1. Introduction
Imagine a tennis player preparing herself for the critical serve (match point for the opponent), and instructing herself to “serve to her (opponents’) backhand, but not hit wide of the tramlines (out)”. Then imagine her anguish five seconds later, as her serve falls wide of the tramlines, causing her to lose the match. A less severe error, such as making a weak serve to the centre of the court or serving box, would at least have given her a chance of playing a rally, winning the point and staying in the match, but instead she made the mistake she least wanted to make, the so-called
ironic error [
1]. Incidents of ironic errors are thought to be frequent—many people will be able to recall a time when they did the precise thing that they wanted to avoid—especially in pressure-filled environments [
2]. For example, an individual may use an inappropriate word that they were trying to suppress during a job interview. Similarly, an Olympic pentathlete revealed that “in some circumstances (e.g., anxiety, pressure), my intention is not to do the best but to avoid making a bad shot. That is when I make a bad shot. When I think about avoiding the error, I make the error” [
3] (p. 252). Furthermore, under certain conditions, the inability to managing pressure in sport is strongly associated with sporting performance by increasing level of anxiety, decreasing thought control and as a result of decreased performance expectancies [
4]. There have been various theories to explain the manner in which excessive pressure can act to break down human performance. With specific regard to the hypothesized effects of anxiety on performance, the theory of ironic processes of mental control [
2] and the De la Pena et al’s [
5] implicit overcompensation hypothesis are largely indiscriminate. That is, both theories propose contradictory explanations that anxiety impairs efficient functioning of processing thoughts that leads to the performer to focus on thoughts that will be detrimental to their performance [
6,
7].
Wegner’s [
2] theory of the ironic process of mental control explains the “tendency to feel, act, and think in ways that are opposite to the intended direction of emotion, behaviour, and cognition” [
1] (p.202). More specifically, Wegner’s [
2] theory proposes that when performers’ brain seeks to make the body perform in an intentional way, the brain requires dual processed in order to work efficiently and overcome the performers’ desired state of mind. These two processes are defined by Wegner [
2] as the operating process and the monitoring process. First, the operating process aims to create the desired state of mind. For example, this process consciously searches for and directs the person toward areas of sensation and memory of the brain that relate to the intentional state of mind. As a result, this mentally demanding process increases the likelihood of the desired state which will be achieved by the conscious operating process [
2,
8].
Second, the subconscious monitoring process searches for signals of failure, more specifically these are undesired actions or thoughts. If this subconscious monitoring process identifies any such failures then it immediately reactivates the conscious operating process, which aims to bring about the regulation by filling the mind with mental contents that are more relevant to the desired state. The mental capacity required by both processes under normal conditions fluctuates and adapts, depending on such factors as the task required [
2,
9]. In daily life, this dual process works automatically and operates as of a feedback loop that allows people effective mental control [
2]. However, under certain conditions such as competition, exam or giving a speech in front of people, there is a limited cognitive space for the effortful conscious operating process to work effectively [
2]. The theory holds that under conditions of reduced cognitive capacity, the monitoring process may have a greater allocation of cognitive resources focused upon it, and thus will outweigh the operating process [
10]. It is in these situations that the ironic monitor comes to the forefront, and duly causes increased sensitivity to actions that are the ironic opposite of those intended: counter-intentional errors [
2,
10]. Therefore, the subconscious monitoring process becomes more prevalent when pressure is on for one’s and mental control begins to work against itself by attending to those undesired actions [
2].
Previous research has shown that focusing on instructions to guide thoughts and actions, especially avoidant instructions, can lead to performance outcomes that ironically are contradictory to our intentions (i.e., engaging in the behaviour one was trying to avoid). For example, Wegner and colleagues [
8] provided support in a golf-putting task when instructed “don’t overshoot the glow spot”. Results revealed that under high cognitive load, participants put the ball past the spot significantly more often, than those under a low cognitive load condition [
8]. However, using the number of retention tasks to induce cognitive load is not related to golf or any other sport; therefore, that type of laboratory task leads to a lack of ecological validity.
Although the previous research [
7,
11,
12,
13] provided considerable support for Wegner’s [
2] theory, De la Pena and colleagues [
5] revealed conflicting results. For example, in their investigation golf players who were instructed not to putt short of the target overcompensated when cognitively loaded (e.g., visual, cognitive, auditory, or self-presentation) and putted significantly farther than under conditions of no cognitive load. However, in this investigation, using the visual, auditory type of methods to induce cognitive load lacks an ecological validity similar to Wegner and colleague’s examination [
8]. Malhotra and colleagues [
14] provided support for the implicit overcompensation hypothesis in highly automatized skills in driving when drivers were given avoidant instructions (e.g., stay away from the centreline); the results revealed over compensatory behaviour and therefore participants drove further away from the centreline.
Moreover, time pressures inherent in motor tasks likely present an additional load e.g., Ref [
15] which could increase the likelihood of ironic errors under pressurized situations. For instance, police and military marksmen may have less than one second to identify an armed on-rusher as friend or foe and to react accordingly. The consequences of an ironic error in this situation could be grave. Indeed, annual reports of police performance in the United States show that while police officers perform relatively well on low-pressure shooting tests (with hit percentages above 90%), they perform substantially worse when engaged in firing in the line of duty under high-pressure (with hit percentages around or even below 50%; e.g., Ref [
16] for a review see Ref [
17]). The opening example of a tennis player hitting her return wide of the tramlines is another example of an ironic error during a time-constrained motor task.
However, research to date is limited that has demonstrated an overcompensatory effect in response to avoidant instructions [
5,
18] especially under ecologically valid experiments and pressure (e.g., mental load). For example, De la Pena and colleagues [
5] stated that in their investigations, methods of inducing mental load may have failed to sufficiently tax participants’ cognitive resources. Woodman and colleagues [
7] acknowledged that future investigations of ironic processes of mental control theory [
2] should continue to ensure that participants’ cognitive resources are significantly taxed in an ecologically valid manner. Therefore, to increase scientific rigour and ecological validity, the aim of the current study was to examine the effects of avoidant instructions and to compare these predictions using a tennis serving task. When serving in tennis, we often see unforced double fault errors or unintended serves (e.g., hitting the undesired side of the court or hitting the net) during the game; however, we do not know are these errors occur due to simply a part of general performance deterioration that can be caused by an overriding implicit counter massage or ironically and specifically due to undesired state of mind. The main research question of the present research is: are these pressure-induced performance failures associated with specifically and precisely ironic effects or activating an overriding implicit counter message?
4. Discussion
The primary goal of the present study was to perform a direct test of the movement predictions for the implicit overcompensation hypothesis and ironic processes theory of pressure-induced performance errors. As hypothesized, under the high-anxiety condition, compared to the low-anxiety condition, participants’ ironic error hits were significantly farther from the target zone and significantly farther into the ironic error zone. In other words, when anxious, participants performed ironically [
7] which provides support for the Wegner’s [
2] theory rather than an implicit overcompensation hypothesis. More specifically, results revealed that when instructed not hit into the certain part of the serving box, tennis players hit significantly more balls into the incorrect zone under pressure.
This result contrasts the predictions made by the theory of the implicit overcompensation hypothesis. According to De la Pena et al. [
5], negative or unintentional instructions may lead to overcompensation effect of the performers. For example, in their investigation, De la Pena and colleagues [
5] found that the instruction “not to undershoot” creates an implicit message that it is better to do in an opposite way that ends up in overshooting the target. This clearly would lead performers to an implicit overcompensation of movement rather than the ironic effect of doing something unintentionally. However, this is not an issue in the present study; the results demonstrated that when anxious, participants served significantly more serves into the avoided zone (e.g., zone number 2). More specifically, despite manipulating the task instruction and creating the pressurized situation for tennis players, their target serves and non-target non-ironic error serves did not change across anxiety conditions. Previous research provided support for Wegner’s theory [
2] and found a similar effect for the non-target ironic error; however, this is the first study that did not find any difference in target serves from low- to high-anxiety conditions. The findings of the present research provide better support for Wegner’s theory as the only change was specifically for the non-target ironic error zone. Consequently, the results of the present study clearly explain that given instruction not to serve in a certain place may ironically lead performers to serve exactly into the non-target ironic error zone under pressure. Malhotra and colleagues [
14] found opposite results in a driving motor control task that avoidant instructions did not lead participants to ironic behaviours. In the same study, researchers reported that the overcompensatory effects of behaviour were present only when driving without cognitive load (e.g., tone counting). In other words, avoidant instructions led drivers to overcompensate when driving with no cognitive load. However, an important difference between the theories is that Wegner’s [
2] theory predicts that performance when cognitively loaded (e.g., anxious) will break down in a precisely ironic manner (e.g., hitting the golf ball in the water—the one spot to avoid). As a result, given an avoidant instruction with no cognitive load may not lead participants to ironic behaviour and equally may not provide clear comparative results for these theories.
Indeed, when exploring Wegner’s [
2] ironic processes of mental control theory in motor control and sports settings, it is essential to note that this is not very different in nature to basic cognitive models of attention and interference, e.g., Ref [
24,
25], that can be used in other domains such as education, business and military services. Such models suggest that in evaluative settings, performance levels under pressure will be reduced [
26] in other domains too. Specifically, these models maintain that more errors are made as cognitive resources are diverted away from the task by cognitively distracting responses such as worry, emotionality and task generated interference [
27]. More specifically, the states of mind people feel before and during task engagement allow them to perform tasks of varying natures and difficulties such as those encountered in the sporting environment [
28]. Therefore, it posits that the unwanted performance errors may be generated by having to respond to factors such as worry and anxiety, causing an overload on cognitive resources, to the point at which capacity to focus on performance suffers, and thus more errors are made. These assumptions need to be explored by future research in different contexts.
From the applied perspective, results emanating from the current study suggest that in order to reduce an athlete’s susceptibility to counter-intentional errors, task instructions given to them should remain simple, positive and action based. This should be particularly apparent under pressure situations, as negative instructions would leave the performer susceptible to counter-intentional errors caused by a monitoring process. It should also be particularly apparent when educating novice performers that non-experts may not possess the psychological skills to deal with adversity [
1]. Therefore, if negative instructions are given to especially novice performers, they may again be particularly susceptible to counter-intentional errors under pressure due to the limited space for the operating process.
Notwithstanding, there remain some limitations and unanswered questions to be tackled by future research. First, although the large within-subject effects (ηp2’s = 0.25) was revealed in the method section of the current study, this study included a relatively small sample size; therefore, using larger sample sizes would be worthwhile to increase statistical power. Second, while a tennis serving task was chosen due to its suitability for evaluating ironic effects in a popular sport, future investigation should employ more complex motor tasks involving attentional demands of the movement. The current research lacks attentional measures such as probe reaction time that requires participants to perform a choice reaction task involving the coordination of multiple joints, as occurs in sport. Lastly, the incidence of counter-intentional errors in relation to gender differences was not explored in the current study due to its relative lack of female participants and to date, there is no study that has explored this. This is particularly apparent based on findings that the self-report of anxiety symptoms and overall anxiety sensitivity of female performers is significantly higher than that of male performers, e.g., Ref [
29]. If this is the case, then the ironic processes theory suggests that female performers may be more susceptible to counter-intentional errors than males. This is because the additional anxiety females are under would place a greater demand on cognitive resources and thus allow a greater possibility to increase non-target ironic errors. Thus, an interesting route to pursue would be possible gender differences in the incidence of counter-intentional errors in future.