A Theory of Guilt Appeals: A Review Showing the Importance of Investigating Cognitive Processes as Mediators between Emotion and Behavior
Abstract
:1. Guilt and Prosocial Behaviors: From Reparation to Backlash Effects
2. What Cognitive Processes Are Involved in Guilt Appeals?
3. Reinterpretation of Studies on Guilt and Prosocial Behavior
4. Extension to Other Emotions: Mediators of the Emotion–Behavior Relationship
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Haidt, J. The moral emotions. In Handbook of Affective Sciences; Davidson, R.J., Scherer, K.R., Goldsmith, H.H., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; pp. 852–870. [Google Scholar]
- Tangney, J.P.; Dearing, R. Shame and Guilt; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Frijda, N.H.; Mesquita, B. The social roles and functions of emotions. In Emotion and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence; Kitayama, S., Markus, H.R., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1994; pp. 51–87. [Google Scholar]
- Keltner, D.; Haidt, J. Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cogn. Emot. 1999, 13, 505–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortony, A.; Clore, G.L.; Collins, A. The Cognitive Structure of Emotions; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Stillwell, A.M.; Heatherton, T.F. Guilt as interpersonal phenomenon. Psychol. Bull. 1994, 115, 243–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Manstead, A.S.R.; Tetlock, P.E.; Manstead, T. Cognitive appraisals and emotional experience: Further evidence. Cogn. Emot. 1989, 3, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, H.B. Shame and Guilt in Neurosis; International Universities Press: New York, NY, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Monteith, M.J. Self-regulation of stereotypical responses: Implications for progress in prejudice reduction. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 469–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Twenge, J.M.; Baumeister, R.F.; DeWall, C.N.; Ciarocco, N.J.; Bartels, J.M. Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, A. The Theory of Moral Sentiments; Kincaid, A., Bell, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1759. [Google Scholar]
- Carlsmith, J.M.; Gross, A.E. Some effects of guilt on compliance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1969, 11, 232–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freedman, J.L.; Wallington, S.A.; Bless, E. Compliance without pressure: The effect of guilt. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1967, 7, 117–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Regan, D.T.; Williams, M.; Sparling, S. Voluntary expiation of guilt: A field experiment. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1972, 24, 42–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlson, M.; Miller, N. Explanation of the relation between negative mood and helping. Psychol. Bull. 1987, 102, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ketelaar, T.; Au, W.T. The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cogn. Emot. 2003, 17, 429–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Hooge, I.E.; Zeelenberg, M.; Breugelmans, S.M. Moral sentiments and co-operation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cogn. Emot. 2007, 21, 1025–1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graton, A.; Ric, F.; Gonzalez, E. Reparation or reactance? The influence of guilt on reaction to persuasive communication. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 62, 40–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelissen, R.M.A.; Dijker, A.J.M.; de Vries, N.K. How to turn a hawk into a dove and vice versa: Interactions between emotions and goals in a give-some dilemma game. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 43, 280–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coulter, R.H.; Pinto, M.B. Guilt appeals in advertising: What are their effects? J. Appl. Psychol. 1995, 80, 697–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alden, D.L.; Crowley, A.E. Sex guilt and receptivity to condom advertising. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 25, 1446–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Agrawal, N.; Duhachek, A. Emotional compatibility and the effectiveness of antidrinking messages: A defensive processing perspective on shame and guilt. J. Mark. Res. 2010, 47, 263–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Becheur, I.; Dib, H.; Merunka, D.; Valette-Florence, P. Emotions of fear, guilt or shame in anti-alcohol messages: Measuring direct effects on persuasion and the moderating role of sensation seeking. Eur. Adv. Consum. Res. 2008, 8, 99–106. [Google Scholar]
- Baek, T.H.; Yoon, S. Guilt and shame: Environmental message framing effects. J. Advert. 2017, 46, 440–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, C.-T. Are guilt appeals a panacea in green advertising? Int. J. Advert. 2012, 31, 741–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Basil, D.Z.; Ridgway, N.M.; Basil, M.D. Guilt appeals: The mediating effect of responsibility. Psychol. Mark. 2006, 23, 1035–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hibbert, S.; Smith, A.; Davies, A.; Ireland, F. Guilt appeals: Persuasion knowledge and charitable giving. Psychol. Mark. 2007, 24, 723–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Hooge, I.E.; Nelissen, R.M.A.; Breugelmans, S.M.; Zeelenberg, M. What is moral about guilt? Acting ‘prosocially’ at the disadvantage of others. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 462–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cryder, E.C.; Springer, S.; Morewedge, C.K. Guilty feelings, targeted actions. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 38, 607–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bessarabova, E.; Turner, M.M.; Fink, E.L.; Blustein, N.B. Extending the Theory of Reactance to Guilt Appeals “You Ain’t Guilting Me into Nothin”. J. Psychol. 2015, 223, 215–224. [Google Scholar]
- O’Keefe, D.J. Guilt and social influence. Commun. Yearb. 2000, 23, 67–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brehm, J.W. Theory of Psychological Reactance; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Brehm, S.S.; Brehm, J.W. Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Forest, D.; Clark, M.S.; Mills, J.; Isen, A.M. Helping as a function of feeling state and nature of the helping behavior. Motiv. Emot. 1979, 3, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isen, A.M.; Simmonds, S.F. The effect of feeling good on a helping task that is incompatible with good mood. Soc. Psychol. 1978, 41, 346–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wicker, F.W.; Payne, G.C.; Morgan, R.D. Participant descriptions of guilt and shame. Motiv. Emot. 1983, 7, 25–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Hooge, I.E.; Breugelmans, S.M.; Zeelenberg, M. Not so ugly after all: When shame acts as a commitment device. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 933–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gausel, N.; Leach, C.W.; Vignoles, V.L.; Brown, R. Defend or repair? Explaining responses to in-group moral failure by disentangling feelings of shame, rejection, and inferiority. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 102, 941–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leach, C.W.; Cidam, A. When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A metaanalysis. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 109, 983–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmader, T.; Lickel, B. The approach and avoidance function of guilt and shame emotions: Comparing reactions to self-caused and other-caused wrongdoing. Motiv. Emot. 2006, 30, 43–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feinberg, M.; Willer, R.; Keltner, D. Flustered and faithful: Embarrassment as a signal of prosociality. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 102, 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zeelenberg, M.; Breugelmans, S.M. The role of interpersonal harm in distinguishing regret from guilt. Emotion 2008, 8, 589–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zeelenberg, M.; Pieters, R. Feeling is for doing: A pragmatic approach to the study of emotions in economic behavior. In Social Psychology and Economics; DeCremer, D., Zeelenberg, M., Murnighan, J.K., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2006; pp. 117–137. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz, N. Feelings-as-information theory. Handb. Theor. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 1, 289–308. [Google Scholar]
- Gouldner, A.W. The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1960, 25, 161–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, H.; Begue, L.; Shankland, R. Guilt and guiltlessness: An integrative review. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2011, 5, 440–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (Vol. 2); Stanford University Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Burnett, M.S.; Lunsford, D.A. Conceptualizing guilt in the consumer decision-making process. J. Consum. Mark. 1994, 11, 33–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortony, A. Is guilt an emotion? Cogn. Emot. 1987, 1, 283–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elison, J. Shame and guilt: A hundred years of apples and oranges. New Ideas Psychol. 2005, 23, 5–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faucher, L.; Tappolet, C. Fear and the focus of attention. Conscious. Emot. 2002, 3, 105–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Broadbent, D.; Broadbent, M. Anxiety and Attentional Bias: State and Trait. Cogn. Emot. 1988, 2, 165–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, E. Processing emotional facial expressions: The role of anxiety and awareness. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2002, 2, 52–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mansell, W.; Clark, D.M.; Ehlers, A.; Chen, Y.-P. Social Anxiety and Attention away from Emotional Faces. Cogn. Emot. 1999, 13, 673–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mogg, K.; Bradley, B.P. Attentional Bias in Generalized Anxiety Disorder Versus Depressive Disorder. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2005, 29, 29–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, J.M.G.; Watts, F.N.; MacLeod, C.; Mathews, A. Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, NH, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Challis, B.H.; Krane, R.V. Mood induction and the priming of semantic memory in a lexical decision task: Asymmetric effects of elation and depression. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 1988, 26, 309–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferraro, F.R.; King, B.; Ronning, B.; Pekarski, K.; Risan, J. Effects of induced emotional state on lexical processing in younger and older adults. J. Psychol. 2003, 137, 262–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zeelenberg, M.; Nelissen, R.M.; Breugelmans, S.M.; Pieters, R. On emotion specificity in decision making: Why feeling is for doing. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2008, 3, 18–27. [Google Scholar]
- Treisman, A.M. Contextual cues in selective listening. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1960, 12, 242–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Broadbent, D.E. Perception and Communication; Pergamon Press: New York, NY, USA, 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Maquestiaux, F. Psychology of Attention; De Boeck: Brussels, Belgium, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- MacLeod, C.; Rutherford, E.M. Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: Mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimuli. Behav. Res. Ther. 1992, 30, 479–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graton, A.; Ric, F. How guilt leads to reparation? Exploring the processes underlying the effects of guilt. Motiv. Emot. 2017, 41, 343–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perloff, R.M. The Dynamics of Persuasion: Communication and Attitudes in the Twenty-First Century; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Nixon, H.K. Attention and Interest in Advertising (No. 72); Columbia University: New York, NY, USA, 1924. [Google Scholar]
- Wedel, M.; Pieters, R. Eye tracking for visual marketing. Found. Trends Mark. 2008, 1, 231–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pieters, R.; Wedel, M. A review of eye-tracking research in marketing. In Review of Marketing Research; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2017; pp. 143–167. [Google Scholar]
- Krugman, D.M.; Fox, R.J.; Fletcher, J.E.; Fischer, P.M.; Rojas, T.H. Do adolescents attend to warnings in cigarette advertising? An eye-tracking approach. J. Advert. Res. 1994, 34, 39–52. [Google Scholar]
- Wedel, M.; Pieters, R. Eye fixations on advertisements and memory for brands: A model and findings. Mark. Sci. 2000, 19, 297–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cotte, J.; Coulter, R.A.; Moore, M. Enhancing or disrupting guilt: The role of ad credibility and perceived manipulative intent. J. Bus. Res. 2005, 58, 361–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Keefe, D.J. Guilt as a mechanism of persuasion. In The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice; Dillard, J.P., Pfau, M., Eds.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2002; pp. 329–344. [Google Scholar]
- Lickel, B.; Schmader, T.; Curtis, M.; Scarnier, M.; Ames, D.R. Vicarious shame and guilt. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2005, 8, 145–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nathanson, D.L. Shame and Pride: Affect, Sex, and the Birth of the Self; Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Elison, J.; Lennon, R.; Pulos, S. Investigating the compass of shame: The development of the Compass of Shame Scale. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2006, 34, 221–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Honk, J.V.; Tuiten, A.; de Haan, E.; vann de Hout, M.; Stam, H. Attentional biases for angry faces: Relationships to trait anger and anxiety. Cogn. Emot. 2001, 15, 279–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cisler, J.M.; Olatunji, B.O.; Lohr, J.M.; Williams, N.L. Attentional bias differences between fear and disgust: Implications for the role of disgust in disgust-related anxiety disorders. Cogn. Emot. 2009, 23, 675–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vogt, J.; Lozo, L.; Koster, E.H.; De Houwer, J. On the role of goal relevance in emotional attention: Disgust evokes early attention to cleanliness. Cogn. Emot. 2011, 25, 466–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Keefe, D.J. Message properties, mediating states, and manipulation checks: Claims, evidence, and data analysis in experimental persuasive message effects research. Commun. Theory 2003, 13, 251–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Graton, A.; Mailliez, M. A Theory of Guilt Appeals: A Review Showing the Importance of Investigating Cognitive Processes as Mediators between Emotion and Behavior. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 117. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120117
Graton A, Mailliez M. A Theory of Guilt Appeals: A Review Showing the Importance of Investigating Cognitive Processes as Mediators between Emotion and Behavior. Behavioral Sciences. 2019; 9(12):117. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120117
Chicago/Turabian StyleGraton, Aurélien, and Melody Mailliez. 2019. "A Theory of Guilt Appeals: A Review Showing the Importance of Investigating Cognitive Processes as Mediators between Emotion and Behavior" Behavioral Sciences 9, no. 12: 117. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120117
APA StyleGraton, A., & Mailliez, M. (2019). A Theory of Guilt Appeals: A Review Showing the Importance of Investigating Cognitive Processes as Mediators between Emotion and Behavior. Behavioral Sciences, 9(12), 117. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120117