1. Introduction
As one of the world’s largest seafarer-producing nations, Indonesia plays a critical role in supplying qualified maritime personnel to the global shipping industry. With a maritime workforce spanning thousands of vessels worldwide, the country has made continued efforts to enhance the competence, adaptability, and safety awareness of its seafarers in line with international standards [
1]. This commitment is particularly vital given the increasingly complex navigational environments and stricter global safety regulations. Among the most effective tools for maintaining and improving seafarer competence is simulation-based training, which enables navigators to experience high-risk scenarios (for example, a ship encounters in restricted waters) without real-world consequences. Simulator-based learning offers a controlled, repeatable, and measurable platform for developing technical skills, decision-making, and stress management under pressure. This approach is especially valuable in countries such as Indonesia, where preparing seafarers for international deployment requires both technological capability and contextual relevance [
2]. Accordingly, this study leverages the experience of Indonesian navigators to investigate the physiological and psychological impacts of encounter scenarios derived from real automatic identification system (AIS) data, thereby contributing to the global research and advancing locally grounded maritime education and training.
Despite the growing body of international research on simulator-based training, few studies have specifically addressed how ship navigators operating in dense and complex traffic environments experience stress during close-quarter encounters. The existing studies have largely focused on generic simulation tasks or non-specific traffic patterns, leaving a gap in understanding how culturally and operationally distinct navigators manage stress in realistic, data-driven scenarios. This study builds upon our previous research on maritime traffic risk assessment, where we integrated the functional resonance analysis method (FRAM), an AIS trajectory analysis, and risk mapping to identify high-risk encounter zones and sociotechnical variability in vessel traffic services (VTS) operations [
3]. While the prior study focused on mapping systemic risks and predicting accident-prone areas from an operational perspective, the present study extends this inquiry by examining how such risk-laden navigational situations impact individual navigator stress responses. By situating the stress analysis within empirically identified high-risk encounter zones, this research bridges systemic risk assessments and human-centered performance analyses.
Understanding the effects of ship encounters on navigator stress levels is crucial for improving maritime safety. These situations, characterized by high cognitive demands, require navigators to make rapid decisions under pressure. According to cognitive load theory, the human brain has a finite capacity to process information, and when overloaded, this capacity can impair performance, leading to errors and accidents [
4]. Ship encounters can cause a rapidly increased cognitive load, which in high-stress environments may delay response times and elevate the risk of human error [
4]. By examining how Indonesian seafarers respond to close-quarter ship encounters within a controlled simulator environment, this study aimed to uncover the dynamic relationships between perceived stress, cognitive workload, and physiological stress responses across varying encounter geometries. Each simulated scenario reflects realistic navigational conditions commonly experienced in dense traffic areas, thereby preserving the ecological validity. The encounter designs were derived from our prior marine traffic risk assessment study [
3], ensuring alignment with empirically identified high-risk patterns. This data-driven approach not only supports a nuanced understanding of navigator stress in operational contexts but also informs the development of intelligent, risk-aware maritime training programs.
The introduction of high-fidelity simulators has provided researchers with valuable tools to study stress and cognitive loading in controlled settings. A simulation allows for the replication of complex navigational scenarios where multiple ships interact, enabling detailed observations of navigators’ responses. High-fidelity simulators closely replicate real-world conditions, creating a realistic training environment that better prepares navigators for the cognitive and emotional challenges encountered in maritime operations [
5]. This study aimed to understand how various factors contribute to or mitigate navigator stress during complex ship encounters. Stress was assessed using the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), administered prior to the simulation, and the physiological responses were measured during the simulation using heart rate variability (HRV) data collected via a Polar
® H10 chest strap. After completing the simulation, the participants evaluated their perceived workload using the NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX). The findings are expected to offer insights into how simulator-based training can be optimized for enhanced effectiveness and safety. Specifically, this study examined how simulated ship encounters impact navigator stress levels and the extent to which simulator environments influence the cognitive load and stress management during high-risk scenarios comparable to those encountered in real life.
2. Literature Review
In confined waters or when approaching a port, the bridge team typically includes the captain, officer, and helmsman, with additional watchmen as needed, and a pilot in compulsory pilotage areas. This team is responsible for complex and high-stakes navigation tasks, which place considerable demands on decision-making and teamwork. Research indicates that the intricate decision-making required during port approaches generates significant stress for ship navigators. When faced with critical navigation tasks, such as maneuvering through tight spaces while entering or exiting ports, these stress-inducing demands can impair navigators’ ability to operate the ship safely and effectively [
6].
When navigators faced this so-called “crisis routine navigation”, they were aware of the impending accident, yet their actions were influenced by uncertainty and time pressure. This situation elevates cognitive stress, which may inadvertently lead to degraded supervisory control behavior. In such contexts, operators may increasingly rely on automation and reduce active system engagement, a condition referred to as “out-of-the-loop” (OOTL) performance [
7,
8]. Stress does not directly cause OOTL states but may act as a catalyst that exacerbates vulnerabilities, including diminished situational awareness, overreliance on automated outputs, and reduced manual control readiness. In high-stakes maritime navigation, stress may either enhance alertness or conversely lead to cognitive disengagement, depending on the task demands, crew experience, and system design [
9]. This dynamic is supported by evidence indicating that while human operators excel at interpreting complex navigational information, prolonged passive monitoring tasks are often more effectively managed using automation, with human intervention occurring only in response to significant anomalies [
10].
The subsequent research delves into the execution and outcomes of decision support systems intended to assist navigators during these crucial stages. By giving prompt and accurate navigational judgments based on complex algorithmic calculations and as an addition to an automatic radar plotting aid, advanced intelligent maneuvering systems assist the decision-maker, which is the navigator to help prevent collisions. The goal of these technologies is to improve situational awareness while lessening the operational burden on navigators but integrating such technologies also presents new difficulties, such as altering work procedures and possibly causing the navigators to become overly dependent on technology, which in some situations ironically makes people more stressed [
11]. This degradation of timely decision-making and the stress induced by altered operational routines can increase the likelihood of near misses, accidents, and ultimately casualties or environmental pollution. These factors are strongly associated with human error, which remains a predominant cause of maritime incidents, with numerous studies consistently showing that over 80% of marine accidents involve human-related errors [
12].
In Indonesia, human error is a major contributing factor in maritime accidents [
13,
14]. The specific contributing elements include inadequate knowledge of collision regulations and lack of familiarity with ship equipment [
15]. Regarding navigational familiarization, several recommendations issued in Indonesian maritime accident reports from 2007 to 2023 remain unresolved [
16]. Physical stressors have also been identified as key factors influencing seafarers’ stress, including heat exposure, noise, ship motion, seasickness, physically demanding work, lack of exercise, and climatic variability during voyages [
17]. Among these, the most significant stressor was high work demands, particularly among crew in the deck department [
18].
Recent research on Indonesian seafarers’ stresses also revealed that performance among seafarers was directly impacted by job satisfaction and work stress. Seafarer performance directly affected shipping safety, while job satisfaction and work stress indirectly affected shipping safety through seafarer performance. This indicated that seafarer performance was most directly impacted by work stress rather than job satisfaction and that seafarer performance was most directly correlated with shipping safety [
19].
Maritime navigators, despite reporting generally low levels of perceived stress, experience stress primarily shaped by self-doubt in their coping abilities, which highlights the importance of self-efficacy in safeguarding against burnout and maintaining occupational resilience [
20]. Additionally, perceived stress is significantly moderated by individual resilience and prior seafaring experience, although the time spent at sea does not appear to be a determining factor [
21]. Dispositional resilience has been found to be the strongest predictor of perceived stress, although a just and supportive work environment remains essential for mitigating its impact [
22]. Positive spillovers from personal life, particularly marital satisfaction, also play a key role in buffering the effects of stress and reducing anxiety [
23].
While these personal and occupational factors contribute to baseline stress, navigational situations, particularly ship encounters, introduce acute stressors that cause an elevated mental workload (MW) and complicate decision-making processes. According to several studies, these factors are key contributors to human errors, often leading to accidents. When the MW exceeds manageable thresholds, performance declines [
24], impairing decision-making and increasing the likelihood of errors [
25], thereby compromising safety [
26]. An excessive workload, often associated with high levels of stress [
27], also contributes to fatigue, which severely affects ship safety. Fatigue results from various factors, including inadequate and poor-quality sleep and rest [
28]. This effect is particularly significant for officers responsible for the vessel’s safety and security. In the era of automation, operators’ MWs are similarly impacted [
29]. Therefore, managing the MW is critical for ensuring safety, especially during ship encounters in port approaches or confined waters.
In this study, we assessed these stress and workload dynamics using two complementary approaches. First, the PSS [
30], a widely used self-report scale for measuring perceived stress [
31], captured the subjective stress experiences of ship navigators. Second, the physiological responses were evaluated through HRV, which measures the variation in time intervals between heartbeats and reflects autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity. HRV was selected as a physiological indicator due to its established role in reflecting stress-related autonomic regulation [
32]. Typically, an individual’s HRV is higher during restful states, indicating parasympathetic (vagal) dominance, and it decreases during periods of physical or psychological stress, reflecting heightened sympathetic tone. The HRV levels also vary with individual factors, generally being higher in younger, physically fit, and healthy populations [
33,
34].
The ANS regulates an individual’s heart rate (HR) through a balance of sympathetic and parasympathetic inputs. Sympathetic activation increases an individual’s HR and reduces their HRV, while parasympathetic activity lowers an individual’s HR and increases their HRV [
35]. This balance is modulated by the central autonomic network, with rapid HR fluctuations mediated by mechanisms such as the baroreceptor reflex and respiratory sinus arrhythmia. HRV is most accurately captured through electrocardiography (ECG), although calibrated non-ECG methods such as photoplethysmography can also be used.
Previous research has consistently demonstrated an inverse relationship between perceived stress (as measured by the PSS) and HRV, reflecting a physiological link between psychological stress and autonomic dysregulation [
36]. For example, studies among nurses exposed to chronic occupational stress observed significant reductions in HRV, particularly in parasympathetic-associated frequency-domain measures [
37]. Similarly, simulator-based research involving ship officers found that high-stress navigational tasks triggered transient increases in low-frequency (LF) power indicating sympathetic arousal alongside overall reductions in HRV. In contrast, low-demand scenarios were associated with greater high-frequency (HF) power and elevated HRV, reflecting parasympathetic dominance [
38].
For the MW, the NASA-TLX questionnaire is widely used for assessing cognitive workload [
39], and is particularly valuable in ship navigation simulators to understand the navigators’ workload during complex tasks. This multidimensional assessment tool was used to evaluate MWs across six dimensions—mental demand (MD), physical demand (PD), temporal demand (TD), performance (OP), effort (EF), and frustration level (FR)—allowing for a detailed understanding of cognitive challenges, especially in high-stress scenarios such as ship encounters [
40]. Research shows that the perceived workload measured by NASA-TLX rises significantly with task complexity, as seen in scenarios involving multiple vessels, and correlates with physiological stress indicators, such as heart rate [
41]. Integrating NASA-TLX into training can help identify workload thresholds, or “redlines”, beyond which navigator performance declines due to excessive cognitive load, highlighting the need for targeted training interventions [
42,
43]. Thus, by integrating PSS and HRV analyses in ship encounter scenarios, a comprehensive understanding of the psychological stress levels and physiological responses of individuals may be achieved. This approach has the potential to enhance performance, well-being, and decision-making in navigation tasks. Additionally, it aligns with proposed future research in the field of human error within maritime transport accidents, which advocates for the incorporation of diverse theoretical perspectives and the application of research methodologies from the social and human sciences [
44]. Furthermore, by also using NASA-TLX assessments with objective physiological measures, this strengthens the understanding of workload effects, offering a holistic approach to improving navigator performance and ensuring safety under stress [
45].
3. Methodology
This study used the results of a risk assessment on Makassar Port, Indonesia from a previous study using AIS data to reveal ship behavior, or in this case the navigator’s reactions when navigating the waters and encounters with another ship during port approach [
3]. From the analysis of the AIS data, the potential risk of a ship’s encounters was then assessed using a perception-based model of risk, developed for Korean seafarers [
46]. The general framework of this research is shown in
Figure 1.
3.1. Previous Work in Risk Assessment in the Study Area
The traffic analysis studies were conducted in previous research utilizing 7 days of AIS data in the study area, which was the entry to Makassar Port, Indonesia [
3]. As one of major the ports in Indonesia, the encounter situations found throughout the aforementioned analysis were then recreated as scenarios in the simulator. In this study, individual ship encounter cases were selected and labeled using the PARK (Potential Assessment of Risk) model [
46], which assigns a risk value from 1 (extremely safe) to 7 (extremely dangerous) based on the vessel type, size, geometry, speed, and proximity factors (CPA, TCPA). Cases with a model-generated risk value of 7, classified as “extremely dangerous”, were selected for a further analysis, as illustrated in
Figure 2.
The details of the selected cases in
Figure 2 were then recreated into ten specific encounter cases with six different segments as follows.
The selected cases in
Table 1 represent a ship’s encounters over 360 degrees, related to the individual’s own ship within a one hour simulation time, while case number 10 was not considered as an encounter situation. Furthermore, the results of the analysis in the study area found that feeder-sized container ships represent the majority of the ships in the area; thus, the simulation also used a similar ship as the individual’s own ship, with the target ship varied between a handy size tanker ship and similar feeder container ship. The details of the model ships used in this experiment are presented in
Table 2.
3.2. Experimental Design and Procedures
In this study, the K-Sim® Navigation—Ships Bridge Simulator with 360° view made by Kongsberg Digital AS (Lysaker, Norway) was used. This simulator was installed in Barombong Maritime Polytechnic (Makassar, Indonesia). The simulated encounter scenarios were not arbitrarily designed but were derived from real-world AIS data captured in high-density maritime zones with a known history of collision risks. This data-driven foundation ensured ecological validity by replicating actual vessel movements, encounter geometries, and navigational stressors typical of daily operations.
Risk assessment criteria such as the proximity thresholds (CPA, TCPA), encounter types (crossing, overtaking, head-on), and collision probability were used to systematically select realistic, high-risk situations likely to induce navigator stress. Although this study was centered on Indonesian seafarers, references to Korean seafarers were incorporated where relevant due to their comparable training regimes, vessel operations, and regulatory standards, such as the Standard of Training, Certification, and Watchkeeping (STCW).
The study involved 24 male participants acting as ship captains, supported by officers and helmsmen to handle the ship’s steering commands, as depicted in the
Figure 3. All of them were active male seafarers with normal hearing and good eyesight, who were not taking ongoing medication and were healthy and fit to perform the simulation. They were also familiarized with the simulator equipment and had performed in several scenarios during their studies. The participant demographics can be seen in the
Table 3. Before the simulation commenced, all of them received a briefing one day prior and were instructed to have adequate rest at night (more than 7 h of sleep), without consuming any alcohol or caffeine. They were also told to avoid any activities involving high stress or resulting in physical fatigue. All of the relevant ethical matters were discussed earlier with the university’s research committee and approved.
Table 3 shows that the demographics of the participants are equally spread, with each age bracket making up 50.0%. The experience groups showed a dominance of highly experience navigators. The participants had a mean age of 39.88 years (SD = 5.68). While the gender composition was not the focus of this study, the age distribution was relatively consistent across the sample. We acknowledge that non-balanced sample characteristics may introduce interpretive bias; therefore, we have reported key demographic metrics for transparency.
Prior to the commencement of the simulation, all participants completed the PSS questionnaire. They then wore a Polar H10 chest strap to collect HRV data via the Elite HRV application installed on a smartphone. As a backup to the chest strap and for quick visual monitoring of their heart rate data, the participants also wore a Polar Vantage V wristwatch. Both the chest strap and the wristwatch were connected via Bluetooth. Once everything was set up, the scenario then commenced according to the scenarios given in
Figure 4 and the selected cases shown in
Table 1. The overall scenarios were depicted as follows.
The simulation scenarios were constructed based on AIS-derived encounter data and risk assessments to reflect realistic and high-risk navigational situations commonly faced by seafarers. Each simulation segment was designed to reflect distinct encounter geometries and was completed at the vessel’s simulated desired speed set by each participant independently. On average, each segment lasted approximately 8 to 10 min, which served as the effective time window for calculating physiological metrics such as the RR interval, SDNN, RMSSD, and LF/HF ratio. This duration complies with accepted standards for short-term HRV analyses.
The simulation environment was set to daylight conditions, with the sea state set to a light breeze (Beaufort scale 2) and the north-easterly wind set to 4–6 knots, with minimal currents. To meet the required time, the distances were limited to 14 nautical miles from the start point to the finish point, and also to exacerbate the simulation, each participant was asked to arrive at the finish point at the exact estimated time of arrival that was set and agreed upon once the simulation began. After completing the simulation, the result of the HRV recording was then processed in Kubios computer software version 4.1.0, and once again all participants were required to fill in the NASA-TLX questionnaire.
3.3. PSS, HRV, and NASA TLX Data Collection and Processing
In this experiment, the PSS-10 scale was used to assess the subjective stress levels of the navigators. The total scores were categorized into three stress levels, low stress (0–13), moderate stress (14–26), and high stress (27–40), following the classification guidelines of [
30,
47]. This categorical approach has been applied in several maritime studies on seafarer stress [
20,
21,
22,
23]. The 10 item version was selected for its superior psychometric properties compared to other PSS variants, ensuring both reliability and validity in measuring perceived stress [
48].
In the HRV analysis, the R wave represents the peak change in the ECG, and the RR interval (RRI) in milliseconds is defined as the time between two consecutive R waves. The HRV data characteristics obtained from the measurements include both time-domain and frequency-domain indices. Time-domain indices include the MRR (mean RR interval), SDNN (standard deviation of normal RR intervals), and RMSSD (root mean square of successive RR interval differences). Frequency-domain indices are derived from the power spectrum density of the resampled RRI data, which include low-frequency (LF; power spectrum in the frequency range of 0.04–0.15 Hz) results, high-frequency (HF; power spectrum in the frequency range of 0.15–0.4 Hz) results, and the LF/HF ratio (the ratio of low- to high-frequency results). The HRV indices relate to stress; thus, the indices selected to be utilized in this research were the MRR, SDNN, RMSSD, and LF/HF ratio [
32,
49,
50]. The sample RR intervals are depicted in
Figure 5.
For NASA-TLX, there were two types of information for each dimension—weights and scores. The weights reflect the perceived importance of each dimension as a contributor to MW in the specific task, while the scores represent the subjectively experienced magnitude of MW for each dimension. To determine the weights, subjects perform pairwise comparisons for each dimension pair, assigning a score of one to the dimension that contributes more to MW and zero to the other. After completing all 15 pairwise comparisons, the total score for each dimension ranges from zero to five. During task engagement, subjects rate each workload dimension based on their experienced MW. The performance dimension is rated on a scale ranging from 0 (good) to 100 (poor), while all other dimensions use a scale ranging from 0 (low) to 100 (high). This research focused specifically on the original version of NASA-TLX, as introduced [
39] with the weighted scale.
3.4. Design, Variables, and Statistical Analysis
The analysis investigated how both subjective and physiological measures reflect navigator stress and workloads during simulated ship encounters. The dependent variables included the perceived stress (from the result of the PSS), workload (from the weighted scale of the NASA-TLX), and physiological responses (HRV metrics, including the time-domain MRR, SDNN, and RMSSD and frequency-domain LF/HF ratio). These were modeled against independent variables such as age, experience, the encounter segment, and the navigational context.
Table 4 presents an overview of the hypothesized variable relationships and the initial modeling framework.
It is important to note that while the PSS and NASA-TLX were administered once per participant at the global level (before and after the simulation), the HRV indicators were collected separately for each segment. Consequently, each segment was included as an independent variable to capture physiological differences across encounter contexts and their contribution to global stress outcomes. This modeling choice acknowledges that while subjective reports are global and not tied to individual segments, localized physiological changes may still relate to overall stress. This assumption is well aligned with the human factors literature, which links localized physiological arousal with global perceptions of cognitive and emotional demand in simulation-based settings.
To explore the dynamics between subjective and physiological responses, the analysis was structured into two exploratory phases:
Additionally, to model global stress outcomes using localized physiological inputs, an exploratory regression strategy was adopted with the PSS as the dependent variable and segment-based HRV values (e.g., MRR2, LF/HF2) as predictors. This approach reflects the assumption that critical segments disproportionately affect global stress perceptions. The statistical models were refined using backward stepwise selection informed by the significance (p < 0.05), practical effect size, and model fit (e.g., adjusted R2 for linear models). Variables with minimal contribution or multicollinearity were excluded to improve the parsimony and interpretability. This multi-layered framework enabled us to investigate bidirectional pathways of how stress affects physiological responses and conversely how physiological arousal predicts workload and stress perceptions. Together, these layers provide a comprehensive understanding of navigator stress during complex maritime scenarios.
5. Discussion
This study began with an analysis of AIS data, which were used to reconstruct specific multi-ship encounter scenarios identified during the risk assessment phase, as illustrated in
Figure 4. Stress was evaluated through three instruments—the PSS, HRV, and NASA-TLX. The participant pool consisted entirely of male officers due to availability constraints at the training facility, while we ensured a sample balance across age and experience. Specifically, 50% of the participants were below 40 years of age and 50% above, while their experience levels were distributed among the low, moderate, and high categories. Although this demographic composition reflects realistic crew compositions in Indonesian merchant marine training institutions, we acknowledge that the gender imbalance may limit the generalizability. This limitation will be addressed as part of our future research recommendations, as well as with added imaginary scenarios that put seafarers in no-way-out situations (for example, an inevitable grounding or collision, where we then add a man overboard situation to see how different groups of seafarers would cope).
The significant difference in PSS scores between the younger and older participants suggests that age may influence how stress is perceived. Younger individuals may be more sensitive to stress or experience it more intensely, potentially due to psychosocial or environmental factors such as work demands, educational pressures, or life transitions. Further research is warranted to investigate the underlying mechanisms driving these age-related differences in perceived stress.
Similarly, an individual’s experience level also appears to play a nuanced role in their perceived stress. The observation that stress perceptions differ significantly between low and moderate experience groups but not between moderate and high experience groups indicates that perceived stress may decrease as individuals gain familiarity with tasks and responsibilities. Specifically, the significant difference observed between the low-experience (group 1) and moderate-experience (Group 2) participants may imply that individuals with little experience are more prone to stress due to their unfamiliarity with operational contexts, a finding that aligns with [
55]. Conversely, those in the moderate-experience group may have adapted to their roles, leading to more manageable stress levels. The absence of significant differences between the moderate- and high-experience groups suggests that perceived stress levels may plateau after a certain threshold of experience is reached. These patterns are further supported by exploratory results from non-parametric pairwise comparisons, which provide additional descriptive insights. Although the Kruskal–Wallis test did not reveal overall statistical significance, the comparison between group 1 and group 2 yielded a test statistic of −9.846 and a
p-value of 0.027 (uncorrected), suggesting a potential difference worth further investigation. However, due to the elevated risk of type I errors in this exploratory setting, these findings should be interpreted cautiously and are not considered confirmatory statistical evidence.
This study supports the role of sympathetic dominance in high-stress conditions, as reflected in elevated LF/HF values across segments 1, 2, and 5 (
Figure 8). These findings reinforce the physiological plausibility of subjective stress assessments in simulator environments. Building on this, regression analyses were conducted to explore how physiological variables predict subjective workload dimensions. The results highlight the protective role of age and the strong influence of ANS markers, such as LFHF2 and MRR2, in stress perception. The results suggest that physiological indicators may play a more dominant role in stress modulation than subjective cognitive demands. Future research should explore potential interaction effects, non-linear relationships, and larger sample sizes to enhance the predictive accuracy of physiological stress models.
In addition to the ANOVA and t-test analyses, we conducted multiple linear regression analyses to investigate potential predictors of subjective workloads across different NASA-TLX subdimensions. The results showed that age and the LF/HF ratio in segment 2 significantly predicted the TD scores (p = 0.0327 and p = 0.0123, respectively), indicating that age-related physiological shifts and sympathetic arousal during high-alert crossings may contribute to an increased temporal workload. Moreover, the LF/HF ratio in segment 6 was significantly associated with the OP (p = 0.0285), indicating that elevated sympathetic activity in this segment may contribute to increased performance-related workload. While the model fit was modest (adjusted R2 scores of 0.169 for temporal and 0.143 for performance results), these findings offer additional insight into how physiological signals during specific navigational encounters contribute to subjective workload experiences.
The ordinal regression model yielded interpretable coefficients and passed the proportional odds assumption test. However, due to the limited sample size, uncorrected multiple comparisons, and reliance on segment-level HRV inputs, the robustness of the model’s labeling may be constrained. As such, the model should be considered preliminary, with future validation recommended using larger datasets and external cohorts to ensure its generalizability.
The experience-related variability in stress perceptions further highlights the interplay between personal resilience, stress management strategies, and job demands. While low-experience individuals may struggle more with stress due to a lack of familiarity, those in moderate- or high-experience groups may have already developed the necessary skills and coping mechanisms to handle stressors, making their stress levels more stable over time. Thus, the variability in stress perceptions based on experience highlights the evolving nature of stress responses as individuals gain exposure and mastery within their roles.
Although this study primarily categorized experience by age and years of service, we acknowledge that an individual’s operational competence and stress-coping ability are also shaped by factors such as their rank, vessel type, and propulsion system familiarity. For instance, extensive experience aboard smaller platform supply vessels (PSVs) may not directly translate to effective command performance on large commercial carriers, particularly those transporting passengers or hazardous cargo such as gas. These vessels differ markedly in their maneuverability, propulsion systems, and risk exposure. Although all participants in this study held valid certifications and possessed diverse sea service backgrounds, we did not stratify their experience by vessel class or propulsion system. Future research studies would benefit from incorporating more granular metrics such as the vessel type, engine configuration, and training specialization to better understand how domain-specific experience affects an individual’s cognitive load and physiological stress responses in simulation-based scenarios.
The regression analysis showed no significant prediction of perceived stress based on an individual’s (CoC) status, last rank, last ship type, age, or total seafaring experience. This suggests that within simulated environments, an individual’s hierarchical status and formal qualifications may not meaningfully differentiate their stress perception results. However, the non-parametric analysis using the Kruskal–Wallis test identified a significant difference in NASA-TLX frustration scores between the CoC groups (p = 0.0125). Specifically, the participants holding ANT II licenses exhibited higher frustration levels than those with ANT I, implying that their licensing level may influence their cognitive appraisal of their workload, especially under conditions of task ambiguity or time pressure. No significant differences were observed across the last duty rank or last ship type in any of the subjective workload or physiological stress indicators, indicating that an individual’s prior operational context alone may not account for variability in stress responses during simulation-based tasks.
The MW, which we initially thought would be crucial in the experiment, did not turn out to not have any effect at all on subjective stress, which may have been due to the short time of the experiment. Similarly, the experience of the subject also did not affect their stress, since all of the participants were aware that the closed-environment experiment was not real. The only factor that effected stress across all segments was age, whereby the younger participants had a higher mean RR interval (which translates to higher stress) than the older participants. This suggests that as an individual’s age increases, there is a notable impact on their HR and HRV. This aligns with the idea that older individuals typically exhibit lower heart rates and less variability, which may explain the significant differences in mean RR intervals across age groups. However, the SDNN and RMSSD results did not show significant differences, possibly due to other factors such as individual health conditions or fitness levels, which can affect these measures.
Although most of the research on marine traffic engineering is focused on artificial intelligence (AI), the results of this research could be of benefit to those developing collision avoidance assistance systems for use by navigators on ships. The simulator used in this study closely reflects real-life navigational conditions but its full potential should be further explored in future research. Several studies that also utilized simulator approaches with the MW and HRV being used in different navigation conditions indicated that varying levels of navigation tasks led to significantly different MW and HRV values. Both the MW and HRV increased as the task difficulty heightened [
52]. A model was also developed to recognized the navigator’s workload using a support vector machine. This approach showed that the changes in various eye movement features varied across different workload levels, and it proved to be a high-accuracy model; the same tasks were also conducted in a simulator [
56]. Biosignals were also utilized to assess maritime students’ stress levels, suggesting that training scenarios can be categorized based on stress levels, which were associated with factors such as reduced visibility, equipment malfunctions, and extreme weather conditions. Furthermore, the study showed that elevated stress levels can impair performance in maritime navigation and affect the reliability of sailing routes. These findings offer actionable insights to improve the quality and effectiveness of maritime training programs, ultimately enhancing the navigational safety at sea [
57]. While this study did not employ AI directly, its findings on the encounter geometry and stress responses can inform future AI-based navigator support systems, particularly in adaptive risk-aware training protocols and collision avoidance tools.
Although the spatial distance to nearby ships in close-quarter encounters was not directly analyzed, segment 2 emerged as the most stress-inducing scenario based on statistical findings for both the PSS scores and LF/HF ratio. This segment typically involved the individual’s own ship facing incoming vessels from both the 135° port and starboard bearings, creating a perceptual burden due to increased peripheral awareness demands. Observationally, many of the 24 participants frequently noted simultaneous ship movements on both sides while also maintaining attention toward the fairway ahead. This aligns with the PARK model of navigational risk, which incorporates both internal and external elements. One of the external risk indicators is the crossing factor, including angles of 45°, 90°, and 135°. Notably, the highest coefficient in the model corresponds to the 135° crossing angle, supporting our empirical findings. This is further corroborated by Kim (2020) [
58], who reported that the perceived collision risk was significantly higher in 90° crossing situations, reinforcing the importance of the encounter geometry in risk perception.
The alignment between the subjective (PSS, NASA-TLX) and physiological (HRV) metrics supports the validity of using simulator-based scenarios to assess navigator stress under varied encounter geometries. While AI-based risk assessment remains the future research direction, this study offers foundational insights into the human factors that should inform such development.