in aging and neurological disease. J. Neurol. 2023, 270, 3179–3191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Coghe, G.; Fenu, G.; Lorefice, L.; Zucca, E.; Porta, M.; Pilloni, G.; Corona, F.; Frau, J.; Giovanna Marrosu, M.; Pau, M.; et al. Association between brain atrophy and cognitive motor interference in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 2018, 25, 208–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Ruggieri, S.; Fanelli, F.; Castelli, L.; Petsas, N.; De Giglio, L.; Prosperini, L. Lesion symptom map of cognitive-postural interference in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. J. 2018, 24, 653–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Grinberg, Y.; Berkowitz, S.; Hershkovitz, L.; Malcay, O.; Kalron, A. The ability of the instrumented tandem walking tests to discriminate fully ambulatory people with MS from healthy adults. Gait Posture 2019, 70, 90–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Veldkamp, R.; Romberg, A.; Hämäläinen, P.; Giffroy, X.; Moumdjian, L.; Leone, C.; Feys, P.; Baert, I. Test-retest reliability of cognitive-motor interference assessments in walking with various task complexities in persons with multiple sclerosis. Neurorehabilit. Neural Repair 2019, 33, 623–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Centonze, D.; Leocani, L.; Feys, P. Advances in physical rehabilitation of multiple sclerosis. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2020, 33, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Galperin, I.; Mirelman, A.; Schmitz-Hübsch, T.; Hsieh, K.L.; Regev, K.; Karni, A.; Brozgol, M.; Thumm, P.C.; Lynch, S.G.; Paul, F.; et al. Treadmill training with virtual reality to enhance gait and cognitive function among people with multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled trial. J. Neurol. 2022, 270, 1388–1401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Abou, L.; Peters, J.; Fritz, N.E.; Sosnoff, J.J.; Kratz, A.L. Motor cognitive dual-task testing to predict future falls in multiple sclerosis: A systematic review. Neurorehabilit. Neural Repair 2022, 36, 757–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Demographic, clinical, and physical activity characteristics of the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
Table 1. Demographic, clinical, and physical activity characteristics of the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
CharacteristicsStatistics
Age, years53 (22–77)
Sex, n, %
Female
Male
221, 76%
69, 24%
Race
Caucasian
Non-Caucasian
190, 65%
100, 34%
Education, years16 (9–21)
MS type, n, %
Relapsing-remitting
Progressive
253, 89%
31, 11%
Disease duration, years15 (1–48)
PDDS, 0–82 (0–7)
MVPA, min/day20 (0–135)
Notes: Data are presented as mean (range), number, or percentage. MS, multiple sclerosis; PDDS, Patient-Determined Disease Steps; MVPA, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for outcomes of cognitive and motor function in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for outcomes of cognitive and motor function in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
VariablesMean (SD)
SDMT, score48.3 (12.5)
CVLT-II, score44.6 (10.3)
6MW, meters438.3 (140.1)
T25FW, meters per sec1.6 (0.5)
Notes: SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition; 6MW, 6-Minute Walk; T25FW, Timed 25-Foot Walk.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations among outcomes of cognitive function, motor function, age, physical activity, and demographic and clinical characteristics in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations among outcomes of cognitive function, motor function, age, physical activity, and demographic and clinical characteristics in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
12345678910
1. SDMT
2. CVLT-II  0.490 **
3. 6MW  0.459 **  0.370 **
4. T25FW  0.433 **  0.322 **  0.911 **
5. Age−0.347 **−0.161 *−0.258 **−0.316 **
6. MVPA  0.316 **  0.218 *  0.665 **  0.617 **−0.278 **
7. Sex−0.129 *−0.170 **  0.090  0.072  0.017  0.216 **
8. Education  0.388 **  0.339 **  0.237 **  0.150 *−0.037  0.139 *  0.033
9. Race−0.085−0.214 **−0.171 **−0.172 **−0.374 **−0.117−0.129 *−0.213 **
10. Disease Duration−0.237 **−0.106−0.220 **−0.222 **  0.648 **−0.274 **−0.110−0.069−0.270 **
11. PDDS−0.401 **−0.334 **−0.742 **−0.680 **  0.272 **−0.487 **  0.001−0.188 **  0.007  0.184 **
Notes: p-value of 0.05 *; p-value of <0.001 **. SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition; 6MW, 6-Minute Walk; T25FW, Timed 25-Foot Walk; MVPA, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity; PDDS, Patient-Determined Disease Steps.
Table 4. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores and 6-Minute Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
Table 4. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores and 6-Minute Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
PredictorBSE Bβp-Value
Step 1Sex154.66868.2080.1390.024
Years of Education47.57212.6650.232<0.001
Race−92.90461.248−0.0950.131
Step 2Sex131.14445.6600.1180.004
Years of Education20.9348.4910.1020.014
Race−135.15042.513−0.1380.002
Disease Duration−4.3762.144−0.0880.042
PDDS−179.41810.336−0.710<0.001
Step 3Sex149.10945.3590.1340.001
Years of Education13.4738.7290.0660.124
Race−114.62942.422−0.1170.007
Disease Duration−3.0352.158−0.0610.161
PDDS−167.50910.936−0.663<0.001
SDMT5.3861.8110.1390.003
Step 4Sex104.90545.2210.0940.021
Years of Education14.1098.4760.0690.097
Race−100.35944.618−0.1020.025
Disease Duration−1.7642.555−0.0350.491
PDDS−151.85211.224−0.601<0.001
SDMT4.6131.8310.1190.012
Age−0.5962.043−0.0170.771
MVPA4.1790.9990.182<0.001
R2 = 0.092 for Step 1; R2 = 0.611 for Step 2; R2 = 0.615 for Step 3; R2 = 0.638 for Step 4.
Notes: PDDS, Patient-Determined Disease Steps; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; 6MW, 6-Minute Walk; T25FW, MVPA, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.
Table 5. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores and Timed 25-Foot Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
Table 5. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores and Timed 25-Foot Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
PredictorBSE Bβp-Value
Step 1Sex0.1590.0840.1280.060
Years of Education0.0350.0150.1530.025
Race−0.1280.076−0.1160.091
Step 2Sex0.1500.0590.1210.012
Years of Education0.0070.0110.0310.522
Race−0.2190.054−0.198<0.001
Disease Duration−0.0070.003−0.1370.007
PDDS−0.1980.014−0.682<0.001
Step 3Sex0.1710.0590.1380.004
Years of Education−0.0010.011−0.0050.925
Race−0.1880.055−0.170<0.001
Disease Duration−0.0060.003−0.1040.042
PDDS−0.1840.015−0.632<0.001
SDMT0.0060.0020.1450.010
Step 4Sex0.1250.0600.1010.038
Years of Education0.0000.011−0.0010.975
Race−0.1890.059−0.1700.002
Disease Duration−0.0020.003−0.0350.560
PDDS−0.1660.015−0.570<0.001
SDMT0.0050.0020.1190.036
Age−0.0030.003−0.0880.189
MVPA0.0040.0010.1540.003
R2 = 0.060 for Step 1; R2 = 0.557 for Step 2; R2 = 0.571 for Step 3; R2 = 0.593 for Step 4.
Notes: PDDS, Patient-Determined Disease Steps; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; T25FW, Timed 25-Foot Walk; MVPA, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.
Table 6. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition scores and 6-Minute Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
Table 6. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition scores and 6-Minute Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
PredictorBSE Bβp-Value
Step 1Sex153.54968.1360.1380.025
Years of Education47.37412.6510.231<0.001
Race−89.22361.008−0.0910.145
Step 2Sex129.61245.6250.1160.005
Years of Education20.6988.4860.1010.015
Race−133.08742.452−0.1360.002
Disease Duration−4.5272.137−0.0910.035
PDDS−179.30710.333−0.709<0.001
Step 3Sex160.39646.1410.144<0.001
Years of Education13.7278.6880.0670.115
Race−102.69143.072−0.1050.018
Disease Duration−3.4372.138−0.0690.109
PDDS−170.34110.625−0.674<0.001
CVLT-II6.1872.1030.1350.004
Step 4Sex116.19145.8840.1040.012
Years of Education13.7778.4060.0670.102
Race−91.62044.662−0.0940.041
Disease Duration−1.6282.542−0.0330.523
PDDS−153.05911.029−0.605<0.001
CVLT-II5.6682.0500.1240.006
Age−1.1851.969−0.0330.548
MVPA4.205.9960.183<0.001
R2 = 0.090 for Step 1; R2 = 0.610 for Step 2; R2 = 0.623 for Step 3; R2 = 0.650 for Step 4.
Notes: PDDS, Patient-Determined Disease Steps; CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition; 6MW, 6-Minute Walk; MVPA, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.
Table 7. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition scores and Timed 25-Foot Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
Table 7. Hierarchical linear regression analysis of California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition scores and Timed 25-Foot Walk scores in the sample of people with multiple sclerosis.
PredictorBSE Bβp-Value
Step 1Sex0.1580.2860.1280.060
Years of Education0.0350.0840.1530.025
Race−0.1270.015−0.1150.094
Step 2Sex0.1490.0590.1210.012
Years of Education0.0070.0110.0300.529
Race−0.2180.054−0.198<0.001
Disease Duration−0.0070.003−0.1390.006
PDDS−0.1980.014−0.682<0.001
Step 3Sex0.1810.0600.1460.003
Years of Education−0.0010.011−0.0040.931
Race−0.1870.055−0.170<0.001
Disease Duration−0.0060.003−0.1150.019
PDDS−0.1900.014−0.655<0.001
CVLT-II0.0060.0030.1250.019
Step 4Sex0.1350.0600.1090.027
Years of Education−0.0010.011−0.0060.905
Race−0.1890.058−0.1720.001
Disease Duration−0.0020.003−0.0350.561
PDDS−0.1700.015−0.583<0.001
CVLT-II0.0060.0030.1150.029
Age−0.0040.003−0.1080.092
MVPA0.0040.0010.1550.003
R2 = 0.060 for Step 1; R2 = 0.557 for Step 2; R2 = 0.569 for Step 3; R2 = 0.593 for Step 4.
Notes: PDDS, Patient-Determined Disease Steps; CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition; T25FW, Timed 25-Foot Walk; MVPA, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jeng, B.; Zheng, P.; Motl, R.W. Cognitive–Motor Coupling in Multiple Sclerosis: Do Chronological Age and Physical Activity Matter? Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 274. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15030274

AMA Style

Jeng B, Zheng P, Motl RW. Cognitive–Motor Coupling in Multiple Sclerosis: Do Chronological Age and Physical Activity Matter? Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(3):274. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15030274

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jeng, Brenda, Peixuan Zheng, and Robert W. Motl. 2025. "Cognitive–Motor Coupling in Multiple Sclerosis: Do Chronological Age and Physical Activity Matter?" Brain Sciences 15, no. 3: 274. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15030274

APA Style

Jeng, B., Zheng, P., & Motl, R. W. (2025). Cognitive–Motor Coupling in Multiple Sclerosis: Do Chronological Age and Physical Activity Matter? Brain Sciences, 15(3), 274. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15030274

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop