Next Article in Journal
The Effects of Acetazolamide on Cerebral Hemodynamics in Adult Patients with an Acute Brain Injury: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
Differences in Brain Network Topology Based on Alcohol Use History in Adolescents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Communication

Reduced Age-Related Gray Matter Loss in the Orbitofrontal Cortex in Long-Term Meditators

1
School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland 1010, New Zealand
2
Psychology Discipline, Institute for Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, VIC 3011, Australia
3
Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, Uppsala University, 751 85 Uppsala, Sweden
4
Laboratory of Neuro Imaging, School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(12), 1677; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13121677
Submission received: 31 October 2023 / Revised: 26 November 2023 / Accepted: 28 November 2023 / Published: 6 December 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Cognitive, Social and Affective Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is a functionally heterogeneous brain region contributing to mental processes relating to meditation practices. The OFC has been reported to decline in volume with increasing age and differs in volume between meditation practitioners and non-practitioners. We hypothesized that the age-related decline of the OFC is diminished in meditation practitioners. We tested this hypothesis in a sample of 50 long-term meditators and 50 matched controls by correlating chronological age with regional gray matter volumes of the left and right OFC, as well as in seven left and right cytoarchitectonically defined subregions of the OFC (Fo1–Fo7). In both meditators and controls, we observed a negative relationship between age and OFC (sub)volumes, indicating that older participants have smaller OFC volumes. However, in meditators, the age-related decline was less steep compared to controls. These age-related differences reached significance for left and right Fo2, Fo3, Fo4, and Fo7, as well as left Fo5 and right Fo6. Since different subregions of the OFC are associated with distinct brain functions, further investigations are required to explore the functional implications of these findings in the context of meditation and the aging brain.

1. Introduction

Meditation research has grown exponentially in recent years, and an increasing number of studies suggest that meditation might slow age-related cognitive decline as well as tissue loss in the brain [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10]. One brain region reported to be involved in meditation and altered in meditation practitioners is the orbitofrontal cortex [7,11,12,13,14]. Since the orbitofrontal cortex is also known to decrease in volume and thickness with age [15,16,17,18,19,20,21], age-related decline in the orbitofrontal cortex may be diminished in meditation practitioners.
The current study set out to investigate the possible effects of meditation on age-related gray matter volume loss in the orbitofrontal cortex within a sample of 50 long-term meditators and 50 age- and sex-matched controls. To assess anatomically and functionally meaningful orbitofrontal subareas, we used the cytoarchitectonic parcellation of this region into medial orbitofrontal areas Fo1–Fo3 [22] and lateral orbitofrontal areas Fo4–Fo7 [23] in addition to the overall orbitofrontal cortex, which was defined as the composite of these subareas (OFC = Fo1 + Fo2 + Fo3 + Fo4 + Fo5 + Fo6 + Fo7). Gray matter volume for each area was measured using an advanced region-of-interest technique, combining image-based signal intensities and cytoarchitectonically defined probabilities [24,25,26]. In both meditators and controls, we anticipated finding a negative relationship between age and gray matter volume. Nevertheless, our hypothesis was that the age-related reduction in volume would be more moderate in long-term meditators compared to controls. Furthermore, we expected that the magnitude of these effects would vary across the subareas of the orbitofrontal cortex.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Sample and Brain Images

The study included 50 meditation practitioners and 50 control subjects, with ages between 24 and 77 years; a detailed sample description is provided elsewhere [9]. Importantly, both groups were matched for sex (28 men and 22 women in each group) as well as for age (mean ± SD controls: 51.4 ± 12.8 years; meditators: 50.4 ± 11.8 years). In addition, both groups were comparable in terms of handedness and education, and were free from neurological and psychiatric disorders, as previously described [27]. The meditators had an active lifetime practice between 4 and 46 years (mean ± SD: 19.8 ± 11.4 years); an overview of individual meditation practices in this sample has been provided elsewhere [9]. All study participants gave informed consent in accordance with the policies and procedures of the Institutional Review Board at the University of California (UCLA).
Brain scans for the control group were sourced from the International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM) database, which contains data from healthy adults (https://ida.loni.usc.edu/). Brain scans for the meditators were collected at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) using the same 1.5 Tesla Siemens Sonata scanner and the same scanning parameters employed for the control group: a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence with a 4.38 ms echo time, a 1900 ms repetition time, a 15° flip angle, 160 contiguous sagittal slices, a 256 × 256 mm2 field-of-view, and a 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 voxel size.

2.2. Data Processing and Volume Extraction

All brain images were processed and analyzed in Matlab (https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html) using SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12), the CAT12 toolbox [28], and the Julich-Brain Atlas [29,30], as described elsewhere [24,25,26,31]. After applying corrections for magnetic field inhomogeneities, brain images were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. Subsequently, the gray matter segments were spatially normalized to the Shooting template [32] and modulated to preserve the original voxel-wise gray matter [26,33,34]. Total intracranial volume (TIV) was calculated in native space and later included as a covariate in the statistical model.
Region-specific gray matter volumes of the orbitofrontal subregions Fo1–Fo7 [22,23] were calculated using the cytoarchitectonic probability maps provided with the Julich-Brain Atlas [29,30]. More specifically, in a first step, the probability maps were spatially normalized to the Shooting template to ensure accurate spatial correspondence with the modulated normalized gray matter segments (see previous section). This was followed by voxel-wise multiplication of these normalized probability maps (i.e., left and right Fo1-Fo7) with the normalized and modulated gray matter segments. The resulting voxel-wise measures were multiplied by the voxel volume and added up to estimate region-specific gray matter volumes (in mm3), as detailed elsewhere [26]. In addition, the volume for the orbitofrontal cortex as a whole was calculated separately for each hemisphere (OFC = Fo1 + Fo2 + Fo3 + Fo4 + Fo5 + Fo6 + Fo7).

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Applying a mass-univariate general linear model, we designated the volumes of the left and right regions of interest (OFC and Fo1–Fo7) as dependent variables, with group, age, and the group-by-age interaction as independent variables. Age was centered at 50 years. Sex and TIV were treated as nuisance variables. As assumptions for parametric testing were not met in all cases (the residuals for left Fo1 and left Fo7 were not normally distributed as determined using a Lilliefors test), significance was established for all regions using a Monte Carlo simulation with 10,000 permutations using the Smith procedure [35,36]. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons by controlling the false discovery rate [37,38]. Significant group-by-age interactions were followed by conducting post hoc tests investigating age-related correlations within meditators and controls separately. For this, we applied multiple regression models, where sex and TIV were treated again as nuisance variables. Last but not least, the region-specific annual volume loss (in %) was calculated at age 50 using the beta-estimates of the statistical model [5,24].

3. Results

As detailed in Table 1, there was a significant group-by-age interaction for the left and right orbitofrontal cortex overall (OFC), as well as for left and right Fo2, Fo3, Fo4, and Fo7. In addition, significant group-by-age interactions were observed for left Fo5 and right Fo6. Effect sizes (calculated as Cohen’s d) ranged between d = 0.374 and d = 0.806, suggesting a range from small to large effects.
Group-specific correlations between age and orbitofrontal gray matter are visualized in Figure 1. As shown in Table 2, they were exclusively negative both in meditators and controls (i.e., the older the individual, the smaller the volumes). All of these negative associations were significant in controls. They were also significant in meditators with the exception of left Fo4. However, the slopes of the regression trajectories were notably steeper in controls compared to meditators, and the correlation coefficients were mostly higher in controls. The rates of annual tissue loss at age 50 ranged between 0.6% and 0.8% in controls and between 0.1% and 0.6% in meditators (see Table 2).

4. Discussion

Effects of aging on the orbitofrontal cortex were observed in both groups, with older participants having smaller volumes of the orbitofrontal cortex. However, aging trajectories were less steep in meditators compared to controls. These results thus support our hypothesis of diminished age-related gray matter loss in the orbitofrontal cortex in long-term meditation practitioners.
These findings seem to be in agreement with outcomes of previous studies that detected larger gray matter volumes [7,11,14] as well as increased glucose metabolism [7] in the orbitofrontal cortex in meditators compared to controls. They also seem to be aligned with reports of increased orbitofrontal activity during meditation [12]. The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in processing and regulating pleasant and unpleasant emotions; it is also implicated in reward-related learning, language, working memory, and memory in general [22,23,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47]. Thus, the outcomes of our study also seem to corroborate other research suggesting that regular mindfulness practices are linked to improved emotion regulation [48,49], increased empathy [50], as well as enhanced memory, attention, and language processing [51,52].
With respect to age-related brain preservation, the findings from the current study suggest that continuous long-term meditation practice has the potential to slow down age-related volume loss within the orbitofrontal cortex and its subregions. This may be a by-product of the constant training and resulting neuroplastic changes known to increase local tissue volumes [10,53,54,55]. However, other mechanisms, such as meditation-induced reduction of stress responses and inflammation, might be at play as well (for a review, see [10]). In further support of this assumption, previous studies in both humans and animals revealed that adverse life events and chronic stress are linked to a reduction of gray matter volumes or specifically GABAergic neurons within the orbitofrontal cortex [56,57].
Further research is clearly necessary to replicate the observed effects in larger samples, ideally using longitudinal and randomized controlled designs over an extended period of time. The latter will also help resolve whether the orbitofrontal cortex of experienced meditators was already different prior to their meditation practice and how much of the age-related effect was a consequence of meditation practice rather than a pre-existing characteristic. Follow-up studies might also want to collect measures of stress and/or glucocorticoid levels to assess the effects of (meditation-induced) stress reduction on orbitofrontal regions. Similarly, measures of anxiety and depression, as well as health and lifestyle indicators, might yield valuable information. Last but not least, given that the different subregions of the orbitofrontal cortex serve different functions, future studies may significantly enhance this field of research by including functional and behavioral measures to determine links between brain structure, brain function, and behavior.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, F.K. and E.L.; methodology, F.K. and E.L.; formal analysis, F.K.; data curation, E.L.; writing—original draft preparation, F.K., S.S. and E.L.; writing—review and editing, F.K. and E.L.; visualization, F.K. and E.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All study participants gave informed consent in accordance with the policies and procedures of the Institutional Review Board at the University of California (UCLA). (Protocol code: IRB#11-000455), approved on 10/03/2012.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

While the conditions of our ethics approval do not permit public archiving of anonymized study data for the meditation sample, the data for the control sample are available via the IDA database at the Laboratory of Neuro Imaging (https://ida.loni.usc.edu/).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all meditators for their participation in our study. Moreover, the authors thank the Brain Mapping Medical Research Organization, the Robson Family and Northstar Fund, the William and Linda Dietel Philanthropic Fund at the Northern Piedmont Community, as well as the following Foundations for their generous support: Brain Mapping Support, Pierson-Lovelace, Ahmanson, Tamkin, Jennifer Jones-Simon, and Capital Group Companies. Last but not least, we are grateful to Trent Thixton, who assisted with brain image acquisition.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gard, T.; Holzel, B.K.; Lazar, S.W. The potential effects of meditation on age-related cognitive decline: A systematic review. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2014, 1307, 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Chan, J.S.Y.; Deng, K.; Wu, J.; Yan, J.H. Effects of Meditation and Mind-Body Exercises on Older Adults’ Cognitive Performance: A Meta-analysis. Gerontologist 2019, 59, e782–e790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Pagnoni, G.; Cekic, M. Age effects on gray matter volume and attentional performance in Zen meditation. Neurobiol. Aging 2007, 28, 1623–1627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Lazar, S.W.; Kerr, C.E.; Wasserman, R.H.; Gray, J.R.; Greve, D.N.; Treadway, M.T.; McGarvey, M.; Quinn, B.T.; Dusek, J.A.; Benson, H.; et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport 2005, 16, 1893–1897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Luders, E.; Cherbuin, N.; Gaser, C. Estimating brain age using high-resolution pattern recognition: Younger brains in long-term meditation practitioners. NeuroImage 2016, 134, 508–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Laneri, D.; Schuster, V.; Dietsche, B.; Jansen, A.; Ott, U.; Sommer, J. Effects of Long-Term Mindfulness Meditation on Brain’s White Matter Microstructure and its Aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2016, 7, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Chetelat, G.; Mezenge, F.; Tomadesso, C.; Landeau, B.; Arenaza-Urquijo, E.; Rauchs, G.; Andre, C.; de Flores, R.; Egret, S.; Gonneaud, J.; et al. Reduced age-associated brain changes in expert meditators: A multimodal neuroimaging pilot study. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 10160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Luders, E. Exploring age-related brain degeneration in meditation practitioners. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2014, 1307, 82–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Luders, E.; Cherbuin, N.; Kurth, F. Forever Young(er): Potential age-defying effects of long-term meditation on gray matter atrophy. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kurth, F.; Cherbuin, N.; Luders, E. Promising Links between Meditation and Reduced (Brain) Aging: An Attempt to Bridge Some Gaps between the Alleged Fountain of Youth and the Youth of the Field. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Luders, E.; Toga, A.W.; Lepore, N.; Gaser, C. The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: Larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter. Neuroimage 2009, 45, 672–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Miyashiro, S.; Yamada, Y.; Muta, T.; Ishikawa, H.; Abe, T.; Hori, M.; Oka, K.; Koshikawa, F.; Ito, E. Activation of the orbitofrontal cortex by both meditation and exercise: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0247685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Manna, A.; Raffone, A.; Perrucci, M.G.; Nardo, D.; Ferretti, A.; Tartaro, A.; Londei, A.; Del Gratta, C.; Belardinelli, M.O.; Romani, G.L. Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation. Brain Res. Bull. 2010, 82, 46–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Hernandez, S.E.; Suero, J.; Barros, A.; Gonzalez-Mora, J.L.; Rubia, K. Increased Grey Matter Associated with Long-Term Sahaja Yoga Meditation: A Voxel-Based Morphometry Study. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0150757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Denburg, N.L.; Cole, C.A.; Hernandez, M.; Yamada, T.H.; Tranel, D.; Bechara, A.; Wallace, R.B. The orbitofrontal cortex, real-world decision making, and normal aging. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2007, 1121, 480–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Pieperhoff, P.; Homke, L.; Schneider, F.; Habel, U.; Shah, N.J.; Zilles, K.; Amunts, K. Deformation field morphometry reveals age-related structural differences between the brains of adults up to 51 years. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 828–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Terribilli, D.; Schaufelberger, M.S.; Duran, F.L.; Zanetti, M.V.; Curiati, P.K.; Menezes, P.R.; Scazufca, M.; Amaro, E., Jr.; Leite, C.C.; Busatto, G.F. Age-related gray matter volume changes in the brain during non-elderly adulthood. Neurobiol. Aging 2011, 32, 354–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ziegler, G.; Dahnke, R.; Jancke, L.; Yotter, R.A.; May, A.; Gaser, C. Brain structural trajectories over the adult lifespan. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012, 33, 2377–2389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pfefferbaum, A.; Rohlfing, T.; Rosenbloom, M.J.; Chu, W.; Colrain, I.M.; Sullivan, E.V. Variation in longitudinal trajectories of regional brain volumes of healthy men and women (ages 10 to 85 years) measured with atlas-based parcellation of MRI. NeuroImage 2013, 65, 176–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fjell, A.M.; Walhovd, K.B.; Fennema-Notestine, C.; McEvoy, L.K.; Hagler, D.J.; Holland, D.; Brewer, J.B.; Dale, A.M. One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 15223–15231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fjell, A.M.; Westlye, L.T.; Amlien, I.; Espeseth, T.; Reinvang, I.; Raz, N.; Agartz, I.; Salat, D.H.; Greve, D.N.; Fischl, B.; et al. High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 2001–2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Henssen, A.; Zilles, K.; Palomero-Gallagher, N.; Schleicher, A.; Mohlberg, H.; Gerboga, F.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Bludau, S.; Amunts, K. Cytoarchitecture and probability maps of the human medial orbitofrontal cortex. Cortex 2016, 75, 87–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Wojtasik, M.; Bludau, S.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Mohlberg, H.; Gerboga, F.; Caspers, S.; Amunts, K. Cytoarchitectonic Characterization and Functional Decoding of Four New Areas in the Human Lateral Orbitofrontal Cortex. Front. Neuroanat. 2020, 14, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Kurth, F.; Cherbuin, N.; Luders, E. The impact of aging on subregions of the hippocampal complex in healthy adults. NeuroImage 2017, 163, 296–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Luders, E.; Kurth, F.; Toga, A.W.; Narr, K.L.; Gaser, C. Meditation effects within the hippocampal complex revealed by voxel-based morphometry and cytoarchitectonic probabilistic mapping. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kurth, F.; Jancke, L.; Luders, E. Integrating cytoarchitectonic tissue probabilities with MRI-based signal intensities to calculate volumes of interest. In Brain Morphometry: Methods and Clinical Applications; Spalletta, G., Gili, T., Piras, F., Eds.; Humana Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 121–129. [Google Scholar]
  27. Luders, E.; Kurth, F.; Mayer, E.A.; Toga, A.W.; Narr, K.L.; Gaser, C. The unique brain anatomy of meditation practitioners: Alterations in cortical gyrification. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Gaser, C.; Dahnke, R.; Thompson, P.M.; Kurth, F.; Luders, E. CAT—A Computational Anatomy Toolbox for the Analysis of Structural MRI Data. bioRxiv 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Amunts, K.; Mohlberg, H.; Bludau, S.; Caspers, S.; Brandstetter, A.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Pieperhoff, P.; Dickscheid, T. Julich-Brain Atlas—Whole-brain collections of cytoarchitectonic probabilistic maps (v2.9). EBRAINS 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Amunts, K.; Mohlberg, H.; Bludau, S.; Zilles, K. Julich-Brain: A 3D probabilistic atlas of the human brain’s cytoarchitecture. Science 2020, 369, 988–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kurth, F.; Cherbuin, N.; Luders, E. Reduced age-related degeneration of the hippocampal subiculum in long-term meditators. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 232, 214–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ashburner, J.; Friston, K.J. Diffeomorphic registration using geodesic shooting and Gauss-Newton optimisation. Neuroimage 2011, 55, 954–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Ashburner, J.; Friston, K.J. Voxel-based morphometry—The methods. Neuroimage 2000, 11, 805–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Good, C.D.; Johnsrude, I.S.; Ashburner, J.; Henson, R.N.; Friston, K.J.; Frackowiak, R.S. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Nichols, T.; Holmes, A. Non-parametric procedures. In Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images; Friston, K., Ashburner, J., Kiebel, S., Nichols, T.E., Penny, W.D., Eds.; Elsevier: London, UK, 2007; pp. 253–272. [Google Scholar]
  36. Winkler, A.M.; Ridgway, G.R.; Webster, M.A.; Smith, S.M.; Nichols, T.E. Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 2014, 92, 381–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Benjamini, Y.; Yekutieli, D. The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann. Stat. 2001, 29, 1165–1188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hochberg, Y.; Benjamini, Y. More powerful procedures for multiple significance testing. Stat. Med. 1990, 9, 811–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Schnider, A.; Treyer, V.; Buck, A. Selection of currently relevant memories by the human posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2000, 20, 5880–5884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A.R. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 2000, 10, 295–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. O’Doherty, J.P.; Dayan, P.; Friston, K.; Critchley, H.; Dolan, R.J. Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 2003, 38, 329–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rolls, E.T. Convergence of sensory systems in the orbitofrontal cortex in primates and brain design for emotion. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell Evol. Biol. 2004, 281, 1212–1225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Rolls, E.T. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 2004, 55, 11–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Rolls, E.T.; Kringelbach, M.L.; de Araujo, I.E. Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odours in the human brain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003, 18, 695–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Quirk, G.J.; Beer, J.S. Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: Convergence of rat and human studies. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006, 16, 723–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Rudebeck, P.H.; Rich, E.L. Orbitofrontal cortex. Curr. Biol. 2018, 28, R1083–R1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Rolls, E.T. The orbitofrontal cortex and emotion in health and disease, including depression. Neuropsychologia 2019, 128, 14–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Farb, N.A.; Anderson, A.K.; Segal, Z.V. The mindful brain and emotion regulation in mood disorders. Can. J. Psychiatry 2012, 57, 70–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Zhang, Q.; Wang, Z.; Wang, X.; Liu, L.; Zhang, J.; Zhou, R. The Effects of Different Stages of Mindfulness Meditation Training on Emotion Regulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Luberto, C.M.; Shinday, N.; Song, R.; Philpotts, L.L.; Park, E.R.; Fricchione, G.L.; Yeh, G.Y. A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of the Effects of Meditation on Empathy, Compassion, and Prosocial Behaviors. Mindfulness 2018, 9, 708–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Fabio, R.A.; Towey, G.E. Long-term meditation: The relationship between cognitive processes, thinking styles and mindfulness. Cogn. Process 2018, 19, 73–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. van Vugt, M.K.; Jha, A.P. Investigating the impact of mindfulness meditation training on working memory: A mathematical modeling approach. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2011, 11, 344–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Boyke, J.; Driemeyer, J.; Gaser, C.; Buchel, C.; May, A. Training-induced brain structure changes in the elderly. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 7031–7035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Draganski, B.; Gaser, C.; Busch, V.; Schuierer, G.; Bogdahn, U.; May, A. Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 2004, 427, 311–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Draganski, B.; Gaser, C.; Kempermann, G.; Kuhn, H.G.; Winkler, J.; Buchel, C.; May, A. Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 6314–6317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Ansell, E.B.; Rando, K.; Tuit, K.; Guarnaccia, J.; Sinha, R. Cumulative adversity and smaller gray matter volume in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and insula regions. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 72, 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Varga, Z.; Csabai, D.; Miseta, A.; Wiborg, O.; Czeh, B. Chronic stress affects the number of GABAergic neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex of rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 316, 104–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Links between age and orbitofrontal gray matter. Left: probability maps of orbitofrontal areas (Fo1–Fo7) based on 10 post-mortem brains [24,25]. The colors indicate the region-specific probability (blue = 25%, green = 50%, orange = 75%, red = 100%). Right: scatterplots depict the correlations between age and the region-specific gray matter volumes in meditators (orange) and controls (blue). Values are adjusted for sex and TIV as per the statistical model.
Figure 1. Links between age and orbitofrontal gray matter. Left: probability maps of orbitofrontal areas (Fo1–Fo7) based on 10 post-mortem brains [24,25]. The colors indicate the region-specific probability (blue = 25%, green = 50%, orange = 75%, red = 100%). Right: scatterplots depict the correlations between age and the region-specific gray matter volumes in meditators (orange) and controls (blue). Values are adjusted for sex and TIV as per the statistical model.
Brainsci 13 01677 g001
Table 1. Group-by-age interactions.
Table 1. Group-by-age interactions.
AreaLeft HemisphereRight Hemisphere
Effect Size (Cohen’s d)Significance (t)Significance
(p, FDR-Corrected)
Effect Size (Cohen’s d)Significance
(t)
Significance,
(p, FDR-Corrected)
OFC0.6443.1230.004 *0.6633.2120.004 *
Fo10.3041.4730.0750.3191.5450.075
Fo20.3741.8140.048 *0.5132.4880.023 *
Fo30.4702.2760.024 *0.6283.0440.006 *
Fo40.8063.9090.002 *0.5252.5470.020 *
Fo50.4392.1300.026 *0.3311.6030.058 T
Fo60.2841.3760.0750.4412.1380.024 *
Fo70.4332.0970.026 *0.4802.3250.024 *
OFC = Orbitofrontal cortex (composite of subareas Fo1-7); FDR = false discovery rate. * Significant; T trend for significance (p-value, uncorrected = 0.047).
Table 2. Age-related links and annual volume loss at age 50, separately within meditators and controls.
Table 2. Age-related links and annual volume loss at age 50, separately within meditators and controls.
AreaMeditatorsControls
Correlation Coefficient (r)Significance
(p, FDR-Corrected)
Volume Loss
(%)
Correlation Coefficient (r)Significance
(p, FDR-Corrected)
Volume Loss
(%)
LeftOFC−0.498<0.001 *−0.376−0.674<0.001 *−0.704
Fo1−0.434<0.001 *−0.477−0.522<0.001 *−0.696
Fo2−0.451<0.001 *−0.514−0.561<0.001 *−0.787
Fo3−0.3300.001 *−0.318−0.519<0.001 *−0.656
Fo4−0.1050.153−0.098−0.526<0.001 *−0.686
Fo5−0.2940.003 *−0.313−0.483<0.001 *−0.667
Fo6−0.409<0.001 *−0.489−0.495<0.001 *−0.740
Fo7−0.3860.001 *−0.421−0.540<0.001 *−0.756
RightOFC−0.506<0.001 *−0.403−0.683<0.001 *−0.751
Fo1−0.461<0.001 *−0.559−0.547<0.001 *−0.812
Fo2−0.399<0.001 *−0.431−0.578<0.001 *−0.801
Fo3−0.416<0.001 *−0.362−0.626<0.001 *−0.754
Fo4−0.3050.003 *−0.329−0.526<0.001 *−0.734
Fo5−0.3450.001 *−0.379−0.469<0.001 *−0.640
Fo6−0.3470.001 *−0.391−0.518<0.001 *−0.739
Fo7−0.407<0.001 *−0.407−0.571<0.001 *−0.745
FDR = false discovery rate. * Significant.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kurth, F.; Strohmaier, S.; Luders, E. Reduced Age-Related Gray Matter Loss in the Orbitofrontal Cortex in Long-Term Meditators. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1677. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13121677

AMA Style

Kurth F, Strohmaier S, Luders E. Reduced Age-Related Gray Matter Loss in the Orbitofrontal Cortex in Long-Term Meditators. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(12):1677. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13121677

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kurth, Florian, Sarah Strohmaier, and Eileen Luders. 2023. "Reduced Age-Related Gray Matter Loss in the Orbitofrontal Cortex in Long-Term Meditators" Brain Sciences 13, no. 12: 1677. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13121677

APA Style

Kurth, F., Strohmaier, S., & Luders, E. (2023). Reduced Age-Related Gray Matter Loss in the Orbitofrontal Cortex in Long-Term Meditators. Brain Sciences, 13(12), 1677. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13121677

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop