ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing
Abstract
1. Introduction
Aims and Hypotheses
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Design
2.3. Procedure and Materials
2.3.1. Prospective Memory Task
2.3.2. EEG Recording and Pre-Processing
3. Data Analyses and Results
3.1. Data Analysis
3.1.1. Behavioural Analyses
3.1.2. Electrophysiological Data Analysis
3.2. Behavioural Results
3.2.1. Ongoing Activity Performance
- Response Times. The analysis yielded a statistically significant main effect of focality condition, F(1,29) = 10.464; p < 0.05; = 0.265, showing faster response times for the ongoing activity in the focal condition (M = 832.158, SD = 122.252) than in the non-focal condition (M = 878.308, SD = 154.869). However, the main effect of language F(1,29) = 0.247; p = 0.623; = 0.008, and the focality by language interaction F(1,29) = 0.240; p = 0.628; = 0.008 did not reach significance.
- Accuracy. The analysis showed a significant main effect of focality, F(1,29) = 87.435; p < 0.01; = 0.751, indicating higher accuracy rates in the focal condition (M = 0.96, SD = 0.03) than in the non-focal condition (M = 0.93, SD = 0.04). Furthermore, there was a significant main effect of language, F(1,29) = 14.128; p < 0.001; = 0.328, with more accuracy in L1 (M = 0.956; SD = 0.031) than in L2 (M = 0.934; SD = 0.04). However, the language by focality interaction was not significant F(1,29) = 0.867; p = 0.360; = 0.029.
3.2.2. PM Task Performance
- Response Times. The main effects of focality, F(1,28) = 1.951 p = 0.173; = 0.065, language F(1,28) = 0.705; p = 0.408; = 0.025, and the focality by language interaction F(1,28) = 3.236; p = 0.083; = 0.104 did not reach the statistical significance.
- Accuracy. The main effect of focality reached statistical significance F(1,28) = 31.080; p < 0.01; = 0.526, indicating greater accuracy in the focal (M = 0.884, SD = 0.149) than in the non-focal condition (M = 0.763, SD = 0.119). However, the main effect of language F(1,28) = 2.207; p = 0.149; = 0.073 and the focality by language interaction were not significant F(1,28) = 0.833; p = 0.083; = 0.104.
3.3. Electrophysiological Results
3.3.1. Electrophysiological Results: P3b
3.3.2. Electrophysiological Results: N300
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Holmes, K.J.; Wolff, P. Simulation from Schematics: Dorsal Stream Processing and the Perception of Implied Motion. Proc. Annu. Meet. Cogn. Sci. Soc. 2010, 32, 2704–2709. [Google Scholar]
- Thierry, G. Neurolinguistic Relativity: How Language Flexes Human Perception and Cognition. Lang. Learn. 2016, 66, 690–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davies, I.R.L.; Corbett, G.G. A Cross-Cultural Study of Colour Grouping: Evidence for Weak Linguistic Relativity. Br. J. Psychol. 1997, 88, 493–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- He, H.; Li, J.; Xiao, Q.; Jiang, S.; Yang, Y.; Zhi, S. Language and Color Perception: Evidence From Mongolian and Chinese Speakers. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bobb, S.C.; Von Holzen, K.; Mayor, J.; Mani, N.; Carreiras, M. Co-Activation of the L2 during L1 Auditory Processing: An ERP Cross-Modal Priming Study. Brain Lang. 2020, 203, 104739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iniesta, A.; Paolieri, D.; Serrano, F.; Bajo, M.T. Bilingual Writing Coactivation: Lexical and Sublexical Processing in a Word Dictation Task. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2021, 24, 902–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blumenfeld, H.K.; Marian, V. Parallel Language Activation and Cognitive Control during Spoken Word Recognition in Bilinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 547–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Athanasopoulos, P. Cognitive Representation of Colour in Bilinguals: The Case of Greek Blues. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2009, 12, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chabal, S.; Marian, V. Speakers of Different Languages Process the Visual World Differently. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2015, 144, 539–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, P.; Chung-Fat-Yim, A.; Marian, V. Cultural Experience Influences Multisensory Emotion Perception in Bilinguals. Languages 2022, 7, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marian, V.; Bartolotti, J.; van den Berg, A.; Hayakawa, S. Costs and Benefits of Native Language Similarity for Non-Native Word Learning. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 651506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, R.E.; Hunt, R.R.; McVay, J.C.; McConnell, M.D. The Cost of Event-Based Prospective Memory: Salient Target Events. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 33, 734–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bisiacchi, P.S.; Schiff, S.; Ciccola, A.; Kliegel, M. The Role of Dual-Task and Task-Switch in Prospective Memory: Behavioural Data and Neural Correlates. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 1362–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scullin, M.K.; Mullet, H.G.; Einstein, G.O.; McDaniel, M.A. Prospective Memory. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDaniel, M.A.; Einstein, G.O. Strategic and Automatic Processes in Prospective Memory Retrieval: A Multiprocess Framework. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 14, S127–S144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Einstein, G.O.; McDaniel, M.A.; Thomas, R.; Mayfield, S.; Shank, H.; Morrisette, N.; Breneiser, J. Multiple Processes in Prospective Memory Retrieval: Factors Determining Monitoring Versus Spontaneous Retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2005, 134, 327–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaan, E.; Kheder, S.; Kreidler, A.; Tomić, A.; Valdés Kroff, J.R. Processing Code-Switches in the Presence of Others: An ERP Study. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López-Rojas, C.; Rossi, E.; Marful, A.; Bajo, M.T. Prospective Memory in Bilinguals and Monolinguals: ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Processing in Bilinguals. Brain Lang. 2022, 225, 105059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, R. The Temporal Dynamics of Prospective Memory: A Review of the ERP and Prospective Memory Literature. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 2233–2245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López-Rojas, C.; Marful, A.; Pérez, A.I.; Bajo, M.T. Prospective memory in bilinguals: Recalling future intentions in first and second language contexts. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2023, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayakawa, S.; Costa, A.; Foucart, A.; Keysar, B. Using a Foreign Language Changes Our Choices. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 791–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morales, J.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. Dual Mechanisms of Cognitive Control in Bilinguals and Monolinguals. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 531–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morales, J.; Yudes, C.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. Bilingualism Modulates Dual Mechanisms of Cognitive Control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia 2015, 66, 157–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez, A.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Speech Perception in Bilingual Contexts: Neuropsychological Impact of Mixing Languages at the Inter-Sentential Level. J. Neurolinguistics 2019, 51, 258–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kliegel, M.; Jäger, T.; Phillips, L.H. Adult Age Differences in Event-Based Prospective Memory: A Meta-Analysis on the Role of Focal versus Nonfocal Cues. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polich, J. Updating P300: An Integrative Theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 2128–2148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, R.; Wymbs, N.; Jakubek, K.; Herndon, R.W. Effects of Intention Load and Background Context on Prospective Remembering: An Event-Related Brain Potential Study. Psychophysiology 2003, 40, 260–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, R. The Influence of Strategic Monitoring on the Neural Correlates of Prospective Memory. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 35, 1034–1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, J.; Xue, T.; Zhang, Z. Salient Cues Facilitate Automatic Processes in Prospective Memory. Neuroreport 2021, 32, 864–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cona, G.; Bisiacchi, P.S.; Moscovitch, M. The Effects of Focal and Nonfocal Cues on the Neural Correlates of Prospective Memory: Insights From ERPs. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 24, 2630–2646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meier, B.; Zimmermann, T.D. Loads and Loads and Loads: The Influence of Prospective Load, Retrospective Load, and Ongoing Task Load in Prospective Memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horiba, Y.; Fukaya, K. Reading and learning from L2 text: Effects of reading goal, topic familiarity, and language proficiency. Read. A Foreign Lang. 2015, 27, 22–46. [Google Scholar]
- Hunter Ball, B.; Bugg, J.M. Context Cue Focality Influences Strategic Prospective Memory Monitoring. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 1405–1415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cejudo, A.B.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. The Cost of Prospective Memory in Children: The Role of Cue Focality. Front. Psychol. 2019, 9, 2738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, L.; Altgassen, M.; Liu, W.; Xiong, W.; Akgün, C.; Kliegel, M. Prospective Memory across Adolescence: The Effects of Age and Cue Focality. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 226–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDaniel, M.A.; Umanath, S.; Einstein, G.O.; Waldum, E.R. Dual Pathways to Prospective Remembering. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 286–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- PANGEA: Power Analysis for General Anova Designs. Available online: https://jakewestfall.shinyapps.io/pangea/ (accessed on 23 January 2023).
- Marian, V.; Blumenfeld, H.K.; Kaushanskaya, M. The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007, 50, 940–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balota, D.A.; Yap, M.J.; Hutchison, K.A.; Cortese, M.J.; Kessler, B.; Loftis, B.; Neely, J.H.; Nelson, D.L.; Simpson, G.B.; Treiman, R. The English Lexicon Project. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 445–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guasch, M.; Boada, R.; Ferré, P.; Sánchez-Casas, R. NIM: A Web-Based Swiss Army Knife to Select Stimuli for Psycholinguistic Studies. Behav. Res. 2013, 45, 765–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An Open Source Toolbox for Analysis of Single-Trial EEG Dynamics Including Independent Component Analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Czernochowski, D.; Horn, S.; Bayen, U.J. Does Frequency Matter? ERP and Behavioral Correlates of Monitoring for Rare and Frequent Prospective Memory Targets. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cejudo, A.B.; López-Rojas, C.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. ERP Correlates of Prospective Memory and Cue Focality in Children. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oostenveld, R.; Fries, P.; Maris, E.; Schoffelen, J.-M. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2010, 2011, 156869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, M.; Lervåg, A. Reading Comprehension and its Underlying Components in Second-language learners: A meta-analysis of studies comparing first-and second-language learners. Psychol. Bull. 2014, 140, 409–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antoniou, M. The Advantages of Bilingualism Debate. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2019, 5, 395–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Y.J.; Thierry, G. Fast Modulation of Executive Function by Language Context in Bilinguals. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 13533–13537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- West, R.; McNerney, M.W.; Travers, S. Gone but Not Forgotten: The Effects of Cancelled Intentions on the Neural Correlates of Prospective Memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2007, 64, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Cao, X.; Cui, J.; Shum, D.H.K.; Chan, R.C.K. The Relation between Prospective Memory and Working Memory: Evidence from Event-Related Potential Data. PsyCh. J. 2013, 2, 113–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilson, J.; Cutmore, T.R.H.; Wang, Y.; Chan, R.C.K.; Shum, D.H.K. Effects of Cue Frequency and Repetition on Prospective Memory: An ERP Investigation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 90, 250–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Patel, S.H.; Azzam, P.N. Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected Studies of the Event-Related Potential. Int. J. Med. Sci. 2005, 2, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrison, C.; Kamal, F.; Taler, V. The Influence of Bilingualism on Working Memory Event-Related Potentials. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, 22, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hockey, A.; Cutmore, T. Inhibitory Control in Prospective Memory: An Event Related Potential Comparison of Task-Switch and Dual Task Processing. Neuropsychologia 2021, 158, 107906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folstein, J.R.; Van Petten, C. Influence of Cognitive Control and Mismatch on the N2 Component of the ERP: A Review. Psychophysiology 2008, 45, 152–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cousens, R.; Cutmore, T.; Wang, Y.; Wilson, J.; Chan, R.C.K.; Shum, D.H.K. Effects of Perceptual and Semantic Cues on ERP Modulations Associated with Prospective Memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2015, 98, 151–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cruz, G.; Miyakoshi, M.; Makeig, S.; Kilborn, K.; Evans, J. ERPs and Their Brain Sources in Perceptual and Conceptual Prospective Memory Tasks: Commonalities and Differences between the Two Tasks. Neuropsychologia 2016, 91, 173–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
L1 | L2 | |
---|---|---|
Proportion of current exposure to the language | 0.64 * (0.16) | 0.30 * (0.14) |
Proportion of preference to read in each language | 0.51 * (0.23) | 0.43 * (0.21) |
Proportion of preference to speak in each language | 0.54 * (0.29) | 0.35 * (0.22) |
Mean age of beginning acquisition (years) | 4.71 (2.66) | |
Mean age of becoming fluent (years) | 12.87 (4.94) | |
Mean level of self-competence (from 0–10) | 8.5 (1.26) | |
Mean level of language exposure with family or friends (from 0–10) | 3.87 (3.25) | |
Mean level of reading exposure (from 0–10) | 5.93 (3.69) | |
Mean level of language exposure by TV or radio (from 0–10) | 8.58 (2.19) | |
Mean level of language exposure by self-learning (from 0–10) | 2.54 (3.29) |
L1 | L2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Type of Trial | ACC | RT | ACC | RT |
ON trials focal | 0.97 (0.03) | 836 (142) | 0.95 (0.03) | 828 (102) |
ON trials non focal | 0.94 (0.03) | 889 (184) | 0.92 (0.04) | 868 (126) |
PM trials focal | 0.85 (0.19) | 1059 (256) | 0.92 (0.1) | 1002 (199) |
PM trials non focal | 0.75 (0.14) | 1069 (206) | 0.78 (0.1) | 1071 (189) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
López-Rojas, C.; Csilinkó, A.; Bajo, M.T.; Marful, A. ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365
López-Rojas C, Csilinkó A, Bajo MT, Marful A. ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(2):365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365
Chicago/Turabian StyleLópez-Rojas, Cristina, Anikó Csilinkó, Mª Teresa Bajo, and Alejandra Marful. 2023. "ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing" Brain Sciences 13, no. 2: 365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365
APA StyleLópez-Rojas, C., Csilinkó, A., Bajo, M. T., & Marful, A. (2023). ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing. Brain Sciences, 13(2), 365. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020365